Decoding the Gurus - Book Review: The Selfish Gene
Episode Date: October 28, 2024In this special international episode of Decoding the Gurus, Chris and Matt jump on the hottest online topic and devote an hour to reviewing Richard Dawkins' influential work from the 1970s, The Selfi...sh Gene. This book influenced Matt and Chris when they were teenage decoders, but how does it hold up now that they have evolved into (quasi)adult forms?Based on their rereading of the book they discuss its contribution to the public understanding of evolution, the academic and public controversies it sparked, and Dawkins' broader contributions to science communication and... the culture war. Consideration is given to the criticisms raised by figures like Stephen Jay Gould and Mary Midgley, the implications of seeing humans as meat machines constructed by genes, and what should be understood as the book's core message.So join Chris and Matt as they confront their true nature as gene propagators but also argue that it is possible to simultaneously recognise the importance of human cultural & social development and our genetic & biological legacies. LinksDawkins, R. (2016) The Selfish Gene (40th Anniversary edition)
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hello and welcome to decoding the gurus with cognitive anthropologists, me, Chris Kavanagh,
the psychologist of unnamed subfield, Matthew Brown over there.
And as it happens, this is an international broadcast, a dispatch episode.
Because usually, as you know, this is a transnational podcast that we pride ourselves on.
We have an Irishman corresponding from Japan. And we previously had an Australian in Australia
in its native habitat where it should be kept, but it's managed
to break out of its boundaries and now it's foraging over in the United States.
Matt is joining us from San Francisco.
Hello, Matt.
Hello.
Hello.
Yeah.
Yeah.
Yeah.
I know all about this.
Introduce species, that can be a big problem, but I'm causing no troubles over here.
I'm landing my behavior.
Actually, we're not in San Francisco, Chris. species, that can be a big problem. But I'm causing no troubles over here. I'm wanting my behavior.
Actually, we're not in San Francisco, Chris.
We have headed North and we're in Fort Bragg, the Golden Gate.
And we've investigated a beach.
There we go.
Okay. You managed to find a beach.
Australians are good at that.
So yeah, and also just to warn people that you might not be hearing Matt at his full audio fidelity that you used to.
That's because he has brought his special podcasting microphone with him, but he forgot that he can't plug it in, provide a dongle-y thing to the Mac.
So we will we will increase our audio quality, but for today, it's a little bit lower down.
The upside of it is the chair is possibly less squeaky in this hotel.
So lower auto quality overall, but less squeaky chair.
So swings and roundabouts.
Let's see.
But Chris, Chris, do you want to hear my anthropological takes on Americans and Californians so far?
All right, yeah. Remember, Matt, you're only allowed in these kind of episodes. If it's not
behind the supplemental material thing, you're restricted. So you're in depth insights. Well,
you know, you should see if the best ones until you're able to roam freely because we
will look. Yes. They are great. They are great. My insights, the
Doric is they're extremely short, extremely short.
Oh, okay. Well, I know I was gonna say is before you say it,
that this is a selfish gene episode. We're gonna get into
that. But just in case people are confused and they're
worried, what is going on? Is it some of the material? Is it the coding? We're doing a review of The Selfish
Gene, Richard Dawkins book, which we read and all that kind of thing. So we'll get to that. But now,
please go ahead, Matt. I'm just, what's that thing? Flagging or no flagging is when you like put your
body- You're foreshadowing.
I'm foreshadowing. I'm doing something like that. I'm giving people orientations in the discourse space.
So yes, but please continue.
OK, I'm interested in your professional
opinion about this, see whether you agree as an anthropologist.
In-and-out burger, five stars.
That's an extremely excellent takeaway shop by Australian standards.
This is all relative.
The other one is squirrels are good. They're very cute.
I think they're under, under appreciated. They're extremely cute.
Yeah. But what you don't you have a squirrels in Australia?
No, we don't have squirrels in Australia. We have possums, not opossums,
not the freaky ones that have them in the United States. These are cute ones.
We're like squirrels. Do they pretend to die? the freaky ones that have them. The United States, these are cute ones, but like squirrels.
Do they pretend to die?
Which ones are the ones that pretend to die?
Is that all possums, possums, all possums?
No, I think no possums don't do that.
Is that squirrels or is it opossums?
No, it's possums.
Possums are the ones that famously like play dead.
Yeah, OK, not Australian ones, not Australian possums. Yeah. Okay. Not Australia. Not Australia ones. Not Australian possums. Okay. All right. So yeah, that's it. That's it. That's my
Okay, and then I burger is good squirts are cute. And that's it
for now.
Apart from that, I haven't noticed anything.
Are you noticing, well, one question I had, Matt, not to force you to provide more political commentary, but just, you know, it's election season.
It's not that long until the next presidential election in the US. Are you noticing any of that?
Like, you know, fly?
Any unusual activity?
Yeah.
Yeah.
Yeah.
I'm seeing a lot of lawn signs, seeing a lot of posters in windows, not most of them, but
occasionally more than you'd see in Australia.
We don't really do that.
Yeah.
I think so much.
Yeah.
No, it's good.
People, people, people wear their political opinions on their sleeve and you know, they're out
there. That's the nice thing about it.
I'm walking here. I'm a Democrat. I'm voting for governor.
Yeah, so that's good. I feel like I could fit into San
Francisco. I could live there and be accepted among them. But I
think I'd have to become a little bit less scruffy.
They're very neat and tidy in San Francisco.
Well, what I've heard from the discourse, it's a hell sphere of, you know, like
it's just roaming zombies basically in San Francisco.
It's all been taken over by homeless people and law and orders booked down.
They don't prosecute thieves.
Like can't walk down the street without being shanked.
orders broke down. They don't prosecute thieves. Like can't walk down the street, Matt, without being shanked. So you're really quite lucky to get out of there.
There were some kids, well, there were some kids doing wheelies on their motorbikes down the
main street. I felt like the police still have stopped them maybe. Street performance. It was
hard. It's hard. Well, that was good. I did enjoy that. And yeah, you did well,
Matt. You know, we're constrained as we are by our audience's demands that banter be restricted,
but not in supplemental materials. That's where we can roam free. That's where we can let a show. But here we're here to talk about our time rereading or listening to in my case, because I
listened to the audio version of The Selfish Gene. I think we both looked at the 40th anniversary
edition, which has extensive footnotes and a couple of additional chapters and epilogues, but they were also added.
Some of them are from earlier versions as well. But this is, I feel the definitive
selfish gene where you have like Dawkins asides and his, you know, his reflections on
things that he previously said and that kind of thing. So yeah, maybe some first off big thoughts. How did you enjoy it? Did you
hear that? Did it make you question your life? How was it? Well, there was a lot of stuff in it.
There wasn't in the original version that I read. Like it must be almost twice as long now with all
the footnotes and endnotes and commentary and sides. It's got a of key. A lot of the time he's sort of updating it
and correcting some antiquarian sort of references.
And a lot of the time he's making rejoinders
against unfair criticisms,
which definitely sound unfair as voice play Richard Dawkins
and then he sort of demolishes them
and explains why they're wrong.
So yeah, no, he's had fun.
Yeah.
Big thoughts apart from that.
It was, it did look, I know you think this too, but you'll agree with me that it
was, it was good, but it felt like if you're coming to evolutionary theory and
stuff for the first time, then it's really good because he explains all these things
in a basic way and takes it through every step.
But if you've heard of them before, then it could be a bit frustrating.
So yeah, it was, you know, had to fast forward through some bits.
Well, maybe it's worth mentioning.
I'll say as well that the audio book is read by him and by, I think it might be
his now separated wife, but Lala Ward is the older person, so there's sections
read by him, sections read by his wife.
Was that the version that you had, Matt, that had that form?
Yeah.
Yeah.
Same one.
Yeah.
So that I like, first of all, I just liked that because it kind of broke things up a
bit in terms of like just the same delivery.
So I appreciated that and they're both good at, at reading and obviously like the
footnotes were majority of the read by Dawkins because he,
like you say, he's responded to it. Occasionally he felt after, you know, Lala Ward summarized
one bit and then he would say, footnote, you know, in response to unfair criticism. And, yeah,
but I did also appreciate that because he also used the opportunity to like nuance some statements
that he made and whatnot. And we'll talk about some of that. But the overall big thing I wanted
to say was The Selfish Gene was a very influential book for me when I initially read it because
it helped to spur my interest in evolutionary theory and like kind of science in general, the scientific approach.
And although the vast majority of the book is not focused specifically on
humans, right, it's much more focused on like life on earth and evolutionary
processes and it does make reference to humans, especially in the later chapters.
But it's it's not actually primarily fixated on like
linking all those things to humans.
But when I read it, there are obvious things where you can apply it
to human life and just like understanding
about the way that humans evolved and evolutionary processes occur.
And it was very eye opening.
And this time reading it, I remembered like a lot of it
and had the feeling that it's very well written.
Things are like explained really nicely in various parts.
But I did have the same experience you had where there were parts
that were dragged because
I already know what is being discussed and it's describing things in quite a lot of depth.
So I had the feeling like, yeah, yeah, I get this.
There's an entire chapter which describes, it comes towards the end, this is one of the
additional chapters that goes through the Prisoner's Dilemma games
and the strategies used there.
And I know that literature quite well.
And certainly, I understand the scenario.
So going through it all was kind of like, oh, OK, yes, well,
that was.
But it's a very neat, potted description.
And the last big thought I have is that originally it was written in the 1970s
and there are various indications that that is the case.
And I don't mean the political elements, which people have focused on, I think too
much, I mean more in discussions about like technology or, you know, society, which just just now feel very dated.
In the footnotes, he often points out that this is the case, or even, for example, in
describing game theory as this upstart new approach, which people haven't really heard
about now.
It's like game theory, that incredibly well known, like influential literature.
So when Dawkins was writing, a bunch of it was still not that well known or so on. So that was
kind of interesting, but it does make bits of it feel a bit like time has moved on.
Yeah. Yeah. Well, you mentioned how he actually doesn't talk about humans very much.
All of his examples are drawn, or at least almost all of his examples are drawn from
the animal world.
And, you know, he does occasionally make asides, you know, that can be, you know, interpreted
as-
Often get him in trouble.
Yeah, that's right.
And there's a few ones about the welfare state and overpopulation, which we've got to be
careful.
But most of the time it's just talking sort of abstractly, like males and females and
siblings and daughters and relatedness and so on.
So it really reminded me why people got a bit upset about it and why I think many people
just instinctively don't like it, which is that your mind naturally goes to applying a lot of the
behavioral evolutionary things to humans.
And did you notice this Chris?
It's actually like, even for someone like me, it was like so totally
on board with everything.
At a gut level, you kind of find it a little bit insulting as a human to talk
about, oh, you know, you care about this
child 50% as much as yourself exactly 50%. And all of this
stuff, he's obviously talking about the genetic influence on
behavior and acknowledges that's reduced in humans. But what the
reader does is you kind of naturally take it as about us
that humans and it does feel like a little bit disrespectful.
Yeah, I but I remember that hitting more initially, like the first time I read it, because I'm now
more like I've, I've thought about that perspective a lot. So like it, you know, whenever I think
about, maybe that's partly is also because I teach some of this in relating to like, looking at human
societies and culture and religion
from the point of view of like social primates and like elements of human culture as like
cultural tools that can be used to solve group problems and this kind of thing.
So when talking about that, like I remember when I read about Robin Dunbar and some other theorists discussing gossip,
and discussing how gossip is often denigrated as this like waste of time, that's something
that people shouldn't be doing.
But actually, if you view it from like an evolutionary perspective, keeping track of
reputations of third party individuals in your environment would be very important because you
would want to know who's a good partner, who's not. And yeah, so, but like, see, when you start
to analyze your own behavior through that lens, especially if it's linked to reproduction and
mating and whatnot, right? It can be distasteful. And I also think you can get evolutionary
psychologists that, you know, like God's had or someone that I...
Yeah, they just naturally give you the ick anyway.
Yeah, and they're also applying it in a really like, there is plenty of legitimate criticisms
to be made of like, overly zealous applications of evolutionary psychology. But, but, but I think the rejoinder has to be that there also is very powerful
evidence that we are influenced by evolution and evolutionary drives.
And if you don't appreciate that about humans, you're not really understanding
human psychology or the history of human evolution in any great depth.
So yeah, like that's...
Well, you know that I totally agree with you, Chris.
But you know, but it was good to be reminded of like why I think many people do have instinctive
dislike of it. And I think maybe a lot of the critics sort of have that vibe and then they go looking
for something wrong with it. And we could talk about the various critics and the long running
disputes he's had with, what were their names? Gould and...
Yeah, Gould and yes. And also there was a philosopher called Mary Mitchley who wrote critically
about the selfish gene and there are also similarly his later books around
atheism and whatnot would attract a lot of detractors.
So yeah, he's had his fair share of critics and Stephen Gould takes issue
with various perspectives that he takes. So it's not like there's no disagreement
around the topic or no room for it to be. But I do think in some cases, I'm on Dawkins side in that
he's been misinterpreted and that people are assuming that the book, The Selfish Gene is about
the selfish person. And as he argues, like in the new epilogue
and whatnot, he's added that, you know, we would have been in hindsight, he could have called it
the cooperative gene, because it like the selfish component acting in its own interests.
And that that can be for or against individual human
address, usually aligned, usually aligned or not even human
for or against like an individual of any species
that's carrying the genes and things,
but they don't, they're often aligned,
but not always aligned.
And yeah, but he was pointing, they're often aligned, but not always aligned. And, uh, yeah. But, but he,
he was pointing out by using the term selfish, it, it led a lot of the beat to be around,
well, people being perceived as profit, maximizing selfish carriers of genes. And that wasn't his
argument. Like he, I got this right clearly, but, but I feel there were various points in it where he strongly bent over
backwards to highlight he is not saying that.
He is not saying that we should act in the manner
that genes might prescribe if they had their hands on the beam.
And actually, strongly emphasizing,
we don't have to do that.
Like humans, perhaps unique amongst animals have due to our culture and the
development of our cognitive capacities or whatnot, we know I can do all sorts of
things that go against genetic drives.
And we do all the time.
And actually society is much the better for that.
And I thought that was very clear, but I feel like a lot of his critics didn't feel that that was clear enough. And some
of his political sides, I also think give him fuel for the fire because there's one where he makes an
aside about long-term planning or selfish impulses and he highlights, you know, like the working class
as potentially do that.
And then, but the footnote that he added was like saying,
you know, look, I really regret this
cause I sound like a daily meal reader,
but this was written in, yeah, in the context of this,
like some event, political event in the seventies.
And he, he wanted to, he was, and he was, you know,
he said he was thinking about that, but like if he was doing it now, it would be just as easy and
perhaps more appropriate to look for selfish behavior in like the ruling elites and political
class. Right. So like he, he regrets how that came across. And I was like, that's good. That's
actually nice to have that context. Right? But if you read the 1970 version,
I think things like that would justly, perhaps,
you know, skew your interpretation
of what he's wanting to say
and assuming there's like a political agenda attached to it.
So, yeah.
Yeah.
Well, I looked into some of the academic controversies because I remember
when I read it originally, and I think I even read it twice, even the second time, I think
I liked the book and so I was a bit of a Dawkins fan. So I was kind of on board with him, you
know, raising these criticisms and then demolishing them. At the time I was just all these silly
criticisms. So yeah, Dawkins is definitely right.
But then I thought, well, I should look into it again with a bit more of an open mind because
I was just an ignorant fanboy first time around.
And where I got to was, and I think he at some point hints at this sort of thinking
that the disputes that you had with Gould
were really much like a bunch of fuss about nothing really.
Like it was really just more a matter of emphasis
and what kind of metaphors I suppose
you're using to describe things.
I mean, even at the time I thought, you know,
so one of the things they raised
was the punctuated equilibrium in terms of the rate
at which species evolve and diversify
as compared to the gradualist metaphor that classical evolution has.
But when you think about it, that doesn't really mean much in terms of a criticism.
They're both saying the same thing.
A smooth curve looks jagged when you zoom out
to a billion years, but it can look,
it still looks smooth on a smaller time scale.
And there's nothing, we know from all sort of optimization,
like non-linear optimization problems
that the error surface,
which is a bit like the fitness function evolution,
is very, like there are flat basins and valleys and local minima,
all that kind of thing. And basically you always see this kind of adaption or learning happening,
you know, punctuated kind of way, but it's still smooth. Though those sorts of disputes to me
seemed like a bit of fuss about nothing. And the other ones too around like spandrels, do you
remember the concept of spandrels, Chris?
Yeah, yeah.
You can explain now.
Yeah, I mean, I read a couple of different sort
of explanations of them.
But the idea, the general idea is just
that there is not every phenotypic feature
that you observe is going to be a direct adaption,
but rather that there are
sort of structural and practical limitations to things, including I suppose unexpected
interactions of stuff, all of adaptive traits that lead to stuff you see basically in the
bodies and the behavior of organisms that isn't a result of evolution.
Now in rereading the book, Dawkins did mention that a few similar points at a time, like
that cautioning around always assuming that everything you see has got to have an evolutionary
just-so story attached to it and also emphasizing the unexpected consequences of genes because
he emphasizes that genes don't just have a single effect, but rather they're going to
have a whole bunch of different effects and those effects are occurring in combination
with other genes.
So it's pretty complicated.
And so it kind of implies that there would be these spandrels, and in other words, features
that you see, which are not necessarily and adoption and I think
You know just getting older for instance is a good example of one of these things
You know the genetic basis to it basically, you know genes that have multiple effects
Genes that maybe do something good for you beneficial that come into play when you're in your 20s
Might have some negative effects that happen in when you're 60 or 70
But when the vast majority of individuals have basically zero chance of having children after that
age and are likely going to be dead by natural or unnatural causes before then anyway, it
doesn't really matter.
It doesn't matter.
Yes.
Yeah, anyway, so I think that concept is really good.
I guess I softened a bit, a bit like Dawkins, but I think they raise really good points
actually about that.
It's kind of like a caution against the falling into that just so story trap of assuming there
has to be an adaptive explanation for things.
Oftentimes, there are features, there are behaviors that you see, which
can be biologically determined and even are a product and therefore a product of
evolution and they don't always have a direct adaptive benefit.
No.
Yeah.
I did think that Dawkins acknowledged that maybe like in the earlier
additions, it was less of an emphasis, but I, I took him to be highlighting that
there were features and, and like genes that could be inherited that were
piggybacking in other things and that were, yeah, harmful to the organism
after it was passed its reproductive age.
And also, um, I, I believe the concept about
acceptation was covered or you know the possibility that something could be
genetic drift or come along and then later come to have like a functional purpose.
But yeah I didn't take the perspective offered in the Selfish gene to argue against that. The central argument that I saw of the whole book is that you can look at evolution
from the point of view of like different, different levels.
But the one which is most fundamental is the gene, because that is the unit of
inheritance that has been selected for. because that is the unit of inheritance
and that has been selected for, right?
And that if you don't look at the gene,
there are various puzzles which appear
and which don't work at the level of the individual,
including things like eusocial insects, right?
And so on, but not to say that you should,
like I didn't take it to be saying,
apply a Weinsteinian hyperactive framework where you take anything you see to be like a
perfect attunement between the environment and the genetic features of a, of a species,
like obviously not. So yeah, yeah, that, that just, that struck me. And
like one thing about looking at things in that regard from the point of view of the
gene, I feel like people are mind that much less when it's applied to other animals beyond us. But like Dawkins and other people's argument wants to say, well, these things
obviously still apply in humans, but there is this issue, right?
That we have overtaken our biological legacy in a lot of ways because of culture.
And then at the end, when he gets into talking about other evolutionary processes,
right, the spreading of ideas, memes, or talking about how there may be later discovered other
evolutionary systems, which are not the end of selection is not genes, right, because he wants
to talk about evolution as a general process whereby you have selection and transmission
of information across generations.
So he said this book is about the genes, the replicators on earth, the original ones, but
there's nothing in principle that means that you need DNA and genes to get an evolutionary process occurring.
You just need selection and differential reproduction and the inheritance.
And yeah, and I thought that was like quite well done, even though I do think
some of the stuff around memes is not really thought out, but he himself says
that he just intended that more as like an illustrative example that, you know, there,
these processes could apply elsewhere and the mechanisms might be different, but it could still
be useful to think about this basic process. Yeah, yeah, that's right. I mean, people like myself
have played with genetic algorithms as a thing and you can simulate it in software. And yeah,
you get the same evolved results because it follows almost mathematically, right? I think
you start quite well where he says there's like an iron rule, which is if something is going to be
around for a long time, then you have to be it has to be very good at just like keeping its shape,
like keeping its structure. So many chemicals just immediately, you know, disassemble or
vaporize or, you know, atoms jump away from each other. And some forms of chemicals are
stable, right? So the stable ones tend to be around in the universe today. Andy points
out that evolution is just like another way of achieving like a kind
of dynamic stability for a very specific kind of, of chemical, which is for us,
happens to be, you know, DNA, but it's just following this sort of rule of if
there's going to, if you, if you expect to see this thing around in a long, long
time, then it's got to be pretty good at either just surviving for a really, really long time or doing this other tricky
way of making copies of itself and being able to replicate.
Then all of the original ones might be gone, but you're still going to see a lot of it.
So yeah, no, I mean, I think it's a really important perspective, but I think it is challenging
for people, like you said, like, like intellectually, everyone agrees that we are biological automata on some level.
And evolution affects not only what we look like and how our bodies work, but also how
we behave.
Like if you want to choose to stop breathing, you just can't because evolution won't let
you.
Right?
We don't have full control over ourselves.
Like I found out in an out burger earlier today.
Yeah.
Evolutionary drives that driving you to model adaptive behavior.
Burger.
Yeah.
Yeah.
So, you know, obviously sexual behavior is another one and also, you know,
familial loves, right?
And this is the kind of thing that
makes us a bit upset to have our personal relationships and our feelings reduced to a
kind of mathematical equation of relatedness. Even as you say Chris, like, they could be wrong.
People, Dawkins and evolutionary fans like you and I, nobody's saying like this is a sum total
of what we are and what we can be.
In fact, it's a pretty bad way to be,
as Dawkins emphasizes, culture is great.
And we actually wanna hijack these sort of totally amoral
and rather horrible processes of evolution in many ways
for our own devices.
But I guess I'm just saying I understand why probably many
people just have a gut kind of like, I don't like this that much because it is kind of offensive
because you can't help but think of reduce yourself to those animalistic terms.
Well, the interesting thing for me is there's a little bit of a parallel because like,
in some parts he talks about imagining and he's clear that he's talking in terms
of a metaphor, even though it is describing actual biological processes. But the notion that genes
build these meat machines to carry them around and allow them to make copies. And they build
the instructions, but they don't have exact control, right?
He talks about the analogy of like somebody building a chess playing software program
where they give the instructions, but they're not like commanding each thing, right? And
in the same way, she's build up these biological entities, which they are using to help them create copies in the next generation.
But the biological entities are what we would recognize as organisms. And that perspective,
like shifting everyone to people being like essential puppets of genes at one step removed,
I think could be offensive, but I will say,
I've seen people take much less offense
whenever it's presented in philosophical terms
from like, say a Buddhist analysis,
where people look at humans as like aggregate forces
of desire and like psychologically constituted elements
of thought, right?
That are craving all these different things.
So that is just another way.
And like, you know, I know Buddhists in history, for example, wrote some of
them recommended like the people hang out in graveyards and embrace corpses and
start to see their body as like meat sacks that are carrying around big bags of
like gases and liquids to dissolve things.
And, you know, see your eyes like pools of liquid,
squidgy stuff. And it is true. Like we talked about when we read immune, that the description
about the human as like a giant tube, like a tube with a hole that goes in where various environmental
stuff are taken and a hole on the other end, where the stuff
that can't be considered as productive.
And then there's other things, right?
But that also is a reductionist version, right?
But I think that there is, for some people, where in that reduction, it's insulting to
the kind of uniqueness and the complexity of the human spirit and intellectual achievement and whatnot.
But for a lot of people, it's more like, no, we recognize that that is all there, but also
acknowledge the fundamental biological reality. But I feel for some people that is viewed as like
And I, but I feel for some people that is viewed as like one or the other must be in effect, right?
And, and more real.
And for me, it, it's always been like an interesting and yes, like a kind of challenging
perspective, but it's never made me then go, well, it doesn't really matter that like
people care for their children or whatnot, because there's biological propensities all through the animal
kingdom that care for offspring.
So what's admirable about that?
You know, you're just like, no, it doesn't, it doesn't have to be that
recognizing it means that stories of heroic sacrifice for families or what
not now become like less significant or meaningful. But I think
for some people it does. And especially I would say for those inclined towards spirituality and
religion, like they see in this an alternative story, which, you know, like we saw with Jordan
Peterson, they feel is like cold and diminishes human specialness.
Yeah, yeah. Yeah, I agree with all of that. And I think, yeah, I mean, those two cultures, really,
I think you can have the sort of point of view where you lean into all of this science stuff,
and you fully recognize as it forms like a scaffolding and a
basis for what humans are. And then you go, okay, so humans, we construct meaning, right? So this
sort of humanistic point of view. And that sort of keeps them separate. You know, like humanism is
kind of soft and fluffy and values the human spirit. You know, a lot of the things that
spirituality and religion does, but it doesn't need to
sort of own all of the territory.
It can quite happily let, I think, the biological substrate stuff sit there and say, hey, look,
we're creating all of these ideas and culture and things that we find meaningful on top
of that.
But yeah, so I'm kind of on that side rather than the kind of people that see these things
as overlapping and conflicting, because that makes them want to resolve them in a way that actually
is detrimental to your scientific understanding. Yeah. And I didn't notice that somebody in the
Patreon actually pointed out, in the first couple of pages of the book, Dawkins, rather polemically
states that any consideration of the human condition that
developed before Darwinism is useless. Right. And I, I feel like that was polemical, but he was
talking in terms of under, like I would have took it that he's talking in terms of understanding,
like why does life on earth exist? Like the biological origins of life, not in terms of understanding, like, why does life on Earth exist? Like, the biological origins of life,
not in terms of, like, all of literature is useless because they didn't know about these,
like, underlying biological and genetic aspects. Like, I don't think that was what he was saying.
I think he was saying all previous origin stories about human life and where we came from were based on like
misunderstandings or not knowing because we didn't know the actual like scientific information
about genes and about evolutionary processes. And after we get that, you can have versions
where they're taking that into account. And yeah, so it was, but it was a, it is the kind
of thing that if you tell philosophers, everything you did prior to biologists developing evolution
was pointless, that they will react to that. Yeah, very true. Very true. Okay. What else did
you like about it, Chris? Or something that you especially didn't like?
Well, oh, yeah, I can say something I did like because I forgot that this was in there.
There's a, not super extended bit, but there's a part about consciousness, Ma.
And he essentially argues the same position that I've argued on here.
And he talks about some, I think, philosopher or whatnot that has referenced it or cognitive
scientist.
And he talks about Daniel Dennett as well.
But I like the fact that he gave like a potted argument for consciousness as potentially
related to, you know, modeling the environment and that he was, I think, at the time talking
about how computers, if they become sufficiently
developed, it may be that they end up developing something like consciousness because in modeling
the environment and the social interactions that having a eye in that environment can eventually
lead to a sense of like self and identity. And I just was like, Oh, I forgot that was in there.
Like maybe that is where I first came across the idea. It might be or it might not be because I think I came across it elsewhere.
But I was just, I found, you know, I agree with that point of view. But I think there's a good
summary of it in this book. And yeah, that was something I forgot was in there.
Yeah, that was something I'd forgot was in there.
Mm-hmm.
Yeah. Yeah, that's good.
I, I like being reminded about, you know, a lot of those, I guess, stuff around
altruism and selfishness and it's a good reminder that you can explain an awful
lot of, of cooperative behavior through selfishness essentially, um, at the
genetic level.
So, so obviously as the name implies,
it's called the selfish gene, not the selfish organism. And you know, that's obviously the
main theme that it's actually the genes, like you said, that carry the information that is
actually replicated. So everything else is kind of temporary and epi-synominal. It's the genes that
really matter and their interests, not the individual's interests. So that leads onto a whole bunch of logic around kin selection and how cooperative
behavior and just having those behavioral heuristics, like, I don't know, be nice to
other organisms of your species that you remember from when you were young or whatever.
So that's the kind of heuristical behavior that a gene can code for. And that's what he was referring to.
Yeah. And that is, I think an interesting thing to point out is like in the terminology
he's using, he is using the description, the script of selfish, right. As we, which as
we know, there are a lot of controversy, but in this description, it's quite clear a selfish gene could create a highly cooperative
and altruistic individual.
Right?
So like, so like selfish genes created ants, right?
And all the colony insects.
And they're super altruistic.
The ultimate.
At least all the members of their colony.
Exactly.
Not so much the other ones.
But you know, it's
all obviously perfectly explained by the fact that
they're acting in the interest of the genes, not in terms of the
interest of the little ant. So that's a simple example. But you
know, it gets gets kind of nuanced and complicated when
you've got levels of certainty as to whether or not these really
are your children or not. And in terms of, you know, grandmothers
and grandfathers, and, and, you know, it of, you know, grandmothers and grandfathers and, and you know, it does,
you know, one thing I remember somebody was criticizing evolution once is saying like,
it creates like it was not testable because it was all with just so story. That was the
basic idea, right? But it wasn't science, essentially. And this is a creationist or
the someone? No, it was sort of someone who sort of was politically incorrect
somehow. I forget. Oh, okay. Coming from the other angle. But, you know, I was reminded with this,
which is like how many predictions the theory makes, right? Like the theory is extremely simple.
It's very elegant, as scientists and mathematicians like to say, but it makes a lot of quite specific testable predictions
about what should happen.
You know, the vast majority of them are confirmed,
but obviously some aren't because again,
like Dawkins emphasizes, it's complicated, right?
And you know, you don't necessarily know
about all the individual, like the specific risk
and reward ratios and so on for different
species.
Yeah. And there was some, like the book is absolutely full of really fascinating examples
from animals, right, about how these kind of genetic proclivities result in certain
kinds of behaviors or certain kinds of social organizations and whatnot. And I appreciate that there was one, I can't remember if it was talking about bees or wasps.
It was some insect, right? And talking about, so, oh, yes, actually, it was in this section,
which I think has led to a controversy where he is talking about can a single gene influence code for something, right?
Like a single gene.
And I actually think he is very clear throughout the majority of the book that
he's, he's sometimes talking in shorthand, but he doesn't think there are single
genes, which code for something complex usually, but he does give a caveat at
one point where he was talking about how
it could be the case where you had a single gene that when activated led to a cascade
for other genes, which resulted in a kind of behavior emerging. And he was talking about
hygienic wasps or hygienic bees, that they remove larvae that haven't developed right or whatever from the little
honeycomb things that they're in. But they have to have two behaviors, like they're removing the top
and removing the inside. And some clever experiment did it where they showed that you could
knock these out in different generations depending on the cross-breeding different
populations. But it was very clear in that, two things. One, that it's very specific circumstances
where that happens. So he basically just wanted to say, it is possible that there could be complex
behaviors that are affected by a small like gene changes in gene frequencies.
But most, most things are the product of like very complex interactions of multiple genes.
So it's better not to talk like the gene for or this kind of thing because it can give the wrong opinion.
But even better than that, after he gave this really neat example around these hygienic wasps
and described an experiment, the footnote mentioned that actually he had perhaps overemphasized how neat this experiment
was because there was some complexity in which one of the behaviors was found in one of the
hives where it wasn't anticipated to be.
Even though it was just like one uh, like one field prediction,
it's important because why it was there isn't clear.
Right.
And, and he regarded this like they should have made this more explicit
than what that I'm, and maybe he got criticism for that part.
But I was like, well, that's neat because you're highlighting,
there was a prediction made here's a way that they tried to test that.
And also there was more complexity than my, you know, pop science description.
Alloyed.
Um, but yeah, so that, like that to me is all very interesting, complicated problems.
And I can see why it would lead some people to interpret it that is, is
overemphasizing the role of like individual genes, right?
But I, but I do think if you take the book in like individual genes, right?
But I do think if you take the book in its totality,
it's very clear that it isn't making that argument
like that there is single genes in general
for like very complex behaviors.
Yeah, no, I didn't get that impression at all really.
Yeah, like speaking of like Chris, he is pretty careful.
I mean, just stepping back a bit,
let's put him back through the guru lens just a little bit.
And yeah, he just briefly, he does do pretty well,
just objectively speaking,
like compared to some of these popular books
that we see from Yuval Noah Harari
or some of these other ones, Gad Sadr,
somewhere in between, there are other names there too, but you know,
it's the sort of thing we've complained about where they'll,
they'll find a little study that it's a little example they can use to
decorate their thing.
And they don't care how strong it is, whether it means anything,
whether it's been supported by the other literature. Now, what you,
you just described with Dawkins is the exact opposite, right?
He's citing something that, you know,
it's a pretty important,
substantive example to these precise thing that he's talking about.
And he doesn't rely on weak information.
And when he, a couple of times I remember he did mention some weak
information because it was interesting.
And my goodness, he took like half a page emphasizing how tentative this was and
how they didn't draw strong conclusions and neither should they, because they
just get sent save this time.
And so yeah, it's pretty good.
Well, that was one thing that constantly struck me as well.
Like when you compare what Dawkins is doing in this book to Jordan Peterson or Brett
Weinstein or any of the alert gurus, he's constantly clear.
This is just a metaphor, right?
This is, or when he's talking in metaphors, he's explaining, no, this is just a metaphor.
When he's talking in metaphors, he's explaining, now this is a useful metaphor, but you have
to remember that I've been, so I mean, throughout it, he keeps saying, I'm using the language
of intentionality, I'm talking about strategies that genes are employing with wants and desires,
but of course they don't have them.
They don't have them. Right. They don't have them. These are, this is just like a useful way to talk about it, but you have to always
remember that it is, it is not implying that they have human emotions and
desires and wants, right?
And he is very, to me, it, it, I heard that reiterated multiple times.
I even heard like Pages interrupted
where he wants to make that point.
But a lot of his critics, I feel,
still take him to task for that,
saying that he fell prey of what he's warning against.
But I didn't read that.
Like on the occasions where that happened
or was at risk of happening,
there was usually shortly after things saying, no, that story I just told, it is a way to
illustrate a thing, but we have to remember that we're actually talking about
things that don't have any will and don't have any, you know, self-consciousness
and whatnot.
So yeah, that, that was just something that, that struck me that I thought he
was very clear about,
but a lot of people critical of his work don't think he was clear about.
In terms of grooving, one thing is, I did notice this, that his little footnotes, apart
from the grievance margarine, which I think sometimes was justified because he's sometimes
like, hi, this idiot, interpreted me as saying this when I have this exact sentence
here, right?
Like he, he certainly does take the opportunity to respond to
critics.
But the other bit was there was a lot of him saying he met this
person and he had this idea and he told them about it and they
went on the explored or, you know, I introduced such and such to this person that they produced this fruitful collaboration
out of it and most of it sounded legitimate, but it also struck me as like
strangely petty or whatever.
There was like a thing where he mentioned EO Wilson and him made a the same error
in attributing some name to a theory or theories or whatever.
And this led people to say that he had taken the idea from E.O. Wilson. And he was like,
actually, I found a notebook that showed that I had made this mistake in advance. And it
was, you know, a case of like independent errors. And he did link it to the point he wanted to be a
fight that you can't always infer inheritance from, you know, there can be cases where there was
independent parallel evolutionary developments. But I was like, was this a big thing? Was there like a big concern that you stole a word
from E.O. Wilson and like, yeah, so there was-
Well, I hear you.
But I mean, on the other side of the equation there,
I remember like, having read that, I was thinking,
like you could come away from it thinking
that really Richard Dawkins hasn't contributed anything
to what he was explaining in the book. And rather, because he cites all of these people and he really
emphasizes how this person really made it clear. And what I've done here is I'm rehashing
what so-and-so has said. So he's pretty good at citing his sources, I thought, and doesn't really sort of say, he's my, he's my, you know, he's obviously reverential towards evolutionary theories, but I never really heard him sort of saying, he's my grand idea. that the whole section about memes, right?
And I think that is one of the weaker parts of the book,
the chapter on meme and meme complexes.
On the other hand, for the time and the influence
that the concept of memes and whatnot went on to have,
I thought it was quite impressive. Like,
yeah, well, he came up with the idea and it's, it's, you know, memes are popular,
right? I mean, it's clearly an important contribution,
but I agree. So the, the section around memes, I liked the whole bit about
thinking about it in terms of, uh,
like it is interesting to think about another alternative evolutionary
process where things could be happening. I'm thinking about brains as, or human bodies,
even if you want to, you know, extend it beyond brains, but like repositories of ideas and that
there actually are limited resources and that it might be a useful way to look at things as
kind of like competing in an ideas phase. And it reminded me of the epidemiology of representations
by Dan Sperber, which, you know, or any number of all our theories that have taken similar approaches.
And I do think it's an interesting like approach to take and that you can, looking at
like cultural evolution and whatnot, it is an interesting perspective to take
and one that I'm sympathetic to, but I just think there were elements of it
that were like not really fleshed out, but he said that.
He was like, this isn't the developed theory of memes.
It's just more a bit of a thought experiment around that, but it went on to be,
I think, quite influential. Although memeetics as an area, I think, spread into other topics that
have kind of escaped Dawkins' version of it. But yeah, that's influential, I think, in a way.
And that's nothing to do with self-esteem.
No, I think like it preempted the Internet.
And we talk about tweets going viral and so on.
And people have this, there's a limited attention span for people.
And clearly all of the retweets and the reposts and so on.
Yeah, I don't know if you got there,
though, remember, but the section where he talks
about viruses and hackers and he kind of reprimands them saying like, think
about what you're doing with your life.
Right.
Because like, it's kind of, it's very upset about hackers.
And this was like you say in the earlier era of like internet and connectivity.
So he was even speculating about maybe there will be jobs in the
future where there are like computer doctors who come around to inoculate you from and he did a
thing like talking directly to hackers saying you know, unlike viruses, which can do no other, you
humans are, you know, writing nefarious code. And for what reason, other than like your own malevolent enjoyment,
that I think of what,
it was kind of,
that's the little pot of honey side to his personality.
I had a little pot of honey.
Um, yeah, no, it's, I thought it kind of, kind of cute after a bit.
Yeah.
And I will, I'd say this as well, that like Dawkins in the culture war era, I feel like
this side of him is lost and this is the best side of him, right?
This is why he is a figure in the discourse.
It isn't because of that.
I mean, maybe there's partly because the honeypot tree like tweets or, or the things
of like dogs and the signs that he saw right there in America.
Like I fully acknowledge the Dawkins when it comes to culture war stuff.
Is it like not really that good on it?
I mean, like he he was promoting James Lindsay because he read cynical theories after Lindsay had went
mental.
He just doesn't seem to bother much about looking into things.
But the fact is that there still is so much more substantial to him than there is to Jordan
Peterson or Bret Weinstein.
His command of evolutionary stuff
is just so much better. And he was a very good science popularizer. He belongs more to me
in the realm of the kind of Carl Sagan group than the latter day culture warriors.
And, you know, the latter day culture warriors.
So, yeah, just, but just to say, like, I don't blame people for, you know, focusing because that's what his output has often been recently is culture wars stuff or
the, the new atheist era, but I, I feel that this is why a lot of people respect
Dawkins more than a Brett Weinstein.
And it's, it's justified because he because he is a better thinker and he did
and does have expertise and a skill for communicating science about evolution.
So I feel like people would be well served to compartmentalize culture war Dawkins from
the popular science writer Dawkins.
And it's not like the two are completely separate characters, right?
They're intertwined. But I just I don't see loads of culture war stuff slipping
into his popular science writing.
No, and I don't see it being a motivating cause in any way, shape or form.
So, yeah, I mean, look, you don, nobody needs to idolize or put anyone up on a pedestal,
right?
And he's definitely, I think as a, as a very, as a man from a different time, at his age,
he's like a babe in the woods when it comes to the internet, the culture wars, and there's
just, just modern life.
Like for instance, there's no way he should have been sharing a stage with Brett Weinstein, right? It's he was, he just shouldn't have been right, but he
doesn't, doesn't check or doesn't know or it doesn't, I don't know. But it's
almost worth reading the Selfish Dream just so you can then go and listen to
him listening to Brett Weinstein talking about linear selection and then having
some kind of teleology and so on.
And only then can you truly understand the aghast look
on Richard Dawkins' face,
because it is so the opposite of everything
that is being explained there.
So look, I mean, read his books on evolution.
I read a couple of his books about religion,
but I didn't really care about that.
And I didn't really enjoy them.
I think they were fine.
No, I think he's bad as a, as a modern day character on the internet.
You shouldn't tweet anymore.
I mean, I feel like in most of them, they might have something like a one thing that I noticed in
this, which made sense to me is like, he focuses a lot on this, on sexual reproduction and its role in like the mixing up of genes,
right? And philosophizing about, you know, why does that exist if the goal is to make copies?
There's a more high fidelity version of copying, right? So, but his argument is you can't take sex
for granted just because it's there in lots of animals. Like, why is it there?
Why is it the dominant feature that we see in animals?
And then that did make me think that, like, you know, he's weirded into
the culture war and debates around trans stuff.
And he I don't think he does a very nuanced job of it.
But I think he like as far as that,
you know, influences his perspective on it, he is trying
to like focus on the biological aspect. And I just think that, you know, but he doesn't
do any of the other requisite stuff, like look into the cultural thing. So it's kind
of like he approaches these topics as if, well, I'm just talking as a biologist, but he's
talking about humans and culture and politics and all of these things.
And it doesn't mean that the underlying biology that we should deny it, that it
isn't important to think about sexual reproduction and the role it plays in
human evolution or animal evolution or the function of sex and genetics and all
these kinds of things.
It's fine to do that, but you have to also, as a human, realize that there is a social
environment and there are people pushing forward certain perspectives and whatnot. And like,
just his willingness, I think is next. One of his next things is Peter Vergaussian introducing him,
right? And like the amount of research that he'll have done into Peter
Vergaussian's arguments and views or whatever, we next to none in general.
So.
Yeah.
Yeah.
You should have, you should have retired.
I reckon.
Well, how long?
20 years ago?
15.
Well, I mean, I think he has retired, but he, he, he's still
retiring.
Shut up.
I dare you, Matt. I dare you.
Sorry, sorry.
Yeah. But I mean, so taking this book in isolation, I still, I would fully recommend
this book, especially for somebody who isn't familiar with evolutionary
biology or evolutionary theory.
It's a really good introduction.
It's well written.
It covers a lot of topics, a lot of big ideas.
And I think it is still kind of mind blowing if you haven't approached that perspective
before.
And with the updated version, there's more nuance than in some of the earlier
ones. So yeah, I really liked it, even though I recognize various limitations and I have my
issues with the kind of later chapters in their extension. But I would hardly set for my students,
for example, the chapter on the prisoner dilemma,
because it does a really neat job of covering like a fairly complex topic.
So yeah, that gets a, I was glad to revisit it, Matt, and I still enjoyed it, but it,
but it was less mind blowing now than it was, you know, 20 or 30 years ago when I read it.
Yeah, that's right.
You can't have your, you can't have your mind blown twice.
You can't watch the sixth sense twice.
These things could only happen once.
Yeah, no, I'd recommend it to how long.
Do you remember the extended phenotype?
Because I remember it being good.
Would you say it's just like a good read after this one?
I don't think I've read the extended phenotype because I got the argument.
Like I read some articles.
No Chris, there's more.
There's more nuances.
I did, I think I did start reading that and then got bored.
But I remember the ancestors, Teal bored me when I read it, like because of the
format of it, even though it was, it was interesting, but the extended phenotype, there's a chapter that's been added to this, which basically
provides like a potted summary of it.
And I, yeah, maybe I should actually read that, but it is talking about the fact that
there are products of genes that do not necessarily manifest in the body that can come from the
behaviors, right?
Like spiders webs or beaver dams, or insects,
forging shells to carry about in their back or whatnot. And we have to consider these as the gene
interacting with the environment and having implications beyond the organism. Yeah, that's right. And I think that's the best thing about books like this,
which, and it doesn't have to be about biology or evolution, it could be about physics or something like that. Or the immune system like you and I found where, you know, we
mentioned you have this weird feeling where you suddenly realize that your body is more like a
rainforest than anything else. And it's just a queasy feeling and it's true. And it just,
it encourages you to look at something familiar with with fresh eyes and the nice thing about the selfish gene is it encourages you to think about all of the
stuff as as from a gene's point of view these little atoms of information for
whom you know we are the vehicles that they build around themselves and like
you said with extended phenotype we build other things and change the
environment but to to the, the actual bits of information
that are actually being copied with some propensity
potentially infinitely into the future,
these are all environmental things
that they can interact with, but only passively, right?
Because they're as dumb as a rock.
They're ultimately just bits of molecules
floating through space and time. So it's just cool
to have that kind of step back, big picture view. Yeah. And the fact, you know, like in
reductionism in general, I mean, I feel there's a, this is why some of it falls a bit flat to me
when someone like a Sam Harris or whatever is like, think about your thought and I break it down and
like, you know, where did that thought come from? Where did it start? Isn't it like ultimately not really there?
And like, if you continue down that you can be like, well, what was that? Wasn't that just the
firing of a neuron in a biological organism, some process happening? And if you break that down,
there's molecules and atoms and like, where is the special unit right at the end of that. But like
so in the same way you can look at all the complex life in the world, all the bird songs,
all the beautiful environments and human social relationships and everything and say, well,
it's all from these like little molecules that are like replicating units, right? And it's true.
It is true just in the same way people are made from atoms
and all those kinds of things. But I think there is an issue that the higher levels matter, right?
And especially matter once you get to the point with like humans, whereby people are doing things
where they're actually working against the, you know, what mechanisms, the, the kind
of genes would have if they, if we had societies organized around them. And just, I mean, it's
reiterated in point, we've said it multiple times, but like Dawkins final message of the
first edition and the message for art is we uniquely amongst organisms now no longer are slaves to our genes. We can do things that they wouldn't
choose us to do. We do it all the time with birth control and with other things. And we
don't tie our value in life, unless you're Elon Musk, to the amount of genetic material
that you put into the next generation. And, yeah, that struck me as a
humanistic message. Yeah, yeah. No, no, another humanistic message which I got from him, and I
can't remember whether it was from this book or from a different one of his that I read, which was
to think of ourselves, our conscious selves, human culture, whatever you want to call it,
everything that isn't kind of purely biologically driven.
Like all of that is like a virus that's invaded the vehicles
that the genes built for themselves.
And it really encourages you to summon up this image, right?
So of these genes building better and better vehicles,
better and better survival machines,
purely for themselves, for their own benefit
and scare quotes, and then realizing that if I build a vehicle,
because like we said, genes can only
influence behavior through very broad heuristics, which
are not very flexible, can't change quickly,
even in archaeological time.
So by building in bigger brains and getting more flexibility in terms of those repertoires
and allowing learning and stuff to happen in species like humans, they were building
themselves better and better survival machines that would take better and better care of
them.
And, but they flew too close to the sun, Chris, right?
They built such a sophisticated machine in the case of humans that we realized what was
going on.
And now we get to do with our bodies what we like.
So yeah, I like those little sort of metaphorical allegories you can build off what really happened
with evolution and human consciousness. Yeah, and I you know, another thing that I think is
interesting, and is the kind of thing that you, you would want
to look into the literature of right, but I'm aware of some of
the literature by human cognition, and it's the early
developing, attentional preference towards predators, a
predator shaped thing is more attention grabbing
to us than other alternatives, right? And there are also potentially particular species like snakes
and spiders that we pick out more readily than other less significant evolutionary threats
throughout history. And that to me is like interesting to think about
because of course, like if you think about humans
as a social primate, that we would have mechanisms
just like all other primates that are honed
towards detecting certain sim silhouettes
or collection of shapes as potential dangers.
And that we are oriented more towards this makes perfect sense.
But it is the kind of thing that I think requires a little bit of recognizing that, you know,
that we have the legacy of evolutionary processes inside us.
And this can also have like implications whereby humans might be more concerned about stuff
which grabs our attention over things which are much more likely to kill us.
High cholesterol diets or cars, right? And yeah, just I think it is.
I think I hear what you're saying, which is that like by actually leaning into those lessons from
evolution, like threats and self-help, prejudice condition towards threat detection, which obviously
makes us react to certain kinds of media and engage with certain kinds of media more than others, right?
These are like useful lessons for blind spots or Achilles heels in our tendencies.
Obvious ones are around eating too much or various types of social behavior that we don't find very
admirable. For instance, setting up a pecking order and trying to accrue social capital
so you can put others down below you.
Now, I feel certain that humans, like pretty much every other species, that's, that literally
some degree in social groups has a predisposition towards it.
But by recognizing that we do have a species typical predisposition actually
allows us to have some self-awareness and do something about it.
And you know, so I think it's the wrong thing to do to sort of adopt that denialist kind
of point of view.
But we see on the internet, obviously, with these silly bros with the alpha male thing
and these weird evolutionary kind of high quality mates
and things like that. Like you see the exact opposite of that, don't you? So you see the
two ways to go wrong. One is to take these kind of evolutionary lessons as like a guidebook
or some kind of hack to sort of win at life. Or you could be in complete denial about them
and pretend that we are these sort of pure beams of spirituality and individuality, purely culturally constructed, and these meatbags wherein has no effect whatsoever. I think the mature path is the middle road,ual is a very complex topic, but there's all sorts of different rituals, right?
But one recurrent pattern of ritual that you see is cleansing, like people
washing or, or symbolically washing, right?
Like when people go to a shrine in Japan, they pour water over their hands, right?
Usually when people go into Christian churches, sometimes they touch the
holy water, right, and do the sign of the cross.
Or there's so many examples of like rituals which are either symbolically cleansing, you
know, somebody passes a like kind of burning incense over you, or legitimately cleansing,
like you're being washed, you know, by water or oil or whatever the case might be.
But you can view those and you can, for legitimate reasons,
look at the cultural origins of all these different behaviors.
You can trace back in antiquity or across cultures
where certain rituals are transmitted and so on.
But I also think that research looking
at how that might be connected to hazard avoidance psychology and how engaging in cleansing
actions, even when they're actually removed from the practice, could bring psychological relief.
Right. And that could in some ways be a spandrel over the behavior that like feeling dirty and
unpleasant is, and you know and wanting to become clean
could have things to avoid pathogens, right?
Like that feeling.
So, but I think that's interesting, right?
And it doesn't mean that there's no value
to looking at cleansing rituals in modern Rome
and how they spread related to like the Greek religions
and whatnot.
Like that's still a valid way to look at it,
but you can also look at it from the point of view of our cognitive inheritance and concerns about
pathogen avoidance. And I think as long as you're doing research that tests that, do rituals that
involve cleansing lead to anxiety reduction in individuals? And can you do
a nice study to test that? And people have tried to and generally have found evidence in support of
that. But it's the kind of thing where I'm very open to that being a way to approach things that
is valid as much as at the higher level, looking at the cultural diffusion or the history of it,
not from a cognitive evolutionary perspective. So I don't think you only need to do one.
Yeah, no, no, of course you're completely right. There's no like inherent contradiction between
looking at things at those two levels. You know, only stupid biologists would say that culture has no impact on humans.
And only a stupid anthropologist would say that there is no biological
underpinning to humans, right?
They don't contradict, you know, you can just like, you've got a lot of
diversity in a gene pool, you've got a lot of diversity in individuals, you
know, species and a lot of diversity in species as well.
It shouldn't be surprising that when you've got this extra cultural and cognitive flexibility
layered on top of whatever evolution is setting up as the scaffolding, you're going to see
a heap of diversity across individuals and groups.
And the ratio to which something can be species typical for humans versus being highly variable is itself really interesting.
And it informs both the evo psych type stuff and also the anthropological stuff.
Yeah.
Yeah.
Well, see, it's led to us developing our own speculative like little
accounts and descriptions here.
So I just, yeah, it's a, it's an interesting book.
We would be interested to hear all those opinions on it, but those are ours.
And it is also long. I'll just, I'll just point out that like, you know, an audio book
format, I think it's like 16 or 17 hours. So that is a sizable chunky little book, but yeah, good job Dawkins.
You did all right.
And I on for this book alone, I can say I still see value to it.
I wish this was what you spent more time doing the company on Twitter, but I guess
the genetic book of the dead, I haven't read that, but this has kind of been me
thinking, Oh, maybe it'd be interesting to read that and see, you know, what he's thinking is in 2024
Yeah, I might check that out too. Yep. Yeah good. Thanks, Chris
I enjoyed talking about that book enjoyed reading it again, too
So, uh, yeah good job onwards and upwards next time. We'll get you a good ethnography next month
I don't even know what that is, but yeah, okay, okay
All right. Well enjoy your American sojourn as it continues
Thank you I'm going to be back. Music