Hidden Brain - Bringing Up Baby
Episode Date: December 21, 2021Bababa, dadada, ahgaga. Got that? Babies are speaking to us all the time, but most of us have no clue what they're saying. To us non-babies, it all sounds like charming, mysterious gobbledegook. To re...searchers, though, babbling conveys important information about a baby's readiness to learn. This week, we'll revisit a favorite episode exploring the language and behavior of the newest members of the human family.If you like this show, please check out our new podcast, My Unsung Hero! And if you'd like to support our work, you can do so at support.hiddenbrain.org.Â
Transcript
Discussion (0)
This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta.
There's a video on YouTube you might have seen.
It has nearly 200 million views.
In this video, a pair of twin brothers are standing in a kitchen, having a little chat.
Papa Papa Papa!
Papa Papa Papa!
These twin brothers, a diaper clad. Babies.
Now, if this video featured 10-year-old twins or adult twins, I guarantee you it wouldn't
have gone viral.
What makes this video special is that we have no clue what these babies are saying.
To us non babies, it sounds like Goblety Goook. Cute, mysterious, gobbledygook.
Here at Hidden Brain, we love trying to understand the puzzles and contradictions of human
behavior. But we spend most of our time talking about the older members of the human race. Today we focus on the younger set.
The much younger set.
I find babies are so impressive.
We can't really ask them what they're thinking.
We have to come up with clever ways of figuring out what's going on in their little brains.
Later in the show, we'll hear from a researcher who says parents are often misguided when they
try to steer their children's lives.
If you just do the right things, get the right skills, read the right box, you're going
to be able to shape your child into a particular kind of adult.
But first, we'll start with a language of babies, even babies who don't speak words.
Hi, I'm Laura Sreli.
When Laura was in college, she spent her summer vacations back home in
Sabri, Ontario, and she worked at the Magical Nook, a daycare center.
It was a large center that had different age groups,
so there was a baby room and a toddler room,
preschooler, school-aged children,
but my favorite room was the toddler room for sure.
Laura's job was to help the teachers of the center
engage with the kids and enforce the rules.
The music was an essential tool.
We would sing wheels on the bus. Music was an essential tool.
We would sing.
Wheels on the bus.
Old McDonald's.
If we didn't sing the clean up song, the preschoolers, then I don't think anything would have
ever gotten cleaned up.
But as soon as we started, it seemed to be the trick to convince them that yes they were
willing to put their toys away.
Lorna regularly found herself in awe of the toddlers.
One little kid who I had a good connection with, she grabbed my hand and brought me over
to the little slide.
And she really wanted to go down the slide you could tell.
So I helped her up the little stairs and I caught her at the bottom of the slide
and all the rest of the toddlers in this playground
just looked over and saw her and looked at each other
and then ran over to the slide
and formed a cute little line up
waiting her turn for me to help them down. Laura's days at the magical knuck are over. She's now a professor at the University of Toronto's
carbure and her work focuses on early childhood development. With years of knowledge now under
her belt, she looks back on that moment at the slide and sees it as a form of communication.
In order for kids to look around and see what their peers are doing
and to understand the goals that are embedded
in those activities and to then realize
that they need to follow the social construct
of lining up.
It seems like a super simple thing,
but there's a lot of understanding about themselves,
about other people, about goals and activities
that they need to really
completely understand in order for that to happen.
Laura says actions like standing in line for the slide can be categorized as pro-social
behaviors.
They're actions we take to help others and to benefit the group.
She decided to study what she'd seen on the playground more systematically.
She invited a bunch of parents to bring their toddlers into the lab.
We were specifically testing 14 month old babies, so they're walking, they're not quite talking.
These 14 month olds said bye bye to mommy and daddy, and then was strapped into front facing baby carriers
worn by assistants in the study.
The researchers turned on some music.
OK, 1, 2, 3. assistance in the study. The researchers turned on some music.
And the person carrying the baby began to bounce.
It was like...
So we would bounce them down on one beat enough on the other, down and up sort of thing.
So if you're this baby, you're strapped onto someone's chest, you can't see their face,
instead you're looking in front of you at another assistant.
The person facing the baby would either move in synchrony with how they were being
bounced, so they were bouncing together, or they would move either too quickly or too slowly
so that their movements weren't aligned with what the baby was experiencing.
After about two and a half minutes, the bouncing stopped, slowly so that their movements weren't aligned with what the baby was experiencing.
After about two and a half minutes, the bouncing stopped, and the baby was removed from the
carrier.
And then this person who had faced them and moved either in or out of sync with them would
perform some little simple social games with them.
So she would do things like job pictures with markers or throw paper balls in a bucket.
Every now and then, the assistant who had faced the baby
would drop a marker or paper ball,
and then pretend like the object was out of reach.
She would reach, pathetically, for them,
for about 30 seconds, and we look to see what the babies did
in this really weird situation where this person they just
met needs help, but isn't really asking for help,
and doesn't really seem to be able to achieve
their goal on their own. Laura measured how many babies would pick up and hand back the objects.
And looked at whether the babies who were bounced in sync with helped at a different rate than
the babies we bounced out of sync with. She found that babies who felt that they were bouncing
in sync with a dance partner were more likely to help that partner pick up an object when it was out of reach.
It was remarkable.
So they would help on over half of the trials if we moved in sync with them, whereas if
we moved out of sync with them they would help on less than a third of the trials.
You've likely experienced the same drive if you've ever moved in sync with someone else,
maybe on a sports team in school or in marching band.
Or maybe like Laura as a dancer.
B-O-G-S-T-B-R-G-S-T
Put our service to the test.
Tie your napkin around your neck.
This song was a soundtrack of Laura's last recycle in high school
when she dressed up as Mrs. Parts for a performance of Beauty and the Beast.
There were many numbers in the performance,
but in the BR guests performance,
everyone comes together,
all the little kitchen utensils and everything,
and we do have to really synchronize
at certain parts of the dance.
And so when you're experiencing that,
you really feel like you're part of this bigger group,
and you're focusing on
doing all of these steps but that's become quite automatic at this point and
instead you can really sort of feel the connection with your code answers and
you start to just ignore the audience completely because you're part of this
bigger thing.
Laura's fellow dancers remain some of her closest friends to this day. You won your own free pass to be our guest if you're stressed.
Laura's fellow dancers remain some of her closest friends to this day.
I'm going to be the MC at one of their weddings in September.
So yeah, we've definitely stayed very close.
Music and dance create bonds because as Laura says, synchronous movement is a language of its own.
This unspoken language can guide behavior, even though many of us are not familiar with
its syntax and vocabulary.
I think music is providing a really interesting context where a lot of social cues are happening.
So when we're moving with other people, when we're doing familiar things and singing familiar
songs, these are actually queuing us babies and adults to think about the relationships that
we have with these people.
We think about teaching our kids to be pro-social and to be good citizens, but they're already attending to this information from a really early age.
The toddler experiment also shows that kids who can't speak are still hungry to communicate.
When a small child reaches out to pick up a marker or a paper ball, she may be saying,
I like you.
I want to help you.
Let's be friends.
I find babies are so impressive.
We can't really ask them what they're thinking.
We have to come up with clever ways of figuring out what's going on in their little brains,
and everybody underestimates them.
Music and dance aren't the only ways adults and small kids communicate with one another.
There's another language that actually sounds a lot like language.
The traditional way of looking and babbling, even as recently as 15, 20 years ago,
was really that it was just motor practice
that it had no bearing on later language.
It was just something babies did to exercise their mouths.
This is Rachel Lahlbert.
I am an assistant professor of psychology
at Leibniz Valley College in Anvil, Pennsylvania.
She studies infant language development.
Those quote unquote traditional views on babbling.
Rachel says, toss them out.
When babies babble,
they're indicating that they're either in a heightened state of arousal
or by actually babbling,
they're increasing their arousal level
and putting themselves in kind of this optimal state of being ready to learn.
Over and above, then if they were just quietly looking at
an object.
Rachel says Babbles have a certain jeunessey croix.
Bonjour toutez tous, je m'appelle Madame Macron.
When she hears a baby Babble, she's taken back to high school French class.
And I had one of those immersive French teachers who would only speak French in the classroom.
And so it was really like being dropped into un non-native environment, où il y a de faire des choses.
Je visite vraiment que je pense que elle serait de venir
la classroom et elle serait de commencer à parler.
La chambre de la chambre de la chambre est de quelque chose qui est
de faire des clairs de panic, comme elle serait de faire. Et donc, elle serait de faire de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la chambre de la down her speech and pointing and labeling particular objects in the room.
So she would in French be kind of pointing and saying, you know, are you sitting in a desk?
Look at the desk. Here's a desk.
And kind of repeating those key vocabulary words until we start to figure out, oh, okay,
this is the word for desk.
And I thought, well, this must be somewhat similar to what a baby experiences, right?
Where there's all this conversation going around.
Blah, blah, blah.
They start to become active communication partners and trying to engage in that world,
but how do they make sense of it?
Parents often see themselves as the know-it-all French teacher
and the babbling baby as a clumsy student.
Rachel says this framework is the one that's mostly used in studying infant language development.
But the baby is applying as much of a role in this interaction as the mom.
By changing the way that they babble and what they're looking at,
might actually be changing their opportunities for learning because they're changing what the parents say.
It's kind of what we call a social feedback loop.
So when the baby babbles, mom responds, the way mom responds actually will change in
real time what the child says next.
And they go around and around in this conversation influencing each other.
There's a word to describe a back and forth exchange, where the thing that one person says
affects what another person says. This feedback loop of communication and learning is what we call
language.
Babies make as many as a thousand vocalizations every day.
On average, parents respond to about 60% of these babbles.
Even just silently responding, if the mom just acknowledges the infants vocalization with
a touch, leans in smiles and touches the baby, the infant will immediately, within the span
of just 10 minutes, start to produce more speech-like vocalizations.
So that just that nonverbal feedback, that acknowledgement of the baby's vocalization,
changes in the moment how they're babbling.
Rachel says that when babies are babbling, they are more receptive to new information.
Parents can take advantage of this by having conversations with their baby.
And one rule of being a good conversationalist, stay attentive to what your partner is saying.
So parents that are just talking constantly about irrelevant things that aren't what
their child's looking at.
For example, if a baby's looking at a ball and I'm talking about this cup over here that
I'm playing with, well you're actually providing mismatching information for them that's going
to make it harder to learn.
Rachel says if you listen closely, babbling often falls into four distinct categories. They have different sounds and they elicit different responses. The first type is called QRV.
QRV is the least mature infant vocalization so sounds are quasi-resident vocalization. And so it's those really kind of nasal creaky kind of sounds that a lot of times parents
interpret almost as fussing. The second type is called, frv. So that's a fully resonant vowel,
and around three or four months the baby's vocal track opens up, and then they're able to produce
those more open kind of adult-like sounding vowels that kind of have those O's or A quality.
And so parents recognize those as more speech-like than those first, creaky, nasalized vowels,
and so they're more likely to respond to those.
The third is called MSFR.
So that's a marginal syllable, and babies will start around six or so months throwing
consonants into the mix.
So you can hear in that sound there's kind of a DJ kind of sound, but it's a slow drawn
out transition between when the consonant stops and when the vowel begins.
And the pinnacle.
The P as the resistance, if you will. So that's what's called a canonical syllable.
Those are those nice babas and dadas that parents naturally associate with babbling.
And so babies start regularly producing those pretty frequently around nine months.
Parents react very strongly to these canonical syllables.
They hear them as the most speech-like.
They tend to often interpret them as approximations of words.
So in that example, if a baby was holding a ball,
a mom would be very likely to respond,
oh, oh, that's a ball?
Yep, you're right, that's a ball.
You tried to say ball, that's great.
To Rachel, these different vocalizations are endlessly fascinating.
Now that she knows how to telepath the QRVs,
from the FRVs, and the MSFRs from the canonical
syllables, she simply can't stop herself.
On my wedding day, I remember very distinctly, I was in graduate school at the time and
need deep encoding this all the time, spending hours listening to these different sounds.
And I remember being up at the front of the church about to say my vows and hearing
some babies out in the audience, babble and
kind of immediately coating it and going, oh, that was a marginal syllable. And then thinking,
no, focus, this is a very important moment you're supposed to be attending to what you're
pastors saying.
You don't have to memorize all the jargon to take away something valuable from the research.
Just knowing that when babies are babbling,
they're ready to learn, that can be a useful takeaway
for everyone.
that can be a useful takeaway for everyone.
When we return, we discuss two final elements of baby language. One is going to make you very happy.
The other? Well, that's something you don't want to hear
as you're saying your wedding bells.
You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedantam.
This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedantam.
This hour we are demystifying the behavior of young children.
And we begin this next segment with a warning.
If you're driving or listening to the story while cooking,
you're about to hear something very distracting.
This is Katrina Doudna of Sunnyvale, California.
It's dinner time.
She's throwing a tantrum because she wants to sit at the head of the table.
Problem is, the table's round. There is no head.
When she was in the midst of a tantrum, she picked something that she knew was completely unreasonable.
I don't want my feet. Take my feet off. I don't want my feet. I don't want my feet."
That's Katrina's mom, Noemi. She talked with me a number of years ago about her then three-year-old daughter.
I once teased her, which is a turn out to be a big mistake, but I once said,
well, okay, let's go get some scissors and take care of your feet.
No!
My curiosity was focused on what are the elements of the tantrums,
are they, you know, the snips and snails and puppy dog tails or what?
This is Mike. It's Mike Potegal.
I am currently an associate professor in program
and occupational therapy at University of Minnesota.
Mike was inspired to study tantrums
after witnessing his own daughters, operatic meltdowns.
Among the things that I learned from that
was that there was a sequence in which various
behaviors appeared, various behaviors of which tantrums are composed. Just like Rachel Albert did
with babbling, Mike picked out a rhythm in the cacophony of his daughter's tantrums. To prove
his point, he teamed up with a colleague, James Green, he's a psychologist at the University of Connecticut. We developed a one Z that toddlers can wear that has a high quality wireless microphone
sewn into it. Parents put this one Z on the child and press a go button on the equipment.
And then they waited for a meltdown. James and Mike recorded more than a hundred of them
and what they found
Confirmed their hypothesis. They are two major emotional. I think of them as emotional behavioral components of tantrums The first is anger hitting kicking screaming throwing things pushing and pulling and the other component is
distress
whining crying
throwing the self down that is dropping to the floor, and comfort seeking.
Don't confuse these components with stages.
Mike says the behaviors occur simultaneously.
The distress is more consistent.
Throughout the tantrum, from its beginning to its end.
The anger punctuates the tantrum.
It provides the dramatic spikes, and it also tapers toward the end. The anger punctuates the tantrum. It provides the dramatic spikes, and it also tapers
toward the end. In every case, the anger terminates before the distress.
Mike says there's a reason that most tantrums end with a distress call.
Because the distress, sadness, tends to elicit comfort giving. And so it would make sense from an evolutionary perspective if the anger which is disruptive
is smoothed over by the stress component which then other things equal will call for parent
comforting the child.
Let's go back to Katrina Doudna, a three-year-old Maestra who's throwing a fit because she can't sit at the head of the family's
Circular dining table
Dad tries to reason with him
James Green says that he's fallen into the same trap before.
When children are at the peak of anger,
and they're screaming, and they're kicking,
probably asking questions, might prolong that period of anger.
Dad decides to intervene.
I'm going to pick you this chair for you.
Now Katrina's on the floor. Dad decides to intervene. I'm gonna pick you this chair for you. Woo! Woo!
Now Katrina's on the floor.
She grabs a stool and slams it against the wall.
It seems like things are escalating, but James says it's just the opposite.
Once she's thrown herself on the floor and thrown something, or in this case knocked the chair against the wall.
We're probably on the downslope of this tantrum.
She's spent a lot of energy, screaming, yelling, and now doing these physical behaviors.
Listen to Katrina and Pay attention to how the register of her voice changes. Mike says in many cases, parents can safely ignore their child's tantrums.
And this approach has a formal name planned ignoring, and it basically means turning
your back on the child and walking out of the room.
Because talking to them and telling them to stop is giving them attention, albeit negative attention.
This works best when the child throws a tantrum to get attention,
or because you've said no to eating cookies at dinner.
But if the child throws a tantrum and responds to something, you've asked them to do,
a very different approach is appropriate.
And the reason for that is that what the tantrum does, it's basically a stall.
It's a way for the child to not comply with the parent request.
The best approach, if it can be managed, is to get the child to comply with the request.
For younger children, this can be done with hand-over-hand kind of forced cooperation.
You can say to the child, okay, get your PJs on.
And if the child refuses, you can say, okay, I'm going to count to three or five
or whatever you like. And if you have not begun putting your PJs on, I'm going to
put my hands on your hands and help you do it. And children really do not like this.
Because he says it's a threat to their autonomy,
and if the child still doesn't comply,
maybe you can use it as an opportunity to do some research,
just like Mike.
We would walk down the street and I see a kid having a tantrum
and I would mutter or say out loud to my wife,
data, and then, of course, you give me a side-long look about
what's wrong with you.
We've talked so far about the secret languages of dancing and babbling.
We've looked at the grammar of temper tantrums.
Now we turn to Lala Bies.
Laura Cerelli, the first researcher we've featured on Dancing and Synchronization, has also done work on the language of music with infants.
Across cultures, we all engage with music and we do it at such an early age.
So we really wanted to continue to explore this idea that one of the real benefits and perhaps one of the underlying
reasons why we invest so much time and energy and music across cultures is
because of these social and emotional implications. Remember the magic of the
cleanup song that Laura talked about earlier? She and her fellow researchers wanted
to closely examine the effect of music on babies and their caregivers. They asked parents to volunteer
to come into the lab with their babies. 30 mothers said they would do it.
They would be in a sound proof room and babies would be sat in a little high
chair and moms would sit facing them. They were also measuring stress levels.
So we used little stickers on the bottom of the baby's foot and on the mother's fingertips.
And what we're able to measure with this is actually their sweat gland activity.
The mother is then sang two versions of Twinkle Twinkle Little Star, one version,
Twinkle Twinkle Twinkle Little Star.
How I wonder what you are. was upbeat.
The other?
Was a lullaby.
What we found was that when mom sang in a playful way, both mom and babies arousal levels stayed stable,
but when mom sang in a soothing way, both mom and
babies arousal levels decreased as the song progressed. So what's this, this is telling
us is that lullabies are really working to calm not only baby but also the mother. And
so this supports this idea that there's a function here.
Is this telling us just what, is it just confirming what our intuitions are telling us?
Or do you think it's telling us something more than that?
Anacdotally, I'm sure most parents would agree that little by suit their babies.
But usually when we're singing little by suit to our babies,
we're holding them and we're rocking them
and we're adding all of this really important soothing tactile information.
So we wanted to know what the song is doing, removing all of that tactile input.
So the babies are sitting in the high chair, they're facing their mom, but they're not experiencing
that holding and that rocking.
That typically comes along with all the bias.
But I think the really interesting thing here is we usually think of that unidirectional
relationship like when mom sings to babies, it is to change the baby's behavior. But I think it's
the really new interesting thing here is considering how it's also affecting the mom.
Indeed, if there's one thing that all this research shows, is that communicating with babies
is not a one-way street.
Parents and caregivers are shaped by what babies say and sing and scream, just as babies
are hungrily soaking up information from adults.
When we think about raising a human, it's natural that most of us think about the role that
adults play in shaping their children.
What's less clear, but what is abundantly true, is that babies are every bit is involved in raising humans of their own.
It's not just a figure of speech, we're all always growing up.
growing up.
When we come back, while less is sometimes more, when it comes to helping children learn.
You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta.
This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. Step into the basement of Alex Hamilton's home in Frederick, Maryland, and you'll notice
it has a distinct smell.
The wood dust?
That's from his carpentry projects.
Alex's basement is his workshop.
He's built things for every room in his house and outside too.
Alex is showing my producer Rina Cohen around.
Today he's building something for a beloved family member.
One of my dogs likes to go with me everywhere, so I'm making a bike seat just for her.
Alex calls himself a retired perfectionist.
Things don't have to be perfect for you to enjoy them.
That's especially true for another of Alex's hobbies.
Garden is out here. He steps outside his house to show us.
A sausage of a dog scampers up.
Butter bean.
That's my garden helper.
Alex walks around his garden pointing out the fruits and vegetables he's planted.
There's a blackberry?
Broccoli?
Sugar, baby, watermelon.
Artechokes?
Maybe not Artechokes.
Had one Artechoke plant and there was a rabbit who kept eating it down to the ground.
And a couple weeks later it would grow to be two or three inches tall, then the rabbit
would come back and eat it all the way down the ground.
So, good for him.
Never.
I still haven't seen what an hard joke plant looks like.
This pesky rabbit is one of the variables that distinguishes Alex's gardening from his carpentry.
When he's in his workshop, there are things that he can control.
The humidity, the shape, the size, the color.
But as a gardener, he's found he has to let go.
You can't control the weather, you can't control your rain, you can't control the humidity, so I would say more than half of the variables for gardening you have absolutely
no control. Carpenters draft plans and stick to them. Gardeners, well, you have things that creep
up on you all the time. Surprises are what make gardening so frustrating. Those weeds are really
awful. I hate those things. But surprises are also what make gardening so rewarding.
All these flowers here started from one flower and they propagated.
So at the end of the summer we had this beautiful flower bed and we didn't have to do anything.
Alex Hamilton's hobbies are a metaphor for something very different.
According to Berkeley Psychology Professor Alison Gopnik,
the different philosophies of the carpenter and the gardener
play out every day in how parents interact with their children.
In her book, The Gardener and the Carpenter,
Alison looks at two approaches to being a parent
and their consequences.
Your book is built around an analogy.
Parents behaving like gardeners, parents
behaving like carpenters. Unpack those analogies for me.
Well, if you look at the prevailing culture of parents and caregiving in the United States,
it's a picture that's a lot like the picture you might imagine if you thought about a carpenter.
And the idea is that if you just do the right things, get the right skills, read the right books,
you're going to be able to shape your child
into a particular kind of adult.
And that picture is very different
from the kind of picture that comes from the science.
The picture that comes from the science
is much more like being a gardener.
Now, one thing about being a gardener
is you never know what's going to happen in the garden,
the things that you plan, fail,
but then wonderful things happen
that you haven't actually planned.
And there's actually a deeper reason for that.
And the reason is that what being a gardener is all about
is creating a rich, nurturing,
but also variable, diverse, dynamic ecosystem
in which many, many different things can happen,
and a system that can respond to the environment
in unpredictable
kind of ways.
And I think the science suggests that being a caregiver for human beings is much more
like being a gardener than being a carpenter.
It's much more about providing a protected space in which unexpected things can happen
than it is like shaping a child to come out to be a particular kind of desirable at all.
I was surprised to read that the term parenting itself isn't very old.
When did the term become popular and how did it come to be?
Yeah, it's interesting. The very word parenting, which seems so ubiquitous
and taken for granted now, is actually quite recent.
So if you look in Google Engram, it's only around the 1970s in America that the word first
begins to really take off.
And then there's this kind of exponential increase from the 70s up to the present moment.
This is Focus on the Family, hosted by psychologist Dr. James Dobson, the author of such best-selling
books on the family as parenting isn't for cowards.
Hey, parents, listen up to this.
This could be revolutionary.
You have to change the way you think about parenting,
because Amy Chouai ignited a firestorm
by sharing the surprising details of her strict parenting methods
in the book, Battle Him of the Tiger Mother.
And if you think about it, parenting is a kind of strange word.
After all, we don't.
Wipe our husbands or child our parents.
What we say is that those are relationships.
I am a child or I am a wife.
It's not a kind of goal-directed activity that I'm doing that has a particular
outcome that I'm trying to achieve.
And I think being a parent is a much better way of describing what that
relationship is all about than parenting,
than changing a
child into something else.
So it's interesting that that switch in some ways to a verb changes what we think about
the activity.
As you say, we don't sort of start a friendship saying in three years time, I want my friend
to become ex and I'm going to raise my friend to become that person that I want him or
her to be.
Yeah, in fact, if you think about something like a friendship,
of course, it's when friends are in bad shape
that the friendship counts the most.
I think there was actually a historical reason
why the word appeared in the late 20th century
and the culture that goes along with it,
which has become more and more intense,
even in the 30 years since I had my own children.
And I think that reason is that for the first time in human history, people were having
children, especially middle class American people, who had not had much experience of
caring for children before.
So one of the things, again, that comes out of the science is for as long as we've been
human, older brothers and sisters and cousins and aunts and grandmothers
and the whole village has been involved in caring for children.
And that meant that for most of human history,
by the time you were ready to have children yourself,
you'd had lots of practical experience
in caring for children.
You'd also watched lots of different people,
not just your own parents,
but many other people care for children.
And what happened in the 20th century was because families got smaller,
and people got to be more mobile, and people had children at a later age.
For the first time, people were having children who hadn't had much experience
of caring for children, but had had lots of experience of going to school and working.
So I think it was kind of natural for people to think,
oh, okay, this thing that I'm about to do,
this is like going to school and working,
and if I can just find the right manual
or the right secret handbook,
I'm gonna succeed at this task the same way
that I succeeded in my classes or I succeeded in my job.
So I think that historical fact is a lot of the reason why
this culture and with it this kind of sense of anxiety and worry and
also this billion dollar parenting industry all started emerging at the end of the 20th century.
Allison points out that compared to other primates, humans have an especially long childhood.
We just sort of take it for granted that children need to be taken care of for a long time,
but that's actually sort of evolutionarily paradoxical.
And there's an interesting question about why did that happen.
And one idea at least is that having that long period of childhood gave you this protected
period where you could figure out a new environment.
You could move, say, from one village to another, or from one place to another, and childhood
gave you a chance to master that new environment.
And I think that a lot of the things that seem kind of strange about children, the fact
that they're simultaneously so creative, so imaginative, so exploratory, and yet so
bad at taking care of themselves, kind of fit that picture.
What do you think the harm is of parents trying to be carpenters?
Well, I mean, it's a tricky question. I think the main harm is that it makes the process,
the life of being a parent, anxious and difficult and tense and unhappy in all sorts of ways
that are unnecessary. And I think it makes it that way for parents, and it makes it that way for children.
Now, the question about how any kind of behavior
on the part of parents influences children in the long run
is very, very complicated.
So another piece of this evolutionary picture
is that every individual child has their own characteristics.
Every parent does.
There's this complicated interaction
between the parents,
distinctive characteristics in the child, so that actually trying to say, well, if you do
this, then your child is going to come out like that in the long run.
That's pretty much of a mugs game, and I don't think we have very much evidence for that.
So I wouldn't want to say, ah, well, if you're a carpenter, then your children are going
to come out and have some terrible, crazy feature. Children come out in all sorts of unexpected ways. That's the whole point.
It's not just that the carpentry model is making parents and kids stressed. There's
something Addison has noticed about today's adolescence.
You know, in some ways they're doing much better. They're achieving more. They're less likely
to get pregnant or to use drugs. But that goes with a kind of anxiety, high
levels of anxiety, high levels of fear.
And I think that is kind of what you would predict from the Carpentry story.
So the Carpentry story is one where you're so concerned that the child come out, that
you're not giving the child the freedom to take risks and explore and be autonomous.
And it's not risk-taking,
unless there's some chance that it could really go wrong.
And I think that's another aspect
of the current parenting culture that's problematic.
We're so concerned about how these children
are gonna turn out, that we're unwilling to give them
the autonomy that they need to be able to take risks
and go out and explore the world.
So I'm wondering if some of this has to do with what
your goals are as a parent.
The point that I think you're making is that
by creating an environment where children can learn
and explore, you build children who perhaps
are going to be less anxious, more resilient,
more able to deal with the vagaries of life
in front of them.
And I get that, and that does make sense.
But there's also the case that I think our world rewards people who can do very specific things
and do them very well.
So, every four years, the Olympics comes along.
When I see people who are winning gold medals in the Olympics,
those are usually kids who've started ice skating lessons when they were three.
Or you see someone who is joining the orchestra in your town and that someone who has started
you know piano lessons when she was four.
And there are these very specific skills that I think are very hard to learn and take
an enormous amount of time and dedication and practice.
And if you don't actually invest the time to do that early, it becomes very hard to master
those skills later on.
And even though you might say, you know, let the child figure out what it is he or she might want to do,
if a child discovers that she really wants to be an ice-skitter or a ballet dancer when she's 14,
it's probably too late at that point to really be very good at it.
And I'm wondering if some of the tension here comes from what it is that we're actually aiming for.
Are we aiming for children who are well is that we're actually aiming for.
Are we aiming for children who are well-adjusted versus aiming for children who are successful?
And I wish there was an attention between those two things, but I sometimes think they
might be.
Well, it's interesting.
There's no question that part of the cultural background for the parenting approach.
Is this sense that parents have of being
in a very competitive universe where, you know,
small advantages to their children
in terms of their education, for example,
are going to be absolutely crucial to make sure
that the children continue to be in the middle class
or the upper middle class.
The combination of increasing inequality
and increasing relationship between that inequality and things like academic achievement
I think puts a great deal of stress on middle class parents to make sure that their children get into that academic achievement track.
The very moments when parents think they're helping their kids learn, they may in fact stand in the way.
learn, they may in fact stand in the way. Allison described an experiment that shows just what happens when adults take the lead
or hang back.
This is work that was done by Elizabeth Bonoets and Laura Schultz and what they did was they
showed the children this complicated thing that could do lots of different things.
It could squeak, had a mirror, it had a light.
And the question was, would the children discover all the things that the toy could do?
And what they found was that if you give a complicated toy like that
to a four-year-old, as you might expect,
that four-year-old's play,
and they find all the things that it could do.
But when the experimenter just changed what they said,
this is my toy, I'm going to show you how it works,
and did one thing like squeak the squeaker.
The children were much less likely to explore.
What they did, sort of rationally, was squeak the squeaker, the children were much less likely to explore. What they did, so rationally, was squeak the squeaker did what the adult had demonstrated and suggested to them.
And we have a number of different results that are like this where children are very, very sensitive
to quite subtle indicators that someone's being pedagogical, that someone's being a teacher.
And that has some advantages, that narrows down the number of options they're
going to consider.
But it can also have disadvantages in terms of the range of exploration that they're
willing to consider too.
Now, of course, play itself can be a form of learning, and one way to think about the
importance of play is to think about artificial intelligence and how play informs the way
we train robots.
You talk about this idea in the book, how are researchers
using play to help robots learn? And what does that tell us then about the science of raising
kids?
Well, it turns out that a very good way of getting a machine system to learn is to give it an
early period where it can just play, it can just try out lots and lots of different options
and get a lot of information about how the system works. So for example, they designed a robot that had a period
early on where it could just kind of dance around in this kind of weird funny way without actually trying to do
anything in particular. And it turns out that if you gave a robot a chance to just dance around,
figure out what its limbs could do, and then you gave it a specific job,
like, you know, go and pick up this piece of cloth.
The robot was more resilient if it had a chance to play,
and this is one of the interesting things about play.
I think everyone has kind of the intuition
that play is important and valuable.
But of course, if you actually want to have a specific outcome,
like a particular score on a test,
you're always going to do better if you don't play, if you just have very specific instruction.
But if you want that knowledge to be resilient, if you want someone to be able to be flexible,
to say, okay, I didn't learn how to do this particular thing, but now can I apply what
I did to something else, then play really seems to play a deep role.
So for the robots, for example, the robots who had played, you could take off one of their
arms or you could tip them over and they would still be able to figure out how to reach
for the cloth.
And that was not true for the robots who had just been trained on that particular action.
And I think that's a good model for the things that play can do in general.
So you were once in the kitchen with your grandson, Ogi, and you were whipping egg whites for pancakes,
and I understand your grandson was imitating you, but not quite doing it the way you were
doing it. Tell me what happened.
One of the great joys of life is cooking with two and three-year-olds, and they love to
cook, but of course they're extremely inefficient at the same time.
So, you know, what Oggy's whipping egg whites,
the egg whites end up having egg white fresco
all over the walls, and he's doing it in a way
that is not nearly as efficient as it would be
for me to do it myself.
But it's interesting that if you look across cultures,
what caregivers often do is not give their child
instructions on what to do do but let them participate.
So let them take on a piece like whipping the eggs.
Alison and I had mostly focused on how young children learn, so I asked her to talk about older children.
She said that whereas younger children learn well through play and exploration,
older kids learn best through an apprenticeship.
They imitate, they practice learn best through an apprenticeship. They imitate,
they practice, they get critiques. But most schools teach in a very different way. Alison gave me
a thought experiment. So imagine we taught baseball the same way that we teach science currently. What
we would do is we would have children read books about baseball rules. When they got to high school,
we would let them reproduce famous baseball
plays of the past. And it wouldn't be till graduate school that they would actually ever
get to play the game. And that's pretty much the way that we teach science. It's not
till graduate school that you actually ever get a chance to do science as opposed to reading
about science or reconstructing science. And, you know, especially with things like virtual
reality and computer environments.
There's no reason for that to be true.
Children could be actually doing inquiry and doing experiments and doing science early
on.
And I think the same thing's true for things like writing, for example.
Children learn about writing, but the way to write is to write with a good editor to watch
someone who's competent writing.
I think our whole educational system could be oriented towards both exploration,
but also this kind of apprenticeship system
much more effectively.
I'm wondering if some of the tension
between the Gardner and the Carpenter Model
comes down to the different things
that societies have called on young people to do.
So there used to be a time when we needed
large numbers of people to join the industrial
workforce, but increasingly I think we are looking to young people to do the kinds of things that
are much higher order skills. And I think the average futurist would tell you that in 20 or 30 years,
a college education probably isn't going to cut it because the kinds of skills that someone
who's graduated from college
can do will probably be done by robots.
And so at that point, you really need people who have much, much higher levels of skill.
And I'm wondering if this is partly the tension between the Gardner and the Carpenter model,
because if you actually need people who don't just simply understand, you know, how to,
how to do a bunch of things in a factory, but to understand much higher order things,
that requires a level of specialization and a length of training that is not just going
to be acquired through exploration and play.
It actually has to be acquired through systematic and diligent training over very, very long
periods of time.
Is it possible that the way our society is changing is part of the reason why people
are drawn to the
carpentry model?
Well, I think that's the picture that people have.
But again, what the science would tell us is just the opposite.
So, you know, if you compare, even from an evolutionary perspective, you know, you compare
us to our closest primate relatives, we're not actually as good at doing specialized tasks
often as other creatures are.
What we're very good at is improvising and finding new ways of thinking about the world
around us.
Here's a new problem.
I don't know how to handle it.
What would I do?
That's really the skill that we're going to need in the future.
And the irony is that that's a skill that you can't learn by just trying to learn that
particular skill.
That's a skill that seems to come from a sense of freedom and exploration even in other domains. So I think we may be kind of
suckered into thinking, well, if we want children or adults to succeed in this very unpredictable
variable world, what we need to do is give them higher and higher and higher levels of very
specific academic skills. And I think, you know, even if you go down to Silicon Valley near where I am at Berkeley, I think
they get it.
So, you know, famously Google gives their employees a day off to do whatever they want,
and, you know, Pixar has playhouses as part of their environment.
So I think the value of that kind of playful exploration is something that people say in
the tech world get. But I do think it's intention with the sense that this is so important. It has to
be shaped and you want a particular outcome. So the irony is to get to good outcomes, sometimes
you do better by not trying specifically to get to those outcomes and instead not worrying
about outcomes at all. And I think that's one of the real deep messages of the science
that I'm talking about in the book.
Allison Gopnik is a professor of psychology and philosophy at the University of
California Berkeley. She's the author of several books about children's development
and her most recent one is The Gardener and The Carpenter.
Allison, thank you so much for joining me today on Hidden Brain.
Thank you so much, Shankar.
Hidden Brain is produced by Hidden Brain Media. Our production team includes Bridget McCarthy,
Kristen Wong, Laura Quarell, Ryan Katz,
Autumn Barnes and Andrew Chadwick.
Tara Boyle is our executive producer.
I'm Hidden Brain's executive editor.
Our unsung heroes this week are all the teachers and caregivers who've helped the parents
of the Hidden Brain team over the years.
Thanks for your dedication to our children, for your patience and creativity in helping them to learn and thrive. We're endlessly grateful.
If you're a fan of the show and you'd like to pitch into help support our work,
there's a way you can do so. Just go to support.hiddenbrain.org where you can make a gift in any amount.
It really gives us an emotional boost
to see that you value the show and want to help it thrive. That site again is support.hiddenbrain.org.
I'm Shankar Vedantam.
See you soon. you