Hidden Brain - Crying Wolf
Episode Date: April 11, 2023It used to be that we tried our best to conceal disadvantages, hardships, and humiliations. But new research explores a curious shift: some people are flaunting limitations that don't exist. This week..., we talk to psychologists Karl Aquino and Jillian Jordan about the strange phenomenon of wanting to seem worse off than we really are.Think back to the last time you tried to win an argument. What could you have done to bolster your case? Check out our recent episode "Less is More" for helpful strategies. And if you like our work, please consider supporting it! See how you can help at support.hiddenbrain.org. Â
Transcript
Discussion (0)
This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta.
Six years ago, I was diagnosed with a rare, inoperable form of bone cancer called
Condrosarcoma.
A cancer I still have to this day.
Elizabeth Finch used to be a writer on the popular medical TV show, Grey's Anatomy.
Other writers said they deferred to her opinions while writing episodes, since, as a cancer
patient, she was the authority on what patients were going through. I'll told my cancer cost me my hair, my immune system, one knee, half my bank account, half
my thirties, and one functioning kidney.
I mean never be cancer free.
But in a December 2022 interview with the Ancler, a newsletter about goings on in Hollywood,
Elizabeth Finch admitted that her cancer diagnosis was a lie.
One of her colleagues told the newsletter that Elizabeth Finch was someone who sported, quote, a visible chemo-port bandage who regularly took breaks to vomit, who only ate
saltines for long periods of time, and who wrote and talked about her experiences all the time.
We were unable to reach Elizabeth Finch for a response.
Her story may seem extraordinary,
but she's not alone in making false claims.
In 2021, the website Intelligent.com surveyed
more than 1200 white college applicants.
More than a quarter of the respondents
on the online survey said they had lied about
being a racial minority on their college application.
Blanca Véagoméz is the website's Higher Education Counselor. She says that most of the people
who lied claimed to be Native American.
And we found that the number one reason for why these applicants lied was they believe that they
would improve their chances of getting accepted.
It used to be that we tried our best to conceal disadvantages, hardships and humiliations.
Trauma's were often experienced as shameful and hidden away.
In fact, in previous centuries, historians have documented numerous
cases of people who were the target of discrimination, who did their best to avoid the labels that
would invite prejudice. Many people of color try to pass as white to escape Jim Crow laws.
Movies and plays document how women try to escape sexism and sexual violence by pretending to be men.
As Jews fled the Holocaust, some pretended to be Gentiles.
But new research explores a curious shift.
Even as many people with limitations and disabilities continue to hide their challenges,
others flaunt hardships that don't exist.
This week on Hidden Brain, this train psychology of wanting to seem worse off than we really are. Why would you invent a disadvantage that doesn't exist?
For a man by calling Arthur, who grew up in a small town outside Mexico City, the answer
was clear.
It was a way to avoid being the target of homophobic bullying.
He vividly remembers a traumatic event during his freshman year of high school.
Students had gathered for a social event.
We were supposed to be there just like to spend some time and to have fun and have his and bands and things. And then these guys kind of surrounded me.
I think that they didn't try to,
you know, like physically attack me,
but because I was small and they were so big,
I felt like, oh my God, these guys
are gonna do something to me.
Like bullies everywhere,
they knew exactly where to strike.
And they were like, are you gay?
I was like, no, no, no, no, no, no, like, and then they were like, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, like.
And then they were like, no, no, no, we know your gay.
And then I was so scared.
I was about to cry.
And then my English teacher back then, she realized
what was going on.
And she got them away from me.
That was the first time that I actually felt like they
could physically harm me.
So as Arthur went off to college, he decided to invent a story.
He felt he could use it as a shield.
Nice set.
I need to create a character that is more appealing than who I actually am.
Growing up, Arthur had watched his family work with Jewish business people, and he had always
admired the professional way of handling things.
And so, the character that I created was like a Jewish descendant of my grandfather that
was escaping the Spanish civil war. And so when people ask me like,
hey, who are you?
They're like, instead of saying like,
yeah, I come from a small town in the middle of nowhere.
Hi, it was like, no, yes, I come from, you know, like my family.
I'm blah, blah, blah.
And I felt so cool.
The story gave Arthur a new identity. He wasn't a gay kid from small town Mexico.
He was a glamorous grandson of Jewish refugees who had survived persecution in Europe.
I think that it basically shielded me from homophobic bullying.
I thought that if it ever came to that,
that people would have a harder time discriminating me
because I came from that background of persecution.
But if I only said, yeah, I'm gay and it's a theorizing
that I exposed me much more to that ugly side of
bullying and discrimination. So I think that this narrative, like I come from that group that has
already suffered that much, like kind of shielded me in a way.
Can you say more about that? Because if you're suffering from one form of discrimination,
it's not clear to me why identifying yourself with the second form of discrimination
leaves you better off. It feels now you're pure setback in two different ways. Why did you
feel it would make it would make a difference or did it actually make a difference?
I think that it made a difference because homophobic discrimination in Mexico is
much more tolerated than anti-semitism, for example. Openly discriminating at you is something
that very few people would dare to do.
But openly discriminating at gay person
is something that happens on a daily basis everywhere.
So I think that it left me better off.
And even though I was playing the character of gay,
Jew, man, I was still feeling more protected in that character than if I had been purely gay,
for example.
Now some people might challenge Arthur's beliefs about the extent of anti-Semitism in
Mexico.
But whether or not those beliefs were accurate, Arthur found his new backstory gave him confidence
as he talked to friends.
The long historical persecution of the Jews was now his story.
People started to show him a certain deference.
So I would be like, yes, they are listening to me.
You know, I haven't survived and I'm being here and facing you and speaking with pride about my family history and all those things
like it made me brave in a way.
Over time, the story morphed.
Depending on the situation, Arthur would play up the victim in persecution angle.
Other times, he would use his invented identity as a badge of expertise, playing on stereotypes
that Jews were good at business.
As he went through college, he found his story had fresh uses.
Other people might have glamorous vacations and expensive habits, but his story of trauma
and survival also had cachet.
It was almost a form of currency.
After he graduated, Arthur was working as an intern at the British Embassy.
He was invited to a fancy end of your party.
And that was the British residence, the house of the ambassador. And you can imagine like how
gorgeous it was, like the long tables, the beautiful meal and everything. And so we were all invited
there and I was so self-aware, I was like, oh my god, what am I gonna say? Around the dinner table.
And I was sitting around with all the other interns
and their expensive universities and they started talking about their trips and yeah my dad did this,
my dad did that, my mom traveled there and I was just sitting at the table and was like, oh, I got what I'm going to say.
And then eventually, it was my turn to say something. And then I was like, yeah, my dad is going to close that business.
Because you know, we have a lot of connections like in the community.
And they were like, what community?
And I was like, the community.
Like, did you wish community?
like did you wish community.
And then they were like, oh my god, I like it. Like, and then I was like, yes, I am.
And yeah, like in that situation, I felt like, oh my god, it's there.
It worked, it worked.
It was at another job after this internship that Arthur realized his invented story contained both benefits and risks.
He found himself regularly clashing with a coworker.
They had never gotten along well.
On this day, they disagreed over an invoice. And I was like, what did you say?
And in that moment, I think that she knew
that she had already crossed the line.
Arthur realized that his invented story of victimhood
contained the risk of turning him into an actual victim.
But he also saw how the incident gave him the power
to undermine a coworker whom he disliked.
I needed to accuse her.
Yeah, yeah, like I needed to accuse her. Yeah, yeah, like like I needed to accuse her and I detailed my voice like you know what
this happened.
But then instead of wanting her fired, I think that I wanted to get away from the situation
like it was just too harsh and I was just thinking like no let's leave it here.
And at the same time mom I had already received my approval
for studying in Australia. So I was like, enough, like this is the end.
And so you left that position and you went to Australia? Did you keep the story alive there?
No, no, and what happened? I thought that I was already cool like I was there like I didn't think that I need that character anymore
In Australia Arthur didn't have to invent a story about how he was an outsider
He was a gay foreign student from Mexico. He was obviously an outsider
He no longer needed to be the descendant of persecuted Jews.
Yeah, yeah, that's what happened.
That's what happened.
And then I made a ton of friends over there,
fellow Mexicans, and from all over the place where they're still in touch.
And, yeah, exactly, that's exactly what happened.
And yeah, exactly, that's exactly what happened.
Arthur had discovered that his invented story of persecution gave him a number of advantages.
On the surface, this is a paradox.
Why would inventing hardships or trauma be an advantage?
At a meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, I came by research
that explained one dimension of this paradox.
Psychologist Jillian Jordan at Harvard divided volunteers into two groups.
She offered each slightly different scenarios about a college student named Sarah, who invites
some friends over to her dorm room.
One of her classmates, Gabrielle, asks to use her iPad to look something up and she agrees.
And then eventually, Gabrielle and all the other classmates leave and Sarah relaxes at home and watches a TV show on her iPad and then she goes out. And in the neutral condition, that's the whole story.
And in the victim condition, while she's out, we explain to participants that Gabrielle
broke into her dorm room and stole her iPad.
And so, you know, in both cases, she uses the iPad, she lends it to Gabrielle, she goes
out, and then only in the victim condition does Gabrielle actually break in and steal her iPad.
Note that in both conditions, Sarah's behavior is exactly the same.
The only difference in the scenarios is that in one, she is a victim of theft. In the other, she is not.
Julian asked volunteers whether they thought Sarah was a good person. Since Sarah's behavior in both scenarios was identical, there should be no difference
in how she was perceived by people in the two groups.
But that is not what Julian found.
When Sarah was a victim of theft, she is considered by participants to be a more moral and a more
trustworthy person than the neutral condition.
Jillian and her colleagues call this phenomenon the virtuous victim effect.
So the virtuous victim effect is the finding that people tend to see victims of wrongdoing
as more, more, more, good people than non-victims who have behaved identically.
When we think about what it means to be a morally good person,
we typically think about it means you've taken actions
that are morally good and you haven't taken actions
that are morally bad, right?
So I'm a morally good person.
If I help others, I'm kind, I'm trustworthy,
I'm not a morally good person.
If I betray people, I spread nasty gossip,
I'm never willing to help, I just think about myself.
And in this case, people's moral character
is being influenced, not by actions they took.
We talked about how those are constant across conditions
in our experiments, but rather by the ways that they're treated
by other people.
And I think this seems sort of irrational in the sense that both good and bad people
could be mistreated by somebody else. It doesn't really tell you something meaningful
about my proclivity to behave morally, to learn that somebody else treated me badly.
And yeah, the way that I'm treated by somebody else can influence perceptions of my moral
character.
else can influence perceptions of my moral character.
Arthur intuitively grasped this when he came up with a story of family persecution. People seem to think he was a better person because his ancestors had suffered adversity.
You can also see the same phenomenon on a larger scale.
In many political disputes, two groups will each try
to claim the moral high ground by talking about how
they have been wronged.
In these situations, people are not making up traumas,
but selectively focusing on traumas
that show why they are deserving of sympathy.
When the Israelis talk about how they are besieged by enemies
who seek to destroy them, they are inviting us to see them as virtuous victims.
When Palestinians focus on how they have been dispossessed by the Israelis, they are telling
us that they are the virtuous victims.
Gillian told me researchers were increasingly interested in studying such escalating wars
for the moral high ground.
I should say there's also a lot of cool work around this concept of competitive victimhood
when there's like a conflict between two groups and both sides wants to compete
to like paint themselves as the rightful or larger victim.
Sort of even without my research, like we can tell that first of all,
if two groups are accusing the other side of being the perpetrator such that they're the victim,
like you want to kind of compete to be the victim because that means by definition that the other side of being the perpetrator such that they're the victim, like you want to
kind of compete to be the victim because that means by definition that the other side is bad.
When we come back, three girls learn what it feels like to play the victim
and new research into the personality traits that
drive such behavior.
You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedantam.
Some time ago, a listener called in with an unusual story.
We'll call her Paula. She told us that when she was in high school many years ago, she
got close to two other students. We'll call them Sarah and Jessica. We're using pseudonyms at the request of the women.
By the time Jessica, Sarah and Paula were in 10th grade, they were all considered high
achievers.
Paula was the athlete.
I was definitely the jock in my school.
If there was ever anything sport-related, I was on the team.
I was likely captain and VP. That's just kind of the reputation
I had carved out for myself. Jessica was a social activist. She was very invested in helping the
environment. So she actually started her own environmental youth group. And Sarah was a star
who made Stardom look effortless. She was very good at procrastinating, doing everything super last minute, but ended up becoming
straight-A student, was on all these committees, and was beloved by everyone in our school.
Sarah was also a Thessbian, and acted in local community theatre.
Paula told me that the three friends felt like they were headed somewhere.
They had talent, and they had drive. One summer, when the girls were 16, they went on vacation together. It was their first time
traveling without their parents. They spent four days in a new city visiting museums,
eating at restaurants and touring local colleges. For their last adventure, they went to a popular amusement park.
When we got there, it looked really grand, so there's just people everywhere.
It was probably the most people I've ever seen in one place.
The lines for all the rides were over two hours to get to the main rides, and so we're
definitely a bit overwhelmed and feeling like we weren't going to be able to see all the
things that we wanted to see on this trip.
The girls had done some research beforehand to figure out what rides the park offered.
As the day grew hotter, they set their hearts on some water rides.
It's a little bit more fun, not the drop type rides. I don't really like those,
but I like the ones that kind of go on an adventure. And there was like water features with them.
So that's why they were such a draw on this day
because it was so hot.
Everybody was wanting to go on the rides
where you kind of like splashed into water.
They got in line for their first ride.
It really was two hours long.
And as they waited and waited,
and it got hotter and hotter,
their moods plummeted.
If this is what they had to look forward to the rest of the day,
they were going to be miserable. They forward to the rest of the day, they were going
to be miserable. They could see the lines for the other rides were also really long.
It didn't feel worth it, I guess, to have gone all the way, to have gone our way all the way
to this park. We were really excited for the day, and so I think we were a bit kind of between
frustrated and disappointed as well, because we're kind of waiting in line for this long time
like we enjoy each other's company, but we also really wanted to experience everything there.
So I think we were like the combination of frustrated, disappointed and a little desperate.
The three girls looked at each other. They were driven, they were smart, and they wanted to find a way to skip the long lines.
So we were kind of brainstorming ways to kind of get around this dilemma, this problem
that was in front of us.
And so we were kind of came up with the idea of like, okay, well, what type of people don't
have to stand in line.
And we were like, okay, people who work work here potentially maybe they get like a
special pass or you have to like pay more money and then we saw somebody kind of
literally roll up in a wheelchair up the exit and if you go up the exit you just
skipped the front of the line and we just kind of looked at each other and were like, maybe there's something there.
One of them throughout the idea,
to create a character with a disability
who could get them to the front of the lines.
Sarah, the actress, volunteered to take on the role.
She was actually seemed excited by the idea that she could use her acting skills.
And so the rest of us kind of came up with the story behind what was happening.
And so myself and Jessica were family members. And so I looked a little bit more like Sarah.
And so I was going to be Sarah's cousin and Jessica was going to be my friend.
The girls knew they couldn't fake a physical
disability, so they decided to have Sarah fake a neurological disability. Sarah, I
think if I were to think about the type of disability it is, I would say that
she was kind of on the nonverbal autism spectrum would have been kind of the performance, I guess. And so she was acting agitated from the heat.
There was noise, the whole performance.
I mean, she crushed it.
Paula, Jessica, and the new Sarah activated
the next phase of their plan.
And we had her sitting off to the side, and then I went up to the booth, and I kind of
explained and asked questions, like, do you have any passes or anything that we can do
for my cousin, and she was on the bench visible kind of behaving in a way that made it a little bit
more believable and we had a towel over her head and we like put water on it because we were trying
like cool her down and she was like grunting and groaning and and flailing her arms and and
everything while she was sitting and kind of like hitting Jessica a little bit.
So it was like a full, full like auditory and visual performance.
Despite Sarah's acting, the people at the ticketing booth said no to the request for special passes.
It's possible, of course, that they had encountered other people who I tried to game the system.
I could tell that the workers there, they seem like now when I think back, they were definitely like our age.
They would have been teenagers as well, but they were definitely skeptical.
And so they said no, and so then I went over to my friend and we kind of talked a little bit and kind of came up with a game plan that
that Sarah and Jessica would come over with me to make it a little bit more believable.
The three girls ramped up the pressure on the teenagers in the ticketing booth.
Sarah continued to act disabled and distressed. She just was consistent.
She was very good at kind of keeping this whole
very good at kind of keeping this whole Shred up the whole time she seemed agitated and hot and after a little bit I had
her and Jessica go back and sit down again and a big chunk of it was us kind of
going off and me fake consoling her and sitting on the bench with her. I think we
were trying to make it seem a little bit more believable by not going
anywhere and giving up.
And I kind of continued to negotiate with the workers.
The workers pointed out that passes were meant for people who could not physically stand in line.
Sarah could stand. She didn't need a wheelchair.
We really kind of had to position our argument that waiting in line in the heat was going to make her disruptive.
I believe they ended up having to get a supervisor come give us the pass.
I think it probably took us about 20 or 30 minutes, the whole ordeal.
So 20 or 30 minutes, I mean, that is, that is, I thought you were going to say two or three minutes. So 20 or 30 minutes is like, I mean, that is like, that's a lot.
Yeah.
We were very dedicated and committed individuals.
We kind of had like a goal in mind.
We were very tunnel vision, tunnel focused on kind of getting
what we wanted out of the situation.
The first thing they did after the supervisor
interceded to give them the passes was to head over to the entrance
to a water right. They went right past a gigantic, snaking line of hot, agitated people.
And instead of standing in line at the start, we just walk in the exit so where all the
people come out after they finish the ride, show them our pass, they just opened it up and
we got first crack at sitting wherever we wanted and it was amazing. We just did
the same water ride like three times in a row. We were thinking we were so smart, we were so
elated, so excited, was to stick to it.
Sarah couldn't step out of character.
I found it so entertaining that my friend had to stay in character, and she stayed in character
on the ride.
So we're going through these drops and turns and everything like that.
And she's fully expressing in this character the entire time.
And so she would bring her arms forward and sort of flutter and flail them, like kind of
cheering and the elation and giggling and laughing.
She was definitely like having like repetitive movements of joy.
Like that's the part that if you have like a that flash bulb
or that photographic kind of memory,
like the thing that's imprinted on my brain
is like watching her perform joy while on the rides
and like the overtone or the feeling that we
were having at the time was like we outsmarted somebody. The three girls stayed in
the amusement park for several hours and went on every ride they wanted. Paula
estimates they were able to go on 15 different rides, some of them multiple
times. Without the passes, they might have
made it to two or three. On the way home, Paula, Jessica and Sarah had a talk. They
agreed not to tell anyone else about what they had done. In fact, they came up with a
story of how tough the day had been. Yeah, we had a story that the lines were really long,
we only went on a few rides.
It was really fun, but we're really tired, that kind of thing. So we did kind of have a
a chat about a story to make sure that we were all kind of on the same page.
It was only after a while that they started to rethink their actions.
I think once we were coming home, it started to kind of really sink in what like the gravity
of the situation and how it's not really in line with who we are as people or who we want
to be as people or how we see ourselves rather.
And so it
did take a while for it to fully sink in. I think it was definitely like once we were back
home we started thinking that we had done something shameful.
As an adult, Paula has sometimes thought back to those teenagers in the ticketing booth who were suspicious of Sarah's performance.
She realizes that they gave in not because they were convinced by her story, but because
they felt silenced by it.
I don't know if they actually believed us us or if it was just so uncomfortable to call
somebody out for faking something like that because imagine if we weren't faking it, how
horrible that would be for an employee to say you're faking it.
So I think we probably put them in a really uncomfortable position and so they ended up
just agreeing.
When we come back, the personality traits of people who cry wolf.
You're listening to Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedanta.
When Kala Kena was six years old, all the kids at school were asked to share what they
ate for breakfast. So I remember saying that what I had was a dish made out of pigs feet and banana
blossoms. And that generated quite a bit of laughter from the other classmates. Kals family had
recently immigrated to the United States from the Philippines. They're imprinted in my memory, and it's been hard to fully forget about.
I mean, they don't have the same, obviously, the same sting now or anything like that.
I look at them more as a kind of curiosity in a way of thinking about what might be
experience be like for other people.
Much has changed in the US in the years since Carl immigrated here.
Many schools now go out of their way to welcome students of diverse backgrounds and to embrace
cultural differences.
Anti-bullying programs are widespread.
As a culture, while we're still far
from perfect, we've gotten better at caring for the disadvantaged. People who experience hardships
are encouraged to ask for help. Carl eventually became a psychologist. He started to notice how
regularly students came to him with accounts of various limitations.
I will get requests from students
for accommodations for certain things
because they're experiencing anxiety or depression
or some other kind of psychological challenge.
Karl was usually sympathetic,
but he was also struck by how times had changed
from when he was a student.
I remember when I was younger, I would also be
beset by by these anxieties.
I would worry about tests and I just didn't didn't go to my
teacher and talk about it.
So really that that was for me a bit of an eye opener that
there's an increasing willingness to accommodate or be
responsive to different claims that a student might make about hardship or challenge in life.
I think it's actually a reflection of the fact that human beings appear to be increasingly concerned about alleviating each other's suffering.
Partly as a result of these experiences and his own childhood experiences of feeling
like an outsider, Carl got curious about the ways people ask for help.
He wrote an article about this idea.
It's a very simple idea.
You know, people want to help.
How do they know that you need help?
Well, you have to tell them somehow, all right?
By signaling that one is a victim, one is able to potentially get others,
I'll call them bed of factors to provide that person with resources.
Carl hypothesized that people who signal that their legitimate victims of harm would
indeed attract help, especially if they also communicated that their legitimate victims of harm would indeed attract help,
especially if they also communicated that they were worthy of help.
If you want to maximize the effectiveness of this signal of your need,
it helps to combine it with being virtuous, because it makes people more willing to help us.
You're going more likely to help someone who you think has virtue than someone who you think is,
you know, a moral reprobator to generate, right?
I mean, nobody wants to help that other person.
And so it's very obvious that the maximal effectiveness of any kind of signal of need is to combine need being a victim with virtue.
That's the basic idea.
That's the basic idea.
At this point, Karl was not thinking about people who fake hardships. He was just hypothesizing that virtuous victims were more likely to elicit help.
As a way to empirically test his hypotheses, Karl devised a scale to identify people who
tell others about their challenges and traumas.
The scale enrolled a number of questions.
And we ended up with a set of 10 items, 10 questions that reflect different ways that
people might signal their hardship or disadvantage.
I'll just read you a few here.
Discussed how my concerns and needs are not being heard
by political leaders.
Shared how I don't feel comfortable with my body.
Discussed how I don't feel financially secure.
Told others that I get paid less based on my identity.
Explained how I don't feel accepted
in the society because of my identity.
I spoke about how people who share my identity are criminalized by society,
expressed how people like me are underrepresented in the media and
leadership, made it known that I can't move freely within or outside of my
country. Once he had this questionnaire in hand,
Kyle was able to identify people who answered yes
to one or more of these questions.
He now started to look for clues about the motivations and traits of these people.
Okay, so the next part was to see whether there are certain personality characteristics
that might predict whether a person is likely to signal victimhood.
And I just started playing around with the data.
It's really just, um, happenstance.
I, um, I was trying to validate the scale in a sample,
larger sample, um, of students,
and included in that data collection effort,
where other scales that my colleagues had thrown in.
Carl looked to see if the people who self-identified themselves as victims
were also likely to show up on other scales and measures.
To his surprise, he found a connection with another scale
that measured something completely different.
And all I did is I just, I mean, I basically looked at the correlation.
It's just a very simple analysis out of curiosity.
And I found out that the scale that we were developing
was correlated with another scale,
which measures what's called the dark triad of personality.
The so-called dark triad consists of three personality traits
you've probably heard about.
None of them is very nice.
Machiavellianism, basically, whether you're willing to immorally manipulate others to get what you want.
Narcissism, probably most people know, but that's sort of a grandiose belief about yourself.
You think you're superior to others.
And then, subclinical psychopathy, which is not the psychopathy that you associate with a serial killer,
but really more of a kind of, I would call it emotional shalonist or lack of remorse when you do bad things.
And that was just, I had no idea that would happen to solve the correlation.
And there it was, it popped out and I started thinking, hmm, that's curious.
It popped out and I started thinking, hmm, that's curious. What might that show?
And it struck me that I wonder if people who are high in these traits are more likely
to use these signals for personal gain.
Karl had found a correlation, but that does not imply causation.
So he and his colleagues ran a number
of experiments to test if there was a real connection. We know from the literature on the dark
personality that they are more likely to do things like lie or cheat to try to get what they want.
And that's the third part of the paper was to show that the dark personality does predict
was to show that the dark personality does predict a willingness to signal victimhood and virtue.
In one of his experiments,
call examined whether people were willing to paint
a coworker in a bad light
in order to make themselves look better.
So in this task, we had people imagine
that they were working together with another intern
and there was a kind of competitive situation
they're competing for a job.
After giving the volunteers a description of what the other intern had done, the volunteers
were given an opportunity to offer a performance evaluation of the other person.
They could stick to the facts or falsely say the intern had behaved poorly.
Then we gave descriptions of things that that intern might have done, such as they don't
respect your suggestions.
You think they might be saying things behind your back.
We found that people who were higher in the virtuous victim signaling scales were more willing
to report these behaviors that didn't infert, in fact, stated as having occurred.
These fictitious behaviors, another one.
This is just behavior, yeah.
And we reason that because, you know,
that you put them in this kind of competitive situation,
that it's to their benefit to cast their rival
in a less positive light.
I want to stop here to underline something very important.
Carl is not saying that everyone who talks about their hardships and limitations is a manipulator
and narcissist.
Indeed many, perhaps most of the people who tell you they are experiencing limitations,
are doing so sincerely.
But what Karl is saying is that some subset of the people who claim to be victims are
likely pretenders, and these pretenders may be motivated by a desire to manipulate the empathy of potential
benefactors.
It's just another reminder that there are going to be times when one might be moved by
sentiments to help another person.
And that person might not be the kind of person you'd want to help
because they might exploit you.
So, call these behaviors are intersecting at this moment in our cultural life with the fact that
we are living in a society that is increasingly interested, or at least many people say they are increasingly interested,
in being an acting compassionate. So in other words, there's a supply of compassion as well as
a demand for it. And in some ways what your paper is talking about is the intersection of these
two worlds where there's a supply of compassion. There are people who genuinely are in need of compassion, but there are also people who are saying, given that there's
a supply of compassion, how can I get some of it, even if I don't deserve it? Yeah, that's a very
good analysis. If you look at it in terms of a market, you want to reach the equilibrium where
you're actually, you're legitimately allocating resources
to people in need.
As opposed to allocating resources to people who don't really need it, but are being able
to exploit this what you call surplus of compassion.
One of the inspirations for the paper was I read an article in a Canadian newspaper
about a woman who posted on, go find me that she had cancer.
Family and friends rallied around the woman in a raffle was held to collect money, but
police are now alleging the entire campaign was a scam and she didn't really have cancer.
But I think you know when people are because people are very motivated to try to alleviate
suffering, it can be sometimes easy for those who might have
the dark-tried personality, let's say,
to use that for their own benefit.
Do you even want to extremes to shave her head
and cut her skin and call them cancer bumps?
Take time off work for these fake surgeries
that didn't actually happen.
So it was kind of extreme measure.
If I see a GoFundMe page of someone who basically says they have cancer, and I want to help them,
but it turns out that they're merely exploiting me, that in some ways prevents me from actually helping the person who actually needs help.
The person who actually has cancer and might not have the resources to deal with it.
Exactly. So it's about a, you're diverting resources, attention, concern,
away from people in legitimate need, to those who
might be feigning whatever experience they're claiming they have.
And again, we can say nothing in this paper about whether these people are not legitimately
experiencing these things, but the inference that you're drawing is certainly, I think,
one of the implications.
Now, there's sort of a flip side of this, which is, let's say, for example, now I'm alerted
to the fact that there are some unscrupulous people, or perhaps people who have, you know,
dark personality traits, who are likely, or
who are more likely to set up a GoFundMe page saying that they have cancer.
And so now I have my skepticism up.
I have my radar up to try and spot these cases.
But of course, the truth is I can't really look at a GoFundMe page and tell very much about
whether this is the legitimate case or not.
Is it possible that this can inadvertently reduce
my compassion overall?
Because now I'm actually saying I'm skeptical
of the people who are coming to me for help,
because I'm aware that some portion of them
might be counterfeit, or some portion of them
might be pretenders.
Is there a risk in some ways that this can,
in some ways, reduce the supply of compassion in general?
Yeah, I think certainly there is potentially that consequence.
And I think this is one of the implications of the proliferation of victim
signals is that because it's difficult to know exactly which ones are true or
which ones are false, the more there are, the more likely some of them
might be false, just in terms of sure probabilities.
And those false ones will often be more prominent in people's
minds.
And yes, they will be then perhaps potentially more
vigilant in the future, or more skeptical,
or less willing to indiscriminately offer their assistance in aid.
I mean, I think that is one of the potential social welfare consequences of the increased
signaling of victimhood. When we were children, many of us heard the story of the boy who cried wolf.
He was a solitary shepherd with no one to talk to all day.
So one day, he thought of a plan to amuse himself.
He would call out that there was a wolf and watch the people in his village get riled
up.
At first, this is exactly what happened.
In the fable, the villagers heard the boys' shouts and came running to help.
When they saw there was no wolf, they chastised the boy for lying to them.
But far from learning his lesson, he tricked them a second time.
The villagers realised they could not trust the boy.
The next time he cried out for help,
there really was a wolf. But by now, the villagers were tired of this game, and no one came to the boys' aid.
As human beings, all of us depend upon the help of others. It can be tempting at times to exaggerate our suffering or to invent hardships in order
to elicit, compassion and aid. At such times, it's worth remembering that there is a steep
price to be paid for crying wolf. That price isn't just paid by benefactors who are
tricked into offering help when it isn't really needed. The burden falls disproportionately on the people who really do need help the most.
When they discover, there is no compassion left for them. Hidden Brain is produced by Hidden Brain Media.
Our audio production team includes Bridget McCarthy, Annie Murphy-Paul, Kristen Wong, Laura
Quarelle, Ryan Katz, Autumn Barnes, and Andrew Chadwick.
Tara Boyle is our executive producer.
I'm Hidden Brain's Executive Editor.
We're always on the lookout for interesting stories that reveal the complexities of human nature. Have you ever done anything that makes you look back and say,
why in the world did I do that?
If you have a powerful personal story you are willing to share with a hidden brain audience,
please record a short voice memo on your phone and email it to us at ideas at hiddenbrain.org.
Again, if you have a powerful personal story about actions you took and perhaps regret
with the benefit of hindsight, record a two or three minute voice memo outlining the story
and email it to us at ideasathydnbrain.org. Please use the subject line, misgivings.
We end the show today with a story from our sister's show, My Unsung Hero. It comes from Heather Church.
show My Unsung Hero. It comes from Heather Church. Heather will never forget the day, many years ago, when she walked into a busy clothing store in New York City.
When I went in there, I went down to the lower level, taking the stairs and wearing a very long skirt and a pair of boots. My skirt got caught on my boot. I had threw off my balance.
I felt my hand reach for the railing and miss it and my body twisted as my boot tugged on my skirt.
on my skirt. And suddenly I was falling down the stairs backwards. And then I heard steps, stumps, in fact, moving quickly towards me. And someone caught me. My head didn't hit those hard stairs, and I didn't suffer any injuries.
And I don't know who that person is.
That is my unsung hero.
I don't know who they were, and I was so flustered at the time.
I can't even remember their face, but I do remember what they did for me that day, and
I remain grateful.
Thank you.
Listener Heather Church from New York.
She tells us that she hopes that stranger at the store might hear this episode and understand how much their actions continue to resonate with her all these years later.
If you like this episode and would like us to produce more shows like this,
please consider supporting our work.
Go to support.hiddenbrain.org. Again, if you find our work to be
useful in your life, do your part to help us thrive. Go to support.hiddenbrain.org.
I'm Shankar Vedantam. See you soon.
you