Hidden Brain - Encore of Episode 2: Near Victories
Episode Date: February 2, 2016Shankar Vedantam explores "almosts" and "not quites" on this episode of the Hidden Brain podcast, with the help of Monica Wadhwa, Dan Pink, and country music singer Kacey Musgraves....
Transcript
Discussion (0)
This is the Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedantam. In this episode, we're going to talk about
near misses, almost and not quite. First, we're going to talk about research that shows
that coming close to winning something can really affect your drive and motivation.
You want to build that thing in, that, hey, you're almost there.
Then my friend Daniel Pink will join us for a round of stop-watch signs.
He's going to explain how the psychology of getting close to a goal can help charities
raise more money.
Why do people give?
People give not because they want to be altruistic, but because they want to be effective.
Finally, we last sing our songwriter, Casey Mus Musgraves to put on an imaginary lab coat and join
us for a game of mad scientist.
This is my lab coat.
Have rhinestones on it?
We lost her to explain a song from her album Pageant Material and we learned what world
piece has to do with swimsuits.
Okay, near Mrs. Monica Wadwa is a professor at the NCAD Business School in Singapore.
She studies marketing and recently in her research, she hit upon a strange but promising
way to change how consumers behave.
It's funny because this came from one of my childhood experiences actually.
To understand this idea, let's travel back in time to Monica's childhood.
I was born in Bratapin, India, in Delhi.
Like many 13-year-olds, Monica had a craze.
Now her craze was somewhat unusual.
It was buying lottery tickets.
As a child, I kind of wanted to have tons of money, right?
So, and I was lazy.
So, lottery tickets was my answer to good life.
She played the lottery constantly. And remember those days we could buy lottery tickets.
They were used to be stalls.
You could just go get a lottery ticket for,
I remember paying ten rupees for it at that time.
And you buy this lottery ticket and if you get like a certain set of numbers,
if you get all of that correct, you could get a jackpot.
How many digits did it have?
I think it was six.
So you have to get the lottery sort of requires you to
match all six digits to win the prize.
Yes.
There was a problem, though.
Her dad worried that her passion for the lottery was
distracting her from her schoolwork.
A big exam was coming up, and he needed her to focus.
Not to mention, her habit was casting him a ton of money, but Monica kept buying lottery
tickets until one day.
Then I insisted on getting more, he's like no more, that's it. You know, I don't know
about you becoming a millionaire, I'm definitely going to get bankrupt.
So they worked out a deal. Her dad would allow her to buy one last lottery ticket before the big exam, which she did.
And then she waited for the results.
The results would come in a particular newspaper.
So describe to me exactly what happened when that paper arrived that morning.
Do you remember anything about the day at all?
I don't remember the exact thing.
I just remember the emotions.
You know, I remember, I was supremely excited.
You know, you're like kind of a truancy. Did I win it? Did I get it? In looking at the first
yes it matched, second yes it matched, third yes it matched. And I'm just there. I am going
to be a millionaire in just about one second, right? Foot one matched. And then the fifth. Dammit, it did not.
She lost.
I got my 10 rupees back.
But the thing is, in that moment,
13-year-old Monica's emotions
were not what you might expect.
She was actually happy.
I think I was feeling excited
of an after.
I was like, oh, hey, I almost got it.
And so your reaction when you got 4 out of 6, you were not dejected, you were not, oh my god, that was so close, I could have been so rich, and now I just get my 10 rupees back.
It was kind of mixed feeling, you know, it was like, yes, there was a little bit of dejection, but maybe it's just me, but there was a lot more excitement.
I was an almost winner.
You know, I was almost there.
I got it.
How many people actually get almost there?
And this is where she surprised her dad and maybe even herself.
She didn't slack off before the exam.
I actually fucked Hada.
Really, I remember I was very excited for the exam because I's best recollection of her performance
in the exam. Monica realized later that after she had nearly won the lottery, she had a
strong left over urge to succeed, to do well at something, anything.
Just the desire to get something.
And that's something happened to be the exam that it last on to, I guess.
Fast forward a couple decades, and this childhood experience has now become the focus of Monica's academic research and marketing.
When you get closer to a reward, your motivation intensifies as you approach the reward.
But when you miss it, you have this intensified motivational state, which is not satisfied now.
And our theory was that this intensified, but not satisfied motivation will spill over and will make you seek other unrelated rewards now.
She and her colleagues recently tested this theory in a number of ways.
In one experiment, they recruited people to play a video game. We designed a game where they had to uncover some
tiles and if they uncovered eighth diamonds they could win a reward. By design
some people clearly won the game, some clearly lost and then others. They missed
the reward just by one diamond. Monica wanted to know about that last group's
almost win. Would that motivate them to seek another reward,
so we kind of secretly measured how much time, for example,
it takes them to reach another reward,
like a chocolate bar, which they could collect from another booth.
People who nearly one, they walked much faster to get that chocolate.
They were much more eager to get the second unrelated reward
as compared to those who won or those who clearly lost.
You actually had a chocolate bar at another station and you actually measured how fast people
took to walk to that second station. Yes.
And your theory is that people were walking faster in some ways because they had nearly
missed the first reward and that put them in a state where they basically said my desire for the next reward is even higher.
Right.
Turns out the video game's new winners also salivated more when they stared at money, which
I had to ask about.
What's the connection between salivation and watching money?
You're not going to eat the money.
That is to event to saliv celebration has been shown to be a measure
of desire. So when you desire something you celebrate more.
In a second experiment the researchers gave people lottery tickets outside a shopping mall.
Again the idea was that some people would win, some people would lose and some would be
near winners. The researchers then measured how much the near winners spent once they
entered the mall. They spent a lot more than those who clearly near winners. The researchers then measured how much the near winners spent once they entered
them all. They spent a lot more than those who clearly lost or those who won. And this
was like a really cool thing because from a marketing perspective, marketers think that
winning will actually make them spend more, but this was just the other way around.
Monica says the experience of nearly winning something can motivate people of all ages.
I'm wondering if it has implications for things like parenting as well and management of people in a
workforce. I mean, so if you're trying to get your kid to practice the piano, for example, in some
ways, sort of not getting the sticker from the teacher every week might actually be better than
reliably getting the sticker every single week. Right, absolutely. If you get it each day every day,
you are totally sissy at it.
You want to build that thing in that,
hey, you're almost there.
You're just there, but not quite there.
That's Monica Wadwa, professor of marketing
at the NCAD Business School in Singapore.
After this break, we're going to be joined by my friend Daniel Pink for another round
of Stop-Wat Science.
Back now for another round of Stop-Wat Science, I'm Shankar Vidantanthan.
And I'm Daniel Pink.
Dan is our senior Stopwatch science correspondent.
He's also the author of several books about human behavior.
On Stopwatch Science, Dan and I give one another 60 seconds to summarize interesting social
science research.
I'll run a stopwatch as Dan speaks, and here's the buzzer he's going to hear if he exceeds
his time.
That's not the sound of a dinner gong, Dan. That's
a sound that says you need to stop. That's like a Chuck Baris gong show gong. You're off
to stage. What's the sound of my buzzer, Dan? You're going to hear this sound right here.
A little bit of a loony tune sounds appropriate for this segment. All right. That is a profoundly
irritating sound, and I'm going to try and make sure I don't get to hear it. Our topic today is about Neil Wins. As you just heard, Monica Wadba found that getting close to a
win can boost your motivation and drive. Dan and I are going to look at several other factors right now. Dan, if
you're ready, your first 60 seconds starts now. Well, those of us who've taken Psychology 101, probably remember the
famous study that showed when a rat gets closer to food, the rat moves faster.
Okay, so this is a study from the Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology
asking the important question,
what if someone else is the rat and something else is the food?
So what they did is they looked at charities.
When do we make gifts to charity?
And they looked at donations to Kiva,va the online charity and they found not surprisingly
that as people got closer to their stated goal
giving went dramatically up then they also did a study where they simulated a
kid named shea
and they said shea has
to candy bars to sell to hit her quota verses thirty to candy bars to sell
to hit a quota not surprisingly they gave more sell to hit a quota. Not surprisingly,
they gave more money when Sheila was only two away. And this goes to really why do people
give to charity? Some of it's to be altruistic and relieve negative emotions, but it's also
so they can feel effective. And that's what near wins do.
All right, great. I hope my gong was effective. It certainly got down to stop speaking. I think
this is fascinating though, Dan, because of course we all feel that as we get closer
to the goal, it's actually attainable. Right. What I think was interesting about this is that
someone else was actually pursuing the goal. What you were doing is you were giving a little bit of a
lift and you're more likely to give that person some assistance when that person is nearing the goal.
Yeah, and public radio stations do this all the time instead of saying our goal is two million
dollars. They say our goal for this segment is sixty thousand dollars because it makes people feel that they can actually get to that goal that's more attainable well our goal for this segment is to see if
Sean car can do his study in sixty seconds and his time starts right now all right so if you want to know how to motivate people it's very important to ask where they are in the process of reaching their goal are they just starting out or are they nearly on their way to the goal? Are they nearly at the win? Very much like you're just
saying that. CG Wong at the University of Texas at Austin recently published a paper
in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology that found that where you are in the process
makes a big difference in your motivation. When we are in the early stages of a challenge,
it's important to have multiple options. If you hear there are six different ways you can give money to a public radio station, you're more likely
to stay engaged in the pledge drive. On the other hand, as you get close to the win, having
more options turns into a hindrance. In one study, as a blood donation drive started to
approach its final stages, people were more likely to be willing to donate blood when
they were given a single way to do so, rather than given multiple ways. So in other words, at the early stages of a quest, having lots of options gives us a reason
not to lose hope as you approach the win, having lots of options becomes a distraction.
Oh, you were out of options, but you came in right on time. That's interesting. We can think of
it as kind of a motivational funnel. You start wide, then you get closer and closer, you go narrow.
I love the funnel idea, Dan has reached the narrow end of his funnel,
because his next 60 seconds starts right now.
Okay, so we've talked a lot about the positive elements of near wins.
Let's talk about the dark side of near wins,
and this is a study from the journal Neuron,
using two of my all-time favorite technologies,
FMRIs, functional magnetic resonance images, and slot machines.
And here's what they did.
They put a bunch of participants in these FMRIs and had them play slot machines,
but the slot machines, of course, were rigged, as all slot machines are, I guess.
Actually.
Sometimes the participants won, and a certain region of their brain was activated, but then
they had them lose, just by a hair.
And you know what what the exact same region
was activated what so what happened is when they won their brain said let's play again
and then when they lost they said let's play again so this is the dark side of of near
winds there almost have an addictive quality which is why you should stay at a vagus
unless you bring your fm r i i love the study then because actually it measures so nicely
with what monica wadwa was talking about,
which is the effect of coming very close to a win is actually very similar in the brain to actually winning.
Yes, and you're very close to winning here, because you have one minute for your next study and it starts now.
Alright, so this is one of my favorite studies on the science of motivation.
I talked about it with Steven Sk keep on morning edition some months ago max
Austin Elliott the University of Wisconsin-Malwaki along with David Luna and
Torsten Ringberg they recently analyzed how movement can affect motivation
and they find that telling or showing people they are moving upward or
downward can impair or increase their driving motivation in one experiment the
researchers asked people to imagine
riding up 20 floors in an elevator
or riding down 20 floors in an elevator.
The volunteers had then given math puzzles to solve
in three minutes.
And what the researchers find is that the volunteers
who imagine going down,
solved more math problems
than volunteers who imagine going upward.
And the difference wasn't trivial,
it was a 30% difference. The researchers think this is happening because
as you move upward, your self-esteem gets boosted and when you feel better
about yourself, you say, eh, who cares? Why don't you solve math problems? I feel
pretty good already. So this study suggests, Dan, that the next time you and I
sit down to record stopwatch science, instead of sitting on the fourth floor of
NPR, we're going to go to the basement. I'm in, I'm in, absolutely, absolutely fascinating and of course neither one of us has an
overconfidence problem so that should be fair.
Absolutely, I'm sure it doesn't affect us whatsoever but I'm hearing our producers say
they're going to build a new basement in the studio.
Alright, this has been another round to stop what science, I'm Shankar Vidantantham.
And I'm Daniel Pink.
After this break I have a special guest joining me in the studio, country music singer Casey Musgraves. Stay with us.
Now we're going to introduce you to a new segment that we're calling Mad Scientist.
Our first victim, country music singer, Kasey Musgraves.
Kasey, thanks for joining me on The Hidden Brain.
Thank you.
Kasey was just upstairs at NPR to perform a tiny desk concert.
A new album is called Pageant Material.
I'm pageant material.
I'm always higher than my hair.
And it ain't that I don't care about world peace
But I don't see how I can fix it in a swimsuit on stage
As I listen to the album Casey, I told myself there is no possible way
anyone has done social science research that looks at the connection between world peace
and wearing a swimsuit on a stage
and wouldn't you know it, I was wrong.
So it turns out I came by work out of Arizona State University that looks at how beauty
pageant winners go on to win political office.
So between 2000 and 2010, something like 12% of all female governors elected in the United
States were former beauty pageant contestants.
And among them of course, we have Sarah Pailin, former governor of Alaska, beauty pageant
contestant.
If John McCain had been elected president, she would be vice president today in all
likelihood.
She might have been the front runner for president in 2016.
And in 2017, she might have had her finger on the big red nuclear button.
Who knows?
So, you tell me, Casey, can wearing a swimsuit on a stage have anything to do with the
world peace?
Maybe it's to do with the brain in the swimsuit. I don't know
Where did the idea for pageant material come from?
so
Pageant material was written with two friends of mine Luke layered and Shane McNelly and
Shane is from the southeast from Texas as am I. So we both kind of grew up in this
area where that was a total normal thing, you know. That was a thing girls really wanted to do and
he had this title. He said, I really want to write a song called Pageant Material and I just thought
I can totally get behind that. I mean it's just like a light-hearted song, just kind of poking fun
at myself for not being, you know, maybe up to like industry
pageant standards and sometimes in the music industry, there's expectations for women
to maybe act a certain way and I've found myself in certain situations where I don't come
up to par with some of those things.
And so when that happens, do you feel critical about yourself or do you say look these as
silly social norms, I'm glad I'm actually not following them.
The latter, for sure. I mean, you know, I think there's always going to be people that
are looking to find something wrong with what you're doing or, you know, picking them apart,
but I think it's always best just to be yourself. And if that means, you know, you're not smiling
at a certain point or this or that, you know, it's if it's what you're feeling, you know,
just a certain extent, I think you should do it.
Alright, so that's a great segue into our game.
And so I'm gonna tell you how we play mad scientist.
Social scientists are constantly coming up
with crazy studies and experiments.
So on mad scientist, I'm gonna tell you about
a piece of social science research
and you put on your imaginary lab coat
and try and deduce what the research experiment discovered.
Does that sound like a plan?
Does my lab coat have rhinestones on it?
Yeah.
It does.
Since it's fictitious, it does.
Okay.
I think you get to pick the kind of lab coat you want.
Great.
So yours definitely has rhinestones.
Perfect.
So when listening to your title song, Pageant Material, I was struck by a line in the song
that talks about how you feel about being run or up.
God bless the girls who smile and hug when they're called out as you runnin' up on TV.
I wish I could, but I just came.
Where it's my own, it's my only, but I'm feeling.
Alright, so here's the experiment.
Many years ago, Thomas Gilevich, who is a psychologist at Cornell University,
he and his colleagues collected videotape of all the silver and bronze metal winners
at the 1992 Barcelona Olympics.
All right, so the video clips showed the immediate reactions, the first reactions when athletes
discovered they had won either silver or bronze.
Now here's the question and I want you to think it through aloud.
We all know that winning silver is better than winning bronze, coming in second is better
than coming in third.
When Gillovich and his colleagues
Examine the videos. Do you think they found that the silver medal winners looked happier than the bronze medal winners?
Or did they find that the bronze medal winners looked happier than the silver medal winners?
Can I ask you questions you certainly can where's the gold winner?
The gold winner is off jumping
gold winner in this. The gold winner is off jumping exuberantly. So he's already been announced. Yes, he or she has been announced and you know he's jumping up
and down. But this experiment is only looking at silver and bronze metal
winners. Well, I can't help but feel like this is a massive trick. No, but I would say obviously silver because it's a higher rank, but maybe even knowing that
you got bronze and you're not totally out.
Oh man, this is a hard one.
Alright, so I'm going to give you a clue.
The clue has to do with who you're comparing yourself against, that our happiness levels
and our satisfaction with what we get in life is often not just tied to what we have,
but it's often tied in comparison to what other people have.
So who does a silver medal winner compare himself for herself to and who does a bronze medal compare himself for herself to and that's the clue to the answer.
Oh, yeah. So maybe there's a bigger letdown with silver comparing themselves to the ultimate winner.
But there's more joy in a bronze winner
knowing that they're not a loser at all.
So that's your answer?
Yes.
Casey Mustraves, you are correct.
Yes.
You have one match I do.
Can I keep my lab code?
I think you have to return the lab code.
NPR holds on to all the rhinestone lab codes.
I'm afraid it's just company policy. I'm wondering if this actually tells you anything about your own life.
The idea that when you compare yourself, who you're comparing yourself to actually determines
how happy you're going to be, and it makes no sense that bronze medal winners would compare
themselves against people who were the total losers and therefore felt happy. And the silver
medal winners compared themselves to the gold and they felt unhappy, but that's exactly
what Gilo Vich and his colleagues found.
So crazy. I mean, I can totally see that. I heard a quote recently and I think it was, I'm going to get this wrong, but comparison is the ultimate thief of joy.
Yes.
Something like that.
Because there are always going to be people who have more than you. And they'll always be people who have less than you.
And so you'd constantly go through life comparing ourselves to all these people and then feeling these silly aspects of happiness and
Yes, so avoid when it's like if you could just look at yourself instead you might come away way more happy.
Wonderful. That's Casey Musgraves. Her new album is called Pageant Material.
Casey, thank you for playing Mad Scientist with me on the Hidden Brain.
Thanks for the challenge.
To hear more Casey Musgraves, check out her tiny desk concert at NPR Music.
For more Hidden Brain, join us on Facebook and Twitter and listen to my NPR stories on
your local public radio station.
The Hidden Brain podcast is produced by Cara McGurk-Allison and Maggie Pennman, special thanks this
week to Chris Benderrev, Bob Boylin and Jacob Gans.
I'm Shankar Vedantum and this is NPR.
and Jacob Gans. I'm Shankar Vidantum and this is NPR.