Hidden Brain - Hiding Behind Free Speech
Episode Date: September 5, 2017Several weeks ago, white supremacists took to the streets of Charlottesville, Virginia, in a demonstration that left many Americans asking a lot of questions. Who are we as a nation? What do we stand ...for, and what do we tolerate? The United States goes further than many other countries to protect speech — even hate-filled speech like that used in Charlottesville. In this episode, we look at how people use free speech arguments, and why the motivations behind these arguments may not be apparent — even to the people making them.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Before we start today, this episode is about race and free speech.
It includes sounds from the violence and Charlottesville, as well as a racial epithet.
If you have small kids with you, please save this one for later.
This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedantin. A few weeks ago, the words
Charlottesville and Tiki torches weren't national symbols you'd associate with white supremacy.
Today, those words convey a very specific scene.
White lives men! White lives men!
We begin with the deadly chaos on the streets of Charlottesville, Virginia.
Yesterday's dueling rallies turned deadly when a car plowed into a group of those who opposed the alt-right.
It was a weekend of street battles and stark displays of racism.
a weekend of street battles and stark displays of racism.
Photos of enraged white men using the Nazi salute and marching with torches shocked many Americans. And since then, many of us have been doing a lot of soul searching. Who are we?
What do we stand for? And as a nation, what do we tolerate?
Today, we want to zoom in on a specific issue that grows out of the events in Charlottesville.
The Constitution upholds the rights of Americans to say almost anything,
no matter how distasteful, without censure from legislatures, the police, or the courts.
Our protections for speech, even hate-filled, vitriolic speech go further than most nations.
In recent weeks, many people have made free speech arguments to defend the white supremacists
who descended on Charlottesville.
Is hate speech protected under the Constitution in a word?
Yes.
Free speech may give them the right to do this, but are they not familiar with the First Amendment?
There's a fine line between free speech and hate speech.
But hold on, we can't have that conversation or any conversation until we both agree that Are they not familiar with the first amendment? There's a fine line between free speech and hate speech.
But hold on, we can't have that conversation or any conversation until we both agree that we have the right to say what we believe.
And that we're not going to be punished for doing that right.
Are these arguments motivated by principle or something less heroic?
New psychological research suggests that people's motivations might not be what they seem.
And that's true, no matter where you fall on the political spectrum.
People pull out free speech as a defense when they're defending racist speech, but not
when they're defending simply aggressive or negative speech.
Today on Hidden Brain, what we're really saying when we say we defend free speech, and how the words we use and the things we say
shape the culture in which we live.
Chris Krandel is a professor of psychology at the University of Kansas.
He studies prejudice and has looked at how we use free speech claims.
We spoke before the violence in Charlottesville, but we started by talking about an incident
that echoes what happened there.
In 2015, members of the Sigma Alpha Epsilon fraternity at the University of Oklahoma were caught
on video singing a racist song.
They were singing, there will never be a n-word in SAE and they were singing happily and clapping along about
the exclusion of African Americans from the fraternity.
The video of the song was put on Facebook and it spread around the campus and the two song leaders of the fraternity
who were captured on video were expelled from the university and the fraternity itself was
shut down on the campus of the University of Oklahoma and there was really a media firestorm
about there singing the song. In the aftermath of the incident one of your graduate students noticed
something unusual about the responses to the incident. What did he notice?
Yeah, Mark White was following this on the internet and he looked at the
tenor of the comments and people were saying, well this is simply free speech.
These people have a right to say these things because Americans have the right to
free speech. But underneath the surface, in the background, or sometimes right at the forefront, it really
looked like what people were doing was justifying the content of what was said, not the fact
that they have the right to say something freely, but rather that they seem to be justifying
the racist speech itself by giving an account,
hey, free speech, that allowed them to do that
without punishment.
Now, it's worth pointing out, of course,
that the First Amendment protects people
against government intrusions on free speech.
It doesn't actually prevent private companies or universities
from deciding what is or isn't acceptable speech
in the workplace or on a college campus.
But setting aside that distinction, you conducted an experiment where you had volunteers
listen to racially charged commentary and you evaluated whether they would reach for
free speech arguments to defend it.
You evaluated whether people who endorse free speech arguments do so consistently or they
do so selectively.
What walked me through the experiment?
Yeah, so in one version of the experiment,
we did several of these.
In one version of the experiment,
people read about somebody who wrote something
on Facebook that was a deeply racist.
For example, a barista wrote on Facebook
that the black customers were problematic,
that they were noisy and rude
and other racially stereotypic actions.
And the people in the study read that the person had been fired for posting this racist
speech.
In the other condition, people read about a guy complaining about customers, but no racial
information was given.
So the same boisterous, aggressive, loud, cutting-in line behavior was described,
but the racial element was eradicated.
And so the question was, do racial attitudes of our participants determine how much they're going to defend the speaker?
In the racialized condition, the more you had negative attitudes towards
African Americans, the stronger you endorsed free speech as a justification for why the
person should have been able to say that without being fired. So that suggests that racial
attitudes might be behind free speech defense. But you might say quite easily, that's simply a correlation.
I'm not impressed.
Maybe people who have negative racial attitudes
are also libertarians,
and they believe that free speech is super important
and they're just simply expressing that.
The problem with that argument is when you remove
the racial content from the story,
so in the condition whether the guy complains about customers, but there's no racial element,
racial attitudes correlate zero with free speech defense.
It seems that people pull them out and deploy them when they're appropriate.
So people pull out free speech as a defense when they're defending racist speech, but not
when they're defending simply aggressive or negative speech.
I understand that one of the ways in which you measured racial attitudes was simply to
ask people what are your attitudes to what people from different races.
Yeah, people are a little careful in how they speak about racial attitudes, but our participants
were willing to say that they thought that blacks were pushing too hard for their equal
rights and that blacks were getting more than they deserved.
And in some cases, we simply had them rate African-Americans under a zero to 100 scale.
And the lower the number you gave, the more you used the free speech defense in defense of racist speech.
One of the elements of the study that you conducted asked people about their attitudes about police officers.
I find it's not correctly you were measuring controversial speech as it was directed towards a minority group versus
controversial speech directed against police officers.
Yeah, in the police version, we were interested in not only attitudes towards African-American,
so somebody said something racist, but we also thought that police might be the exact opposite. That is, people who
might be very anti-African-American might be particularly pro-police. And certainly in
history of the Midwest here in St. Louis and in Missouri just across the border, there
have been a lot of police African-American interactions that have not been very happy.
So we thought if we used the police, maybe you'd get the opposite
effect. And we did, it wasn't as strong, but we did find that the more a person had negative
attitudes towards African Americans, the less they defended anti-police speech. Although
in the other condition, the more they defended the free speech of racist commenters.
So in other words, if the speech is basically anti-black, someone with high racial animus might say
that speech is permissible and the First Amendment free speech grounds, but if the speech is
anti-police, the person might say, I don't really agree with this person expressing these kinds of
views, so the person is inconsistent in their defense of free speech.
That's right. This is what we find is that people are really inconsistent.
You also found in the experiment, Chris, that it wasn't just people with racial biases who were sometimes hypocritical when it came to free speech issues.
You also found that people who were low in racial bias were also hypocritical just in the opposite direction. Yes, people who were low in racial prejudice were just as inconsistent in applying the
first amendment as a defense than people who were high in racial prejudice. When they were asked
to use the first amendment to defend racist speech, they actually reduced their willingness to do it.
They would defend the free speech
of somebody who was complaining about their customers or police at an average level. But
when it became racialized, the low racist people actually walked away from the First Amendment
defense. They went substantially lower. This suggests that when people are using the free speech defense,
they know what they're doing because the high racists use it in defense of racist speech
and the low racists drop it like a hot potato and say, no, no, no, no, no, we're not defending
the speech with First Amendment. But they would defend a barista who's complaining about his or her customers or the police.
When you found people defending the racist speech on First Amendment grounds, on freedom of expression
grounds, is this because people may have at some level felt bad about themselves? Chris, they felt
bad that the person who has views similar to their own is being punished in some way and they're
trying to defend that person as a way to defend themselves.
That's exactly what we thought was going on and when we started showing these studies, everybody would nod when we say, and so maybe they're defending themselves.
But we did several studies where we tried to find out if people's self-esteem was attacked by this, if they needed help to feel good about themselves.
And we could never find evidence that people were defending themselves.
What we found evidence for was that people were defending their right to say things.
They were defending the speech so that their future speech would be protective.
They wanted to create a world where this kind of prejudicial speech was acceptable for them to say, for others to say in the future.
How would you respond to people who might say, you know, let's look under the hood of Chris Crandell's brain. Isn't it possible he's just a lefty academic who's coming up with research
findings that endorse his own pre-existing views of how the world works?
I think that some of that criticism would probably land. I am sort of a lefty
academic. I'm a little bit more centrist, I suppose, but we were interested in
studying prejudice because prejudice is a particular social problem. It shows up all around the country, it affects people's lives, and so we studied that.
But people would be inconsistent about, for example, being environmentalist and failing
to recycle or going too fast in the right-hand lane of a highway.
People are inconsistent all the time, and they manage to get over that pretty easily,
mostly by being unaware of it or not
paying attention to it too much.
When we come back, we look at the effect
of free speech arguments on our behavior.
I'm also going to ask Chris about how the language we use
and the things we say can reshape our culture,
especially when the person doing the talking is the president.
I could stand in the middle of fifth day of the new and shoot somebody and I wouldn't
lose any voters, okay?
It's like an incredible.
It's not so much what's in your head and heart as it is, you looking around and seeing
what's acceptable, seeing what's okay, seeing what people will tolerate, and the election changed people's notion
of what was tolerable.
Stay with us.
This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedantin.
If you're a parent, your kid might have one of those inflatable punching bag toys.
Sometimes it's a clown, sometimes it's a shark, or like the one in this YouTube video,
an inflatable Spider-Man.
The thing that kids love about these toys is that they can punch them over and over,
and the toys keep bouncing back.
They're weighed down, usually with sand, and they can take all kinds of abuse.
These toys, which are known as bobo dolls, were used in a famous psychology experiment in the 1960s.
The researcher Albert Bandura had kids watch as a person repeatedly hit a bobo doll with a hammer.
I let research a Chris Crandel pick up the story from here.
Now what Banderah had his children watch
was somebody who came in, went and grabbed a hammer,
straddled the bobo doll so that it was lying on the ground,
and then hit it in the face with a hammer,
saying punch him in the face, hit him with a hammer.
Afterwards, sometimes what he called the model, the person doing the punching, was punished,
sometimes nothing happened at all, sometimes they were rewarded.
The children were then put in a room filled with toys, and over in the corner, a bobo doll,
and elsewhere in the room, a hammer.
The kids who had seen the model either have no consequence at all or be rewarded quickly
when over. Grab the hammer, got the bobo doll into the middle of the room and
started immediately pounding it about the face with the hammer and repeating
punch them in the face, hit him with a hammer. The kids who had seen the model be
punished did not do this at all. But here's the trick. Banderro wisely said at the end of
this, you know, if you saw something in that video that interested you, you should
know you won't be punished for this. At this point a majority of the kids get a
look of glee on their face, run over, grab the bobo doll, grab the hammer, and
start beating it about the face and straddling it and doing what they'd seen the model do.
The moral of the story is that we learn from what people do and sometimes we don't do it.
We may want to punch the bobo doll, but we know that we'll be punished for it.
If somebody comes in and says, it's okay, you can use that hammer and bobo doll. They will gleefully run and grab
the tools and start beating the face of the bobo doll.
So what do Albert Bandora and the bobo dolls have to do with free speech? The link here
has to do with social norms and what's permissible and not permissible. Chris Krandel conducted a study in the lead up to the 2016 presidential election.
He looked at both liberals and conservatives and asked them a series of questions.
We were interested in what prejudices people had and we also wanted to ask them what they
thought it was okay to express in the world.
So we asked them about Muslims, Canadians, blind people, immigrants,
women, and we asked them, how do you feel about them on a zero to 100 scale and the
lower the number, the less you like them. But other people, we asked them, what do
you think is okay to express in the US? And we asked them about all the same groups, Muslims, Canadians,
blind people, and so on. So we were able to find out not only what people say they have
as prejudices, but what they think is acceptable to express in the US.
You brought the same people back after the election and you asked them the same questions.
What did they say? For both Trump conservative supporters
and Clinton more liberal supporters,
we found that they thought the nation had changed substantially
in what it was okay to express as a prejudice.
For the groups that Trump had actually targeted
in his campaign, Muslims.
I think Islam hates us.
Mexicans.
They're bringing drugs.
They're bringing crime.
They're rapists.
Illegal immigrants.
They beat us at the border.
People are flowing through.
Drugs are coming across, pouring across.
Fat people.
Only Rosie O'Donnell.
There was a significant increase in how acceptable it was to express prejudice towards them.
The interesting thing is, there was no change in the acceptability of prejudice towards
groups that Trump had not aimed his prejudice at blind people, Canadians, and so on.
What do you draw from the experiment, Chris?
I mean, it seems remarkable that in a matter of weeks, you know, from stage one to stage two of your experiment,
people's views of what they consider acceptable are changing.
Yes, it was really only about 10 days, two weeks, and we don't think that anybody changed their hearts and minds in that time.
What we think is that the election of Donald Trump changed people's understanding of what America felt.
The election of Donald Trump, despite all of his overt expressions of prejudice, meant
that it must be okay in America to have these prejudices.
And so their scores on the acceptability of prejudice for the Trump-targeted groups went
up, but not the ones he didn't target. The interesting thing about this is that people's own prejudices did not go up following their
sense that America accepted it.
And in fact, to a small degree, our participants, both Trump supporters and Clinton supporters,
said they went down a little bit in prejudice just a little bit.
And what we think is going on is that they looked around, saw the acceptability of prejudice,
saw in America how much there really was, more than they thought, and they said to themselves,
oh, I'm less prejudice than I thought I was.
I must be okay, because there's a lot more prejudice out there than I thought.
Dude, it's tricuous as remarkable that things change so quickly.
I mean, I think most of us think about social norms
as being relatively stable and enduring things.
They're the things that are acceptable in a country
or a society.
One day are probably gonna be the same things
that are acceptable tomorrow or next week.
And you're finding there was a dramatic change.
Was it just the scale of the election,
the fervor of the election that you think
might have made the difference? Or do you think that actually social norms actually
are very malleable, and they can change fairly quickly?
You're right that most norms are stable, especially ones that have been around a long time
and are as public as prejudice norms in the US.
But this is a little bit different.
First of all, the presidential election and coverage
of Trump and prejudice was massive. The mass media and personal discussion over Trump
and his discussion of racial groups, immigrant groups, fat people and so on was a huge intervention.
Second of all, people were surprised that the American public
approved of it by electing him. Now of course it wasn't the popular vote but
still winning the presidency alone is likely to have an effect on social
norms. But one key difference with this social norm is that there is a large
number of Americans who are suppressing their prejudice, who were
holding it back, who wouldn't say what they really felt because they knew it would be
punished. This is what is meant by political correctness. People have attitudes that
are negative, but they don't say them out loud because they know that they're unpopular.
The election of Trump removed the suppression. It was a key that opened up the floodgates just a little bit for people who had been suppressing their feelings a lot.
And that's why the number of hate crimes seems to have jumped right after the election,
not before when the speech was all there, but after when the nation seemed to approve of Trump's prejudices.
So it's almost, you know, a parallel, this eerie parallel to the Bandoorestety, because
what you're saying is it's not enough just to see the model strike the Bobo doll with
the hammer.
That isn't what actually causes the children to then imitate the adult.
It's actually seeing what happens to the model.
After he or she does that to the bubble doll,
does the model get rewarded, does the model get punished?
It's that action that then determines whether the children
imitate the model or don't imitate the model.
That's exactly right.
It's not so much what's in your head and heart
as it is you looking around and seeing what's acceptable,
seeing what's okay, seeing what people will tolerate.
And the election changed people's notion of what was tolerable.
I'm wondering if this research has any connection with some other work that you've done, looking
at the question of authenticity.
You found that we all crave politicians who are authentic and certainly that was one of the big appeals of Donald Trump.
But sometimes you've argued or your research has found that authenticity is a way to express more subtle feelings.
Yeah, people said that Donald Trump was very authentic and we wondered there's really two ways that that could go.
One is that we think he's authentic because he says things that are unpopular.
And by saying things that are counter to the social norms, we see that he's revealing
something about himself that's different from every other person.
To say that you like ice cream is to reveal very little about yourself, but to say that
I don't think ice cream is any good, reveals quite a lot about you because it's so unusual and so atypical.
With the case of prejudice, we wondered if maybe people thought he was authentic because
he was saying these horrible things that they thought were horrible, but holy cow, at least
he says it.
I don't frankly have time for total political correctness.
And to be honest with you, this country doesn't have time either.
That's one possibility.
The other possibility is exactly the opposite.
And that is, authenticity is really just code words for saying,
you are saying the prejudice that I have.
An authentic person is somebody who says what I feel when I can't see it myself.
Trump supporters who listen to this critique might say that they are being boxed into a corner.
Just because they believe that Trump is authentic or they believe in free speech protections,
doesn't automatically mean that they're closet racist.
That's a fair criticism.
The reason we are interested in Trump
is that we are interested in trying to explain
how Americans were able to tolerate his racism.
I think that most of us in the social science community
were surprised that Trump
was elected not because of his politics, but because his racism, his attitudes towards
immigrants, his anti-fat prejudice, his misogyny, didn't seem to stop most Americans or many
Americans from voting for him.
And that's the question that we're trying to explain.
It's not particularly a liberal or conservative thing to be hypocritical.
It's a very human thing to do that.
But the public policy implications for prejudice, and we are prejudice researchers, is why we
chose to look at Trump and authenticity in this way.
Where does the research go from here?
And are you still trying to figure out ways to study if social norms are continuing to change
as we move further and further from the 2016 election?
We've been following some of the same people that we followed at the election.
And what we have found is that
the norms for prejudice are becoming more tolerant of prejudice, even beyond what Trump
targeted. So the bad news is that it seems that all prejudices are becoming somewhat
more acceptable as the course goes on.
Chris Krandel is a psychologist at the University of Kansas.
Chris, thank you so much for joining me today on Hidden Brain.
Thank you, Shankar.
Hidden Brain is produced by Jenny Schmidt, Maggie Pennman, Raina Cohen, Renée Claure, and Parth Sha.
Our supervising producer is Tara Boyle. produced by Jenny Schmidt, Maggie Penman, Raina Cohen, Renee Clarre and Parth Shah.
Our supervising producer is Tara Boyle.
This episode of the show was produced by Lucy Perkins, who joined us on a rotation for
a few weeks.
She is now off to Austria on a fellowship.
Lucy is the sort of producer who consistently fires on all cylinders, and we are so impressed
by all she's done in her time with us.
Thanks Lucy.
Our unsung hero this week is Gemma Huli. by all she's done in her time with us. Thanks Lucy.
A rungsang hero this week is Gemma Huli. Gemma is the vice president of member partnership here at NPR.
She oversees the team that interacts with stations across the country
and she helps bring the best of public radio programming to your local airwaves.
Gemma's office is right next to our planning board,
so she's often unwittingly subjected to our meetings as we plan for upcoming episodes. We're launching a radio version of Hidden Brain
this fall, and Gemma's leadership is one of the reasons that launch is going smoothly.
Coming up next week, we're going to look at regret. Why are minds are drawn to kudder,
woodder, shudder kinds of thinking, and why this might sometimes be a good thing.
As I learned more, I really started to realize
that regret is actually a very hopeful emotion.
I'm Sean Carvita Anton, and this is NPR.
you