Hidden Brain - How Others See You
Episode Date: July 3, 2023It's not easy to know how we come across to others, especially when we're meeting people for the first time. Psychologist Erica Boothby says many of us underestimate how much other people actually lik...e us. This week, we revisit one of our most popular episodes to look at how certain social illusions give us a distorted picture of ourselves.Do you like the ideas and insights we feature on Hidden Brain? Then please consider supporting our work by joining our new podcast subscription, Hidden Brain+. You can find it in the Apple Podcasts app, or by going to apple.co/hiddenbrain. Thanks!Â
Transcript
Discussion (0)
This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta.
Many of us spend enormous amounts of time asking ourselves what other people think of us.
Do they notice our flaws? Are they mocking us behind our backs?
Do they think we're boring?
This sort of rumination can be exhausting, anxiety-provoking, and above all else,
it's often spectacularly wrong.
There's just so many things that we mistakes that we fall into,
these sort of social traps that lead us to be a lot more pessimistic
about our social lives than kind of reality warrants.
Today on the show, why are assessments of how people see us can be so off-base
and how these incorrect
impressions shape our relationships at home and in the workplace.
How to see the world with greater clarity and walk with greater confidence this week on
Hidden Brain. When we talk to other people, we are often trying to figure them out.
But we also try to guess what the other person thinks of us.
We worry, how am I coming across? Am I flaws on prominent
display? Or does this person think I'm cool? Most of us think we are good judges of
our social interactions that we can tell if other people like us. But new
research suggests this is often not the case. Our perceptions of our social
interactions are often distorted. At the University of Pennsylvania's
Wharton School, psychologists Erica Boothby studies these distortions and what we can do about them.
Erica Boothby, welcome to Hidden Brain. Thanks for having me.
I'd like to take you back to the start of your interest in this topic, Erica. You were in grad school
and sitting at a cafe with a friend
and you'd also planned to meet a potential collaborator at the cafe. She showed up and
you went over and started chatting. What happened next?
Yeah, so I was at a cafe just down the street from my apartment called Cafe Romeo and I
worked there a lot with my partner who's also a psychologist.
And I went and left him, went to a couple tables down
where I was talking to this person who I thought we might
launch a collaboration.
And I really got the sense from our conversation
that she was a very interesting person
and had a lot of interesting ideas.
But as I was talking to my partner
and debriefing him on how it had gone,
I said, I really doubted that she would want to work with me.
Because our conversation hadn't really gone all that well.
I'd expected her to ask me about one stream of research.
She instead took the conversation
in a totally different direction, and I was unprepared.
And then my partner got a sheepish look on his face
and he admitted that he'd actually been eavesdropping a little.
But the good news was, is that he thought we had really hit it off
and that I'd come off really well in the conversation.
And so we had these wildly different perspectives
on what had happened.
So as psychologists, this guy was thinking,
who was right in this case, right?
Who had a better read on the situation?
Erika asked herself,
whether the incident revealed something not just about her,
but something about people in general.
It was very curious that there was this kind of wide gap
it seemed between what my partner had observed
from the outside and what I had felt as someone
on the inside
of the conversation.
And it got us thinking there must be
some interesting psychology here.
To test this, Erica teamed up with some researchers
from the United Kingdom who were organizing
personal development workshops
where lots of strangers meet one another.
And what we like is that this was outside of the lab,
which is where we recently run studies,
and so we could see how this kind of happens in the real world.
And we found that people believed their conversation partners were more interesting than they thought
their conversation partners found them.
Which is, of course, exactly what you had felt in the cafe, which is that you thought
the conversation went well, but you believe that your potential collaborator would not have found it as interesting.
Exactly.
In the case of the work when I was in Cafe Romeo
having this conversation,
we didn't know exactly what the other person,
the potential collaborator thought of me.
We just knew what my partner had observed.
And so what's interesting is that,
as we continue to pursue this
and bringing this into experimental
paradigms, we could really test people's perceptions against reality.
The experiment that you conducted with the volunteers who thought they were part of a
personal development workshop, it revealed to you in some ways that the experience that you had
was not unique, and it wasn't just that one person thinks that they enjoyed the conversation
by the other person did not, but thinks that they enjoyed the conversation by the other
person did not, but that both people enjoyed the conversation, but both believed that the
other would be less into the conversation.
You came up with a term for this phenomenon.
What was that term, Erica?
This is the Liking Gap.
And the Liking Gap is the phenomenon.
It was commonly this happens among people who are getting acquainted for the first time.
And so when you're meeting someone new, you're having that initial conversation.
Afterward, you reflect on how it went.
And most often, people actually enjoy these initial conversations
and like the other person quite a bit.
But they tend to underestimate how much the other person
likes them and enjoy their conversation.
You ran a similar experiment with first-year college students who are getting to know one another as
potential dorm mates. What happened in that experiment?
as potential dorm mates, what happened in that experiment? Right, so in that case, we were actually curious to see whether we would see a
laking gap as people increasingly get to know each other over time.
And so we recruited college roommates who had just landed at school for the first
time in September, and they get thrown into a room with someone they've never met
before, and a huge question for them is whether the other person is going to like them.
And that really matters to them.
And so we tracked these new roommates
at five different time points between September and May.
We asked them how much they liked their roommates
and how much they thought their roommates liked them.
And what we found there was that people actually underestimated
how much their roommate like them for several
months, like through the fall, into the spring, and finally by the end of the year, we did
see that the liking gap went away.
But you also took this idea and built on it to suggest that it might be that this is not
the only place in which we misjudge how other people think of us and how the interaction is going.
Talk about how and when you decided that this was not
just about one thing, Erica, just about the lacking gap,
but about a much larger set of phenomena
that you eventually came to describe as social illusions.
Yeah, so we're constantly trying to figure out
what other people think of us.
And I have sort of taken this puzzle and zoomed in
on several different facets of it.
And I think there's just so many things
that we, mistakes that we fall into,
these sort of social traps that lead us
to be a lot more pessimistic about our social lives
than kind of reality warrants.
Mm.
I want to talk later in the episode about ways
we can get past some of these distortions
in our perceptions of our social relationships.
But I want to look at how common these distortions are.
I'm thinking about the movie when Harry met Sally.
There's this classic scene when Harry and Sally
finally get together and then have separate conversations
with friends about how it went. I'm worried. I had to get out of there. He just disappeared. about how it went. I'm so embarrassed.
I don't blame him.
That's horrible.
I think I'm catching a cold.
No, it would have been great if it worked out, but it didn't.
I'll call you later, okay?
Okay, bye.
Bye.
Bye.
So, you know, when I hear that clip after reading your work, Erica, I'm struck by how much time
and effort we put into trying to understand these social interactions and how often we get things wrong.
I love that. It's a great example of how we don't tell each other the things that we're thinking
and that we feel. We express them instead oftentimes to other people, right? And this helps
keep these illusions alive. Yeah. Is it possible that some of these social illusions, in fact, are amplified during, you know,
high pressure, high-stake situations like dating? I mean, can these romantic situations, for
example, put the liking gap on steroids? Yeah, I mean, I think anytime where we really,
really care about what someone else thinks of us, we're going to put extra
pressure on ourselves to perform at the highest level. And so in those moments, we're going to be
hyper aware of the ways that we're falling short of the way that we wish we behaved or the things we
wish we said or hadn't said. And so those are going to be extra salient to us to the extent that we
care a lot about how we're coming off. So we've looked at one important dimension of social illusions.
Let's look at another.
And again, let's go back to your time in graduate school.
At one point, you were really self-conscious about appearing smart enough and good enough
to being grad school.
And one time, you were talking with some friends at a bar, and the conversation turned to the
TV shows that people were watching.
Right. Everyone was still kind of in the earliest ages getting to know one another.
And to this day, I still don't know why this was my response, but for whatever reason,
I said I watched the Bachelor.
Bring on the women!
Wait, this is like the whole package.
As conversations do, it kind of just drifted to another topic pretty quickly after that.
But later that night, I kept kind of coming back to that moment and just replaying it in
my head.
Because I just couldn't believe that that was the show that I came up with when I was
with a bunch of people that I really wanted to impress.
You know we hear it, big boy.
And I feel like something like this happens to us all the time, that you know, trivial
things happen, we say or do things, and then we perseverate on this for hours or sometimes
for weeks afterwards, you know, wondering what people thought of us and eating ourselves
up on the inside.
Exactly.
Our thoughts basically just run wild.
Like, what if I hand on it?
What if I had talked about the National Geographic show I watched instead?
Like, why did I had done it? What if I had talked about the National Geographic show I watched instead? Like, why did I say that?
Should have talked about the Ken Burns documentaries that you're a big fan of.
Exactly. I always think, you know, like, oh god, they're just judging me for that, right?
I said the wrong thing. And what happened in this case? Did you ever discover whether your friends
and graduate school thought less of you because you watched the Bachelor?
So, yeah, a few days later,
I actually ran into someone who was at that conversation.
And she asked me if I wanted to join her
as my friends who watched the Bachelor together every week.
Ha ha ha.
Ha ha ha.
The Bachelor's back.
Ha ha ha ha.
So clearly I wasn't as alone as I thought.
Ha ha ha.
We've talked a bit about how some of the social illusions that you study arise in interactions with people who are strangers or acquaintances or even people who are starting to get to know
one another.
But social illusions can also affect long-standing relationships.
So I want to play a clip from the fresh prince of Belair,
our Will Smith's character is Wari that is uncle,
who has been taking care of him for the past several years,
thinks poorly of him.
I just don't want you to think that I'm
this same stupid kid I was when I first moved out here.
How could you possibly believe it?
That's what I'd be thinking.
Look at you.
You're moving out on your own.
You're going to finish college in a year. You're moving out on your own. You're gonna finish college in a year.
You're becoming a man.
Man, I'm damn proud of.
I just don't want your last memory of me to be no better than the first one.
You have no idea what my first memory of you is.
I remember a kid loaded with all the potential in the world. You have no idea what my first memory of you is.
I remember a kid loaded with all the potential in the world.
And now I see a person on the verge of realizing their potential.
Can you talk about this a moment, Erika?
A familiarity with someone doesn't automatically mean that these social illusions disappear.
Right, I think they are more extreme
for people we do not know, in large part,
just because we don't know how they think
or what kinds of things they judge people for.
But even as we get closer to people,
it's not a guarantee,
because we don't have access to every single thing
that those people think.
And so we do, there is kind of a gap.
There's still even as you get closer to someone.
So the consequences of some of these social illusions are not always serious.
Sometimes they can be funny.
Adam Mastriani and his colleagues recently published a study looking at when people want conversations to end.
And they found that conversations almost never end when person A want conversations to end. And they found that conversations almost never end
when person A wants them to end,
and they don't then when person B wants them to end either,
conversations sometimes run shorter than either party wants,
and sometimes they run much longer than either party wants.
And I feel like the speak to the gap
between how we think a conversation is going,
and what we think the other person thinks
about the conversation.
Yeah, exactly.
I love that work.
And I think it's, again, because we aren't explicit
with each other about what we want.
We rely on a lot of implicit cues,
and that's how a lot of social life
and a lot of conversation operates.
So I think that we tend to just sort of hope
that we're able to understand what the other person wants
and when they might want to get out of the conversation
then we gently let them go.
Right.
But we often get that wrong.
Yeah.
In that paper, the researchers write that humans
are often unable to solve these problems
because solving these problems requires people
to share information that they normally
keep from each other.
So the problem is not just that we're not perceiving things, but we're actively hiding
the information that would allow the other person in fact to draw the right conclusions.
Right, that's a problem.
I mean, I think thinking about these things a lot does make me aware that it would help
if we could be more explicit about some things.
I know that we don't always want to play our hand fully.
But maybe if these college roommates that I studied for example had played their hands just a little sooner,
you know, a few months before, maybe things would have been a little bit clearer to people.
But I think we're very guarded in a lot of ways. We're very risk-averse when it comes to getting rejected or the possibility of rejection.
And what it really shows us is the power of our attention and what we're focused on.
And if we are focused on the negative thoughts about ourselves that we're projecting onto other
people, that looms really large and it tends to obscure other things that might actually also be true.
For years, social scientists have been warning about a global epidemic of loneliness.
Social isolation contributes to depression, anxiety, and drug abuse.
Chronic loneliness increases the risk of heart disease and other illnesses.
Is one solution to see that many of us actually have more friends than we realize?
When we come back, the mechanisms in the mind
that make it difficult for us to see our social interactions clearly and how we can do better.
You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta.
Many of us spend sleepless nights worrying about what others think of us, but the reflections
we see of ourselves in the eyes of others are often distorted.
We misread interactions, overthink our own insecurities. We fail to
notice what other people actually notice about us. Erica Boothby and her
colleague studied the psychological mechanisms behind these social illusions.
Erica, you lived in Italy as a high school exchange student. Did you speak
Italian? So I didn't speak Italian.
I had taken just a quick kind of crash course in Italian,
which was for traveling essentially.
So I learned things like how to count to 10.
And I could say my name is Erica.
I'm American.
Maybe a couple other things.
That was kind of the extent of it.
And it was full immersion.
So I was living with an Italian family,
and I was going to Italian high school.
And I felt pretty awkward because I couldn't really engage
in conversations with people.
So even at home with the host family sitting
around the dinner table, I would want to know about my day.
I'd want to be able to tell them, but we just couldn't get there. And it was really challenging.
And I found the same thing at school with my classmates.
They'd all stand around and chat during the breaks.
And I would kind of stand there awkwardly,
trying to figure out how I could jump
into the conversation or say something.
But my talent was just so impoverished at the time.
It was really hard to partake in social life.
But as I kind of got to know people
and my language skills improved,
they improved a lot over the course of the year
that I was there.
I started to realize that people didn't have as many
critical thoughts about me as I had about myself.
I'd been projecting a lot of that
and getting really down on myself,
but those weren't really the thoughts
they were having at all.
Yeah, so it's totally understandable as a high school student in a situation like this,
why you would be focused on your insecurities or the mistakes that you're making as you are
trying to learn and speak Italian. But of course, the people who are around you are probably thinking,
you know, here's this, you know, bright young girl from the United States who's taking the time and
trouble to learn our language, you know, and they're probably filled with admiration for you
even as you might be filled with insecurity.
Yeah, I wish I'd realized that at the time.
But instead, yeah, I was very insecure
and I felt awkward that I couldn't really engage
in these conversations.
But I do think also that this experience
doesn't just apply to people in a foreign country
because in general, our conversation partners are pretty charitable. People don't really care if you
mess up or if you don't say everything perfectly. That's kind of just how conversation works
and people expect it.
You currently teach a negotiations course at Wharton and you have two 90-minute sections back to back. So you're
giving the same lecture twice with very little time in between. What is the social illusion
that goes through your head as you do this, Erica? One thing that tends to happen in social
interactions is that we often feel like our internal states, our emotions, our thoughts,
our kind of on display for everyone to see. And we think that people are using that information
when they're forming their evaluations of us.
And this is something that is often called
the illusion of transparency.
And so yeah, I teach this course, two sections back to back.
And in the first section, I am full of energy.
But by the second lecture, I know that I'm starting
to get a little depleted.
And so I feel like my second class is never as good as the first class,
because I'm comparing it to the first class, and I had more energy then.
And so at the end of the semester, I'm always surprised when I looked at my ratings
for the two classes, and they are identical.
And so this just goes to show from the inside when I'm trying to imagine how other people are evaluating me.
I'm using all these comparisons that they're not using, right? I'm tired. I wasn't as witty the second time,
but of course my second class wasn't at my first class. And so to them, my lecture seems perfectly fine.
And so I think we do the same exact thing in conversation. So when we tell people a story, or we give people a summary of a project
we're working on at work,
we know all the little flaws and the things we say,
or the things that we said better last time we describe them,
but our conversation partners don't have as much
to compare to as we do.
And so they're seeing it totally differently.
Erica has conducted experiments that reveal exactly how much people notice about one another.
In one study, she had volunteers put on a shirt and go to meet another person.
The catch?
The shirt had a photo in the front of Pablo Escobar, the infamous Colombian drug lord responsible for dozens of
murders. We brought people in, we treated the lab like a waiting room, we basically converted
it, and we had two participants come in at a time, they sat across from one another at this
large table, and one person had on this Pablo Escobar shirt, and the other person was just
without them knowing it, they were assigned to be our observer. And then afterward we measured how much that observer had been noticing and thinking
about the other person, what they looked like, anything about them. And we asked the person wearing
the Pablo Escobar shirt, how much attention they thought the other person was paying to them.
I'm imagining that most of them would believe that the other person is keenly aware that
they're wearing a photo of a drug kingpin.
Was that in fact what happened?
No, the person wearing the shirt is very self-conscious, but from the other person's perspective, that
was just whatever they happened to be wearing.
And so people who were wearing the public Eskivar shirt overestimated how much tension was
on their shirt because that was the thing that they were hyper focused on.
But people were paying attention to all kinds of other things about the person.
You know, like what they were doing, did they go on their phone, did they look like they
just came from the gym, like I don't know whatever the thoughts were, but they were not paying
attention to the shirt specifically.
So when we think about the interactions that we have with other people, Erica, we're often
focused on the very minute things about ourselves, you know, how our hair looks or the clothes
we are wearing or the picture on our shirt and other people in fact are not taking us in at this
micro level. Can you talk about some of the illusions that we use study coming about because people
are not evaluating us with
the granularity with which we're evaluating ourselves.
Whenever we're paying attention to something and we're concerned about it, like you said,
you know, I got a new haircut. Now that's when I'm paying attention to my hair feels so
short. I must be obvious to other people, but you know, in reality, people aren't noticing
that specific thing about us, right? The thing that we happen to be focused on or self-conscious about.
But that doesn't mean that people aren't paying attention to us in other ways or having other thoughts about us.
I suspect this is true of us in terms of our evaluations of others.
When we're evaluating someone we meet for the first time, we are really trying to get a global impression of who they are.
Will I be able to get along with this person?
We're not looking at them with great granularity.
Exactly.
I think that really captures a really deep truth.
And so we're trying to just figure out, friend or foo, right?
We're at a high level.
We're like, are they going to hurt us?
Or are they going to be our friends?
Do they seem like a nice enough person?
Maybe can I trust them?
Whereas when we're worried about being judged or when we're on the other side, we're thinking at the micro level in terms of did we say something
wrong, and that's really not what we're being judged on.
Right. You know, I'm reminded of this incident a few years ago, the BBC was doing a live
interview about South Korean politics with an American professor. I don't know if you've seen
this, Erica, but the professor was looking very serious
and explaining why the impeachment of the South Korean
president was a good thing.
I'm actually quite proud of the Koreans,
I don't mean to sound kind of sending,
but I've been living here for 10 years.
This is probably the best day I've actually lived here.
I mean, I'm actually quite impressed
at how they've done this.
And then he was, as he was talking,
his young daughter with pigtails bounces into his office
and the professor tries to maintain his composure and push her away.
Um, I would be surprised that they do.
The um, pardon me.
And then a baby paddles in on one of those baby walkers followed by a woman who's crawling on all fours,
trying to drag both kids out of the room and the professor keeps going on with his analysis.
Now his kids are wailing in the background.
And I feel like all of us have been there in one form or another during the pandemic,
where our real lives in some ways have intruded on the impression that we're trying to create
on others.
But I feel it reveals the lengths we go to present a certain impression of ourselves.
And in some ways, our fears of what would happen if other people could simply see us as we are.
Yeah, that's a great example. I think it's so true. We're all very concerned about impression
management. We want people to see us in a certain light. And in some ways, it doesn't matter
all that much, because I think people are much more charitable
toward us than we expect.
They kind of explain away those things.
They make us relatable.
And so I think we can be worried because we have some vision of how we want to come off
to other people.
But in reality, we could probably stand to be a little bit more just ourselves.
So we've looked at different ways.
People overestimate how others will judge them and judge them harshly.
I want to play you a movie clip about it's slightly different idea.
This is from Harry Potter and the Sorcerer's Stone.
Harry and his best friend Ron are opening Christmas presents at Hogwarts School
and Harry finds he has received a mysterious gift. Why is it?
It's some kind of cloak.
Well, let's see them, put it on.
Woah!
My body's gone!
I know what that is.
That's an invisibility cloak.
I'm invisible.
So turns out Erica, it's not just wizards who own invisibility coke! I'm invisible.
So turns out Erica, it's not just wizards who own invisibility cloaks.
You once ran a study where you surveyed people in a cafeteria and asked them how much they
were watching other people and how much they thought other people were watching them.
What did you find?
We found that people thought that they were observing others far more than they
themselves were being observed. So it's almost like people felt like they were
invisible, but they could pierce the invisibility cloaks that other people were
wearing. Exactly.
Erica mentions a real life example of this. In 2017, two of President Donald Trump's lawyers were having lunch at a restaurant in
downtown Washington DC.
They were discussing serious matters of state.
This is when Thai Cobb and John Dowd met for lunch at this busy, outdoor patio in downtown
Washington DC.
So Cobb, just to remind everyone, was the lawyer in charge of coordinating the White House's responses
to the Mueller investigation and they were looking at a Trump's alleged entanglements with Russia.
And dad was Trump's lead outside attorney in the investigation and the two of them were
discussing very sensitive information at this lunch. So they talked about the ongoing investigation,
some things about Jared Kushner. And these were all things they thought that they were discussing
privately just amongst themselves. And of course there was a reporter from the New York Times
who happened to be sitting at the next table, and as far as that reporter was concerned,
the two men did not have an invisibility cloak around them. That's right. They definitely did not.
So this reporter actually posted a photo on Twitter of the two
attorneys talking, along with the caption,
here's a photo of Ty Cobb and John Dowd,
casually and loudly, discussing details of Russian investigation at BLT State DC.
Well, I said it next table.
And so I proceeded to order a few more iced teas and listen in on this very revealing conversation.
Well, why do you think this happens, Erica? And what do you think explains the fact that we have
almost these dual and conflicting social illusions? On the one hand, we believe that people are
judging us harshly and perhaps being overly observant of our flaws. And on the other, almost simultaneously, we believe that we
have a cloak around us that other people cannot see through.
Yeah, so I am very interested in the psychology of what makes us so oblivious to the fact that
these guys could be sitting and talking about sensitive information, not realizing they're
being observed and not to sound conspiratorial,
not like you're watched all the time,
but I think the point is,
we can get into a mode where we are the people watchers, right?
We're observing other people, we're looking around.
I mean, just think about how much you do this,
pretty much anytime you're in a public place.
And the reality is that all the times that we're doing this,
other people are also doing
it toward us.
They're watching us.
And this is something that happened to me early on as I was starting to think about the
invisibility collusion, but just sitting in a cafe and I'll be working and then periodically
I'll look up all kind of gaze around the room.
And everyone I see seems to be diligently also working like they're focused
on their laptop or they're talking to someone else.
But then what I know now based on my research is that as soon as I turn my attention back
to my computer, they're doing the same thing toward me, right?
They're glancing at me.
And so now I can't unknow that. This is something that I'm now much,
much more aware of. Yeah. Do I remember correctly that you runs around an experiment where you had
people come and sit in a waiting room, but the waiting room in fact was not a waiting room.
It was actually the scene of the study that you were running and you were trying to measure the same
exact phenomenon that we've been talking about. Right. And what's nice is there were two people there
and we knew what they were doing while they were in each other's vicinity. And what we find is that people felt like they were
the ones watching the other person. They didn't feel like the other person was watching them.
And it's actually just really hard to catch people watching you, which is part of the reason
we have this, we see this effect. So vision scientists call this gazed deflection.
When we're looking at someone and watching them,
we usually try to disguise it.
But that means that the people who are looking at you
are also trying to hide that fact.
So it's very hard for us to gather evidence
that we're being watched.
But when, as soon as we're not watching them, they're watching us.
Yeah. So it seems to me that you're telling me that in different ways, we can both overestimate and underestimate the ways in which people notice us.
Is there a tension between those two positions? I mean, how can those two illusions be happening simultaneously?
Yeah, I mean, it's a good question. I think we can get into thinking that we are the ones kind of observing other people.
And this is especially when we're not interacting with them directly.
In that case, we often feel like we're the ones watching other people.
But then when we're interacting with people, we're talking to them,
our critical voice turns on and we get more self-conscious, I think,
and more concerned about the way that we're coming off to other people.
And this can happen also, you know, kind of going back to the public Eskibar shirt, if people are wearing something that they
are self-conscious about or feel weird about, that will also cause them to think someone is
paying more attention to it. So we've talked a lot about our social interactions, but sometimes these
social interactions are also being played out in memory. So we think back to conversations, you know, what we said, what someone else
said. And one of the illusions that you've studied is that, you know, we think a lot about
other people a lot and what they said, we replay conversations in our minds, but we don't
assume that other people are doing the same thing. So exactly the same way that we believe
that we're observant of other people, but other people are not observing us. We also think about other people a lot more than we realize that they are thinking of us.
Can you talk about that phenomenon?
Right. So, you know, we spend a lot of time in conversations in daily life, and we also spend a lot of time thinking about our conversations after they've taken place.
So we think about the advice someone gave us, or maybe a funny story they told us, and we replay those moments.
We remember them, we relive them.
I think it's really clear to us when we're thinking about other people after we've interacted.
But what we find is that people systematically underestimate how much they remain on the other people's mind after their conversation.
Because they don't tell us, right? Just like we don't tell them, right?
And this sort of just perpetuates this illusion.
But I think we've looked at this to see kind of what some
implications are in some specific context.
So in one line of work, we looked at arguments.
So arguments between friends or significant others.
And we asked people how much they thought about the argument
since it happened and how much they had replayed parts of in their mind. And also how much they thought about the argument since it happened and how much they had replayed parts of their mind.
And also how much they thought their counterpart had.
And again, we found people believe they were the ones thinking about the argument more.
And if you're the one, you know, thinking about the argument, ruminating on it, concerned
about it, but you think your partner is doing that much less, you know, that is going to
make you feel a certain way, probably not a great way.
And it affects your beliefs about how likely it is that your partner wants to reconcile
or make up with you.
You know, Erica, I spoke recently with the psychologist Emily Pronen, and one of the things she studies
is how when we think about our own minds, we have access to all this information. We're
aware of our thoughts, our feelings, our hopes, our intentions.
But then when we talk to others, we have a much more limited understanding of what's going
on inside their heads.
And that's because, of course, we're not inside their minds.
How does this basic gap in perception shape the social illusions that you study?
Yeah, I think it's a huge part of it.
What we call the availability or accessibility of thoughts, right?
And our own thoughts are hugely available and accessible to us.
They're very salient.
They come to mind, but we don't really have access to the inner workings of other people's
minds.
And so this is a huge problem that contributes to these illusions.
The research of Michael Thomas-Sello and his colleagues recreated your liking gap experiment with children between the ages of four and eleven.
The researchers paired the kids up and asked them to build a tower together and then they asked each child
how much do you like the other boy or girl? How much would you like to be their friend?
Do you remember this experiment Erica and what they found? Yeah, so in this study,
yeah, they looked at kids age four through eleven and what they found? Yeah, so in this study, yeah, they looked at kids age four
through 11, and what they found is actually
that the legging gap does not exist in four-year-olds,
and it emerges actually this kind of critical period
around age five, which is when young children
are becoming more concerned with their reputations
and the impressions that they make on other people.
And then what's interesting is it also increases all the way to age 11, which is the upper
limit of the range that they were testing in this study.
And so we don't actually have data on teens, but I think for teenagers, the liking gap
is probably massive.
And I'd be really interested in kind of seeing what happens across adulthood, you know, as
people get older.
So it seems as if self-consciousness in some ways is sort of a crucial part of the liking
gap in some of these social illusions.
You know, very young children don't really care so much about what other people think of
them.
And so you could see why maybe they have fewer of these illusions, but if someone who
is 16, 17, and high school teenager is thinking about this a lot because their social interactions
really matter and how well they fit into the group really, really matter. And you can see perhaps as we go get older,
perhaps it starts to fade away again and we become more self-confident and perhaps less self-conscious.
I mean is that sort of the life cycle that you're seeing instead of the evolution of some of these
social illusions? That's my hypothesis. I don't have the data yet to speak to this, but that is what
I would predict.
We are collecting some data right now from people who are having conversations across
generations. So we're collecting data from 30-somethings and 70-somethings. And we're having the 30-ish
year olds talk to each other and the 70-ish year olds talk to each other. And then also
these cross-generational conversations between them. And so I think what's interesting there
is we can see
if there are age effects such that younger people
are showing a bigger, like,
an older people, does it occur also controlling
for who they're talking to, right?
If they're talking to someone within their own age group
or they're talking to someone of a different age group,
whether that affects the, like, an gap as well.
Do you think there are personality characteristics
that make some of these social
illusions either more likely or less likely? Yeah, so we have found that people who are more shy
exhibit a larger liking gap and I would expect that social anxiety would as well. We haven't
measured a lot of personality variables. We've only tested a few kind of in the early days, but I
would be interested in doing that in a larger sample to see what kinds of trends that we could find.
And in the other direction, I might imagine that some personality trait like narcissism, for example, might cause people to have the opposite conclusion.
They actually believe that they are the center of everyone else's world when, in fact, they're not.
Right. That would be interesting to look at as well.
Do you think there's a functional reason
why our brains produce some of the social illusions, Erica?
Why is it do you think that these exist in the mind?
There definitely can be a function in terms of trying
to improve for next time, especially with the laking gap.
So if we're focused on our kind of self-critical thoughts,
the times we fall short, the things we said
that we wish we hadn't,
thinking about those counterfactuals
and how we could have done better
or we could improve for next time,
might actually help us,
it might help us make a better impression next time
or become a more savvy social actor.
But I think it also has some pitfalls.
So I think we kind of need to find the right balance there.
When we come back, how to become more aware of the social illusions that pervade our lives,
and how to fight them?
You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta.
We all wonder what other people think of us, and we all want to be liked.
But we often walk away from conversations with friends and strangers, worried that maybe
we talk too much, or didn't say enough, or said the wrong thing.
Some of us spend sleepless nights worrying
about how social interactions unfolded. Psychologist Erica Boothby studies how a perception of our social
worlds are like looking into a fun-house mirror. She calls these distortions social illusions.
Recognizing these illusions for what they are is an important first step to seeing things more clearly
Yeah, being aware of it I think can help and a lot of people have written me about the licking gap and said how much it resonates with them
I've had clinical psychologists tell me that now they tell their patients about it and it's helped them feel better
Just knowing that they're not alone that other people feel this way too and that does seem to help
In addition to recognizing that these illusions exist, there are other techniques that can
also help us to see our interactions with other people more clearly.
Erika, when you first moved to Philadelphia to begin teaching at Wharton, you didn't
know many people, so you made a coffee date with a friend of a friend.
This was another young academic who was also new in town, and you were thrilled that you
had made a new friend.
But then you didn't hear back from her.
How did your initial conversation go and what went through your mind when you didn't hear
back?
Yeah, so I really liked her.
I could imagine becoming friends.
Of course, I tried to contain my excitement and play it cool.
And then a few days past, no word from her.
I didn't get a text saying she enjoyed a conversation
or anything following up.
And so I just, I don't know, got a little worried.
And I texted our mutual friend.
I raved about what a great, interesting person she was.
And I just said I wasn't sure, how much she liked me
or if she'd actually be interested in meeting up again.
I felt like I'd been kind of rambling a lot in the conversation.
I was nervous. The stakes felt kind of high.
I didn't have any friends here.
And so I wanted it to work out.
But to my surprise, then my mutual friend wrote me back.
And she said, that's so funny.
You both texted me the same thing about each other.
You should just text her.
And so there was victim to my own effect.
I studied the Laking Gap.
And so oftentimes I do try to check my self-critical voice
and override it when I can.
But for some reason in this moment,
I really wasn't thinking like a scientist.
I was just emotionally in the moment.
But luckily, we in this case had our mutual friend
to help set us straight.
Otherwise I'm just not sure we would have ever seen each other again, even though we both
wanted to.
I love the story because it really shows how sometimes we need to turn to friends to family
members, to therapists, to help us see reality more clearly.
Exactly.
I think it all really helps in these cases to get an outside perspective. And I think intuitively many of us understand that this is valuable. I think what I take away from
your work really is that it's really important to do this systematically, not just at a point where
you feel like something has gone wrong, but to systematically invite third parties in to basically
say, what do you think is going on? What do you see happening here? Because it's possible that they
will see things differently than you do. Yeah, I think that's exactly right. There's a second powerful idea in addressing
these social illusions. I want to play you a clip from the TV show Ted Lasso, Erica. A woman is
worried that a guy she's dating isn't as interested in her as she is in him. She thinks he might be seeing
other people and she confronts him. So the other day when he said you were too busy to take me back. Yeah because I was busy.
But you never told me what you were too busy with. Yeah because it was private.
Are you dating other people? It's okay if you are. It's just that I want to know so that I don't
know it's stupid. With yoga, okay? I do yoga with a group of women in their 60s.
They have no idea who I am.
It's twice a week and it's really good for my call.
You're never going to take it out.
Erica, can you talk about the idea that sometimes
a really effective way to find out
what's happening in the minds of other people
is to simply ask them?
Yeah, I think this is a great example.
And it just shows kind of what we're talking about
in terms of impression management, right?
He doesn't want to share where he really was while he didn't text back because that
is embarrassing for him.
But on the other flip side, for her, she really needed to know.
And so, yeah, I think this is a great example of how effective it can be to sometimes just
ask, but there's reasons that we don't.
And sometimes it's well-founded, right?
On the one hand, we don't want to pry or be pushy. And we think that, you know, if someone didn't tell us something, there's
reason for it. And we should kind of let them have that space. And on the other hand,
as we don't ask people things directly, because we're afraid of what the answer might be.
I want to talk about a third way to dismantle some of these illusions.
Can you talk about the importance of trying to pay less attention to ourselves, you know,
our own fears, our anxieties, our needs, and more attention to the other person, what they
actually are saying and doing?
How can that potentially play a role in dismantling some of these illusions?
The research on social anxiety shows that if you go into a conversation with the goal
of learning as much about your partner as possible,
that that shifts your attention from being focused
on your own thoughts and what you might be doing wrong,
toward being focused on your conversation partner.
And so I try doing this myself,
and a lot of the thoughts that normally fuel the liking gap,
are concerns about what people think of us,
just aren't there when I'm more focused on the other person.
So in conversations you're actually asking yourself,
stay focused on the other person,
stay focused on what's happening in their mind,
let me try and understand them a little better.
Yeah, I think even thinking about it not as try
to stay focused on them,
but if you actually think about it as trying to be curious about them and interested in them, that is actually
a better mental strategy.
And it just gives you a different mindset altogether in the conversation.
If you can kind of maintain your curiosity, ask them questions about themselves, ask them
follow questions, like that's really where you want to be, and that will help you get
more immersed in the conversation and less focused on yourself.
I remember reading a research study maybe about a year or so ago,
which talked about sort of how much people underestimate
the power of asking questions and conversations,
and how much they believe that a conversation
is about making statements and offering their opinions,
and how much more they're in fact liked
when, in fact, they ask questions of the other person,
which speaks exactly to what you're just saying, Erika.
Exactly, I mean, it's a win-win
because on the one hand, you're getting outside
of your own head and your own self-critical thoughts,
but also focusing on them, asking them questions,
learning about them, makes them like you more.
I wanna talk about one other idea, Erika,
and this is not so much about removing the social illusions
as much as it's about removing the social illusions as much as it's about
preventing the social illusions from harming our relationships. You grew up near the ocean
in Santa Cruz, California, and recently you've gone back to surf there. Can you tell me the story
of the King of 38th Street? And what the story taught you about the power of compliments. Yeah, so I got really addicted to surfing a couple of years ago,
and I go to this spot at 38th, it's just a great beginner wave,
and I spent a lot of time there,
especially early on just trying to figure out
how to ride waves, how to pop up,
which is just getting from laying down
on the board just standing up.
And there was this guy, he's an amazing surfer,
he's always out there,
and he was always in exactly the right spot,
catching all the best waves.
And my friends and I privately call him the king of 38th.
And he has no idea about that, of course.
But I always keep an eye on him when I'm out there
because he's so good, he's so fun to watch,
and I can really learn a lot from him.
And so one morning I was out in the water,
it was me, the king, and just a handful of other people,
and I was having one of the best days I'd ever had.
Catching just about every way I paddled for
getting these really long rides, and all of a sudden,
I saw the king of 38 himself paddling up to me,
and I thought, oh no, for sure he's gonna tell me
that I had done something wrong.
So the cardinal sin in surfing is dropping in on someone,
which just means paddling into a wave
when someone else is already on it.
And I thought for sure, I did something really dumb.
I hadn't noticed and he was about to lecture me.
But instead, he actually smiled at me,
told me that my positioning was perfect
and he noticed I just had a really nice natural style.
And I was totally floored. I mean, this made my week absolutely because the king himself had
a notice me, b, he's seen that I was having a great day out there, and c, he bothered to come over
and tell me about it. I mean, it was an amazing feeling. And there's a million times I've noticed
people doing things that I really liked, but I usually
kept those thoughts to myself.
But after I got this compliment from the King of 38th, and knowing what I do now about compliments
from my own research, I really try to make it a point to give people more compliments on
the water instead of just keeping those thoughts to myself and not telling anyone.
And I've noticed that that makes a huge difference.
I mean, people are always surprised when I compliment them
when we're surfing because these compliments are kind of rare.
Even though we're all sitting there thinking,
plenty of nice things about each other,
no one actually says what they're thinking.
I'm wondering if one of the advantages of offering compliments is,
in fact, people are often worried that other people don't like them.
In fact, their self-concept is that other people don't like me as much as I like them.
And when people give us compliments in some ways, they are helping us to dismantle the liking gap.
So we see that someone actually does actually like us, is coming up to say something nice to us.
It helps to compensate for this bias that we're carrying around inside our own heads.
I definitely think that's true. I also think that's why it's so surprising to us, it helps to compensate for this bias that we're carrying around inside our own heads. I definitely think that's true.
And I also think that's why it's so surprising to us when we receive these compliments because
we don't expect it, right?
We kind of expect the worst from people in a sense.
We don't realize how charitable they are socially or how positively they already think about
us.
So when they do give us these compliments, it's kind of a window into what they're actually thinking.
Erica Boothby is a psychologist at the University of Pennsylvania's Wharton School.
Erica, thank you for joining me today on Hidden Brain.
Thank you so much for having me.
Hidden Brain is produced by Hidden Brain Media. Our audio production team includes Bridget McCarthy, Annie Murphy Paul, Laura Quarelle, Kristen Wong, Autumn Barnes, Ryan Katz, and Andrew
Chadwick. Tara Boyle is our executive producer. I'm Hidden Brain's executive editor.
Our unsung hero today is Hidden Brain supporter Jacques Cofflin.
Jacques says he listens to the show in his ear buds while doing yard work or woodworking,
and that one of his favorite episodes is separating yourself from the pack,
our conversation about group dynamics with researcher Mina Chicara.
Thanks so much for your support, Jacques. We really appreciate it.
If you'd like to join Jacques in supporting the show,
there are a few ways you can do so.
You can go to support.hiddenbrain.org
to make a contribution.
Or you can become a subscriber to Hiddenbrain+.
That's our new podcast subscription,
where we'll bring you interviews and stories
you won't find anywhere else.
Like our recent interview with physician and novelist Abraham Vergis,
about the person who inspired him to become a writer.
You can try Hidden Brain Plus for free in the Apple Podcasts tab or by going to Apple.co-hiddenbrain.
I'm Shankar dantım.
Siyussun.