Hidden Brain - How to Really Know Another Person
Episode Date: September 19, 2022So often, we think we know what other people are thinking. But researchers have found that our attempts at reading other people go wrong more often than we realize. Â This week, we talk with psycholog...ist Tessa West about what we can all do to read people more accurately.If you like today's show, be sure to check out last week's conversation about emotions, and how they're shaped by where we live. And if you'd like to support our work, you can do so at support.hiddenbrain.org.Â
Transcript
Discussion (0)
This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedantam.
In the classic TV series Star Trek, Mr. Spock has a full-proof technique for accurately reading
the thoughts and feelings of others.
The Vulcan Mind Men. Through our James, Kirk, our minds are moving closer, closer, closer, closer, James Kirk, closer.
Here on Planet Earth, we have no technology that gives us direct access to the minds of
others.
So we look for psychological clues.
We watch people's facial expressions to see how much they like us.
We read their body language to figure out when they are nervous. We listen to their
intonations to pick up signals about their mood. How accurate are these clues? How good are we at detecting what's going on in the minds of others?
In recent years, scientists have spent a lot of time answering these questions.
They've discovered that many of the clues we use to read the minds of others are suspect.
But they have identified one mind-reading technique that is surprisingly effective.
The only problem?
Most of us don't use it.
How to get inside someone else's head this week on Hidden Brain.
Navigating social life involves continually reading the minds of other people.
What could be the meaning behind my colleague's behavior? What could my daughter be thinking and feeling?
What is it my parents really want?
Tessa West is a psychologist at New York University.
She studies interpersonal accuracy. How well the attributions we make about others
matches the reality of what is actually going on inside their minds.
Tessa West, welcome to Hidden Brain.
Thank you so much for having me.
Tessa, during your undergraduate years, I understand you had a part-time job that turned out to be an excellent venue
for reading other people's minds and intentions. What was this job?
So the job I had was selling high-end men's shoes.
It was commission-based, so every pair of shoes I sold, I got about 8% back.
And really, my income was primarily determined by selling rich people very expensive shoes.
And hoping I sold enough of those shoes to be able to make money for tuition.
So I understand the sales team was often eager to find the customers who look like they would be
the most lucrative customers given that your income
depended partly on these commissions.
Absolutely.
The sales team developed all these clever strategies
of figuring out who they could approach,
who would buy the most expensive shoes,
and who was also the least likely to have buyers remorse
and want to return those
shoes later.
So, they would pay attention to things like what people were wearing, how comfortable
they seemed in an expensive store, what their mannerisms were, even things like how much
of an hurry they came across, all these pieces of information were used to try to figure
out who the optimal customer would be.
When someone promising walked into the store, Tessa's colleagues on the sales team would
race to snag the customer.
We called them shoe sharks, and these are the people who would immediately make a beeline
for the best customers.
You know, it was a very competitive hierarchical world selling shoes, and I think you sort
of had to learn how to spot that customer and grab them before anybody else could, even saying
high and making eye contact, kind of count it is, this person is now my territory, they're
my customer, you better not come close.
So as you were finding your feet and sails, one time a customer came in wearing a three-piece
suit with a poodle trailing behind him.
I'm guessing this customer sounded promising. This customer was probably the most promising person I had encountered yet. He had a fancy pocket square
and he showed up with this poodle that was so well groomed. You know, I could tell its haircut
cost more than my haircut and it was huge. You know, I'm a college student. I have no money. He
brings this giant poodle at the time. The store had a policy that you could bring your little toy
poodle, but not a giant poodle.
So it took some real gumption to show up with a giant dog
and he kind of strolls in all the bravado in the world
and immediately picks out 20 of the most expensive pairs
of shoes that he wants to try on.
So I'm thinking to myself, yes, I'm
going to sell all the shoes that I need for this entire week
just with this one customer.
What happened, Tessa?
So basically what happened was he picks out his shoes.
He immediately kind of embraces this high status demeanor.
This is super humiliating, but he actually
asked me to get down on my knees and allow his dog
to give me a kiss, which I did, thinking to myself,
OK, this is money, money, I need it.
And then he tried on all these expensive shoes
and he had me use a shoe horn and put his foot in each shoe,
even tie the laces for him.
I mean, I was with him for several hours.
At the end of the day, he bought nothing.
So he was really just there, is like a power play,
to really show off how great he was,
but he wasn't interested in purchasing anything.
So I mean, obviously, this guy sounds like a real jerk, but from your point of view,
was a learning experience in terms of how well you could pick up what was happening in
someone else's mind?
Yeah, I mean, I think I learned that sometimes the thing we rely on, how fancy someone's
clothes are, you know, how confident they appear, aren't actually the best pieces of information to
accurately read someone's intentions. Or maybe I was just trying to read the wrong intentions.
What he wanted was lots of female attention. What he didn't want was expensive shoes. And
so I really learned a lesson that day.
Another time a customer comes in wearing a sweaty, dirty clothes he's covered in mud. I'm
guessing that this is not a customer
that the sales team fights over. Yeah, this guy comes in, he's wearing sweats, he's got dirt all
over him, and I walk up to this guy, and he's in a hurry, he's very flustered, and he says,
you know, I need five pairs of your most comfortable shoes, you know, for my gardeners. So I pull
up these shoes that are ugly but expensive and offer him these shoes, you know, for my gardeners. So I pull up these shoes that are ugly, but expensive,
and offer him these shoes.
And he says, OK, just get them in three different sizes.
I'm desperate right now.
I live in Monacito, which is like where Oprah lives.
It's a very expensive part of Santa Barbara.
You know, my gardeners have blisters.
And I had to actually mow my own lawn today, you know,
a terrific thing.
I need you to fix my problem right away.
And so immediately he buys, you know, thousands of dollars worth of shoes.
And I learned another lesson.
You cannot always rely on what people are wearing in judging their intentions.
And there I was sort of lucky to be wrong.
But, you know, that was also a very important learning experience for me.
So you finish college, you get your PhD and eventually you go on the job market and at one
point you're invited for an interview at New York University NYU and someone told you
that if you wore glasses for the interview that they would take you more seriously.
Why did you get this advice, Tessa?
Yeah, that's right.
So I think a lot of people assume that when you're a young woman,
I was 25, 26 at the time,
that you need to make yourself look smarter,
more professional,
and one piece of information people use to judge that
is whether you're wearing glasses.
So I took this advice.
I actually normally wear contacts,
but I went out, I bought some glasses,
and I couldn't actually forward.
I took my contacts out,
I wore the glasses on the interview,
hoping that that would work.
I also wore some outfits that other people had told me
would make me look less young, less girly,
all these kinds of things to kind of counter-stereotypes.
And so I really went in and leaned into that advice
because that was what one person's lay theory was
of what actually works.
So you get to this interview,
your glasses are sliding down your nose as you talk.
What, walk me through how the interview played out?
You know, the interview was very tough.
So I'm up there, I'm giving this presentation,
I have a PowerPoint, I'm in my uncomfortable glasses,
and within a minute someone raises their hand,
they kind of question my whole program of work.
And for the next hour and a half or so, just constantly,
those hands are popping up.
What did you mean by this?
How did you manipulate this?
I really felt like people must hate me.
This is very rough.
I was not used to that.
I didn't come from a place where people are aggressive.
I came from a sort of nice culture
where people ask questions at the end
and they always led with, you know,
this is wonderful research,
how about this one additional thing, and this was very different. I understand when you got back to
your hotel that night, you saw that there was a seam in your pants to the head come undone. Yeah,
just to add insult to injury. Oh wow, now I'm reliving this interview. My pants were completely ripped.
So on top of wondering, what did people think of my research?
You layer onto this.
Oh my gosh, they were not only thinking of my research that they clearly hated, but they
could probably also see way too much skin that I had no intention of showing.
To this day, I actually won't watch the videotape of my interview because I'm horrified
of what I will see when I walk away from the camera.
Some days later, one of Tessa's mentors thought he picked up some intel from an NYU search
committee member at a conference.
So I run into my mentor after this conference and I walk into his office and his face is
just long and sad.
And he said, what happened?
He goes, I don't think you're getting the NYU job.
And my heart starts pounding.
And I said, what happened?
Did they tell you I was bad?
I knew it.
I knew they thought I was bad.
He said, no.
I saw one of the search committee members
across the hall.
And he made eye contact with me.
And then he made a b-line for the bathroom.
He didn't want to talk to me.
Therefore, you did not get this position.
I'm really sorry to tell you. So he thought the search committee member was avoiding him
because it was going to be an awkward conversation. Yep.
Tessa resigned herself to the reality that she had lost out on the NYU job.
She grieved what she thought was the end of her academic career. She started to think about
going to cooking school. Then Then a couple of months later,
she got a call from the head of the NYU Search Committee.
And he says, congratulations, you know, I'm here to offer you the position. And I was shocked
and I said, are you calling the right person? And he's like, is this Tessa West? Yeah, this
is Tessa. Are you sure you mean to offer me the job?
And he's like, absolutely, it's you.
And then I said, yes, I'll take it.
And he's like, no, no, no, no, no.
I'm going to pretend you didn't say that.
You're going to have to negotiate to get a good offer,
but congrats.
So I screwed up everything, the whole process.
So I screwed up everything, the whole process.
So I screwed up everything, the whole process. Notice that at each step of the process, mind-reading mistakes led to faulty conclusions.
Tessa assumed wearing glasses would make her look smart. Instead, she came across as uncomfortable.
She figured the tough questioning was evidence of hostility. It wasn't. Her mentor mistreated glance from someone at a conference
who was headed for the bathroom.
From this glance, he concluded NYU was going to reject Tessa.
Wrong again.
In fact, the one time Tessa had hard evidence about what
the NYU committee really thought of her
was when the head of the committee called to offer her the job.
Her reaction?
Disbelief.
Errors in mind reading produced some of their worst consequences in interpersonal relationships.
Some time after she joined NYU, Tessa found herself talking to a colleague from her own school
at a conference.
So go to this conference. It's in San Diego, California, which is beautiful. I'm kind of excited
to enjoy the nice weather, but I'm also very nervous because I'm a new assistant professor.
I feel like I'm a little bit in and over my head. I came from a program that wasn't as prestigious
as some of the other programs that people came from. And I go to this conference and I run into another assistant professor and we're chatting
for a few minutes and then he looks at me and says, I'm really sorry, I don't think I can
talk to you anymore.
And I'm thinking, okay, this is weird.
And he says, I think I need to spend time with people who I don't work with.
I really need to expand my social network.
So I'm gonna walk away now and go talk to other people.
And I remember thinking, is my breath gross?
Am I not a big deal enough?
Am I not high status enough?
So my head is just kind of reeling
with all these reasons why this person wouldn't want to talk
to me anymore.
And I felt a little bit heartbroken over that
that maybe I've done something horribly wrong
to offend this person.
All of these negative thoughts are going through my mind.
It almost sounds like he was looking down at you.
Yeah, I definitely felt that.
What I felt was he was a thirsty social climber,
someone who is just trying really hard to get ahead
and didn't really care whether he's offending someone
to get there and just assume that this kind of strategy
was acceptable.
And part of me questioned whether I was the one
that was making the mistake.
Maybe this is actually what the people in the know do
is they tell others I can't talk to you,
I have to go expand my social network.
I just felt like very lost for the cause there.
I understand that you later discovered that his reaction
was mostly about his own anxieties.
They had very little to do with his feelings
or attitudes towards you.
Is that right?
Yeah, I think he felt that this is what he ought to do.
It wasn't even what he wanted to necessarily do.
He was actually engaged in the conversation,
but he felt like all eyes run him
and other people were looking at him and thinking, you know, why is he talking to his colleague?
Why is he trying to socially connect and, you know, inferring all kinds of things about
him?
Maybe he's antisocial or, you know, maybe he refuses to speak to anyone outside of NYU.
He's a snob about that.
So all of these things were going through his mind, which led him to engage in this behavior
towards me, that honestly had nothing to do with me.
So many tensions and conflicts and human relationships stem from making inaccurate attributions about
what is going on in other people's minds.
Tessa West began to think, are they tricks to getting folks to be more accurate in
reading other people's minds?
You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedantam.
This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedantam.
Psychologist Test-o-west studies interpersonal accuracy, How well we gauge other people's thoughts and feelings. We've seen
how easy it is for us to misattribute thoughts and feelings to others. Quite
regularly, of course, many of us find ourselves at the receiving end of these
errors. Tessa, you've had some memorable experiences of people reading your
mind wrong. One of them involved a student who had been working in your lab for three years.
Can you tell me that story?
Yeah, this story is actually pretty heartbreaking for me. So I've been working with a student
for three years and she always seemed a little bit nervous around me. And one day she comes to me
and confesses, you know, for the past several years I was afraid to come talk to you because I
thought you really hated me. I said, you know, what are past several years, I was afraid to come talk to you because I thought you really hated me.
I said, you know, what are you talking about?
Why on earth would you think that?
And she said, well, you know, a couple years ago, I saw you in the elevator and you gave
me the stink eye.
And from then on, I just decided that you disliked me.
Of course, I can't even remember this elevator moment.
I happen to have one of those faces
where if I'm not actively smiling,
I just look kind of irritable and angry
and annoyed all the time.
I'm like, oh, she caught me in a neutral face.
I know what happened.
And who knows what I was thinking?
I was probably just thinking to myself,
I really want coffee like so bad right now.
That's all I want when she ran into me,
but it really struck me that this student
had a snap moment with me, a thin slice.
We're talking 30 seconds max.
She inferred a ton of information from the look
on my face and it shaped her behavior towards me
for several years.
So this incident spurred you to begin researching
interpersonal accuracy, investigating why we miss
attribute thoughts and feelings.
And again, one of the most important things you found was again, what you just identified
here, our tendency to extrapolate from too little data to too large conclusions.
Yeah, I think that's true.
And I think it's also important that the students saw me in another contact.
She saw me in the classroom where I'm much more performative.
I tend to be very joky, fun.
I try to engage the students.
It's part of making a lecture come alive.
And she can trust that with see me kind of let down.
It's kind of like walking off stage
after giving a big talk or a big speech,
running into the speaker in the bathroom.
Their mannerisms are going to be very different
than when you watch them displaying very deliberative over
the top friendly behaviors.
And I think what really struck her was the contrast between those two.
And I don't think her experience is unique.
I think a lot of us actually make inaccurate judgments of people because we're contrasting
them into very different settings.
And I think we see this happen all the time when students run into their teachers outside
of the classroom.
I mean, just the other day, I was with my son and he ran into a camp counselor who was
wearing headphones and the counselor just kind of kept walking, you know, waved and said,
hi, and kept walking and he's a mommy.
Why doesn't my counselor like me?
I'm like, he's busy, honey.
Like, I know he's normally very friendly towards you when you're playing dodgeball, but this
is a different context.
So I think there is something to be said for learning that the context matters
and reminding yourself of that.
So another version of context
is what we might call culture.
And when I think about the interview
that you had at NYU,
part of what was happening during that interview,
which came across as very aggressive to you,
was really reflective of the internal culture at NYU.
Can you talk about that idea, both of what was happening
at NYU that prompted that kind of interview, as well as a larger
point of the ways in which culture influences how we're
reading other people's minds?
Yeah, I think so at NYU, what was interesting is that I actually
took that behavior of aggression, and I inferred it meant
dislike, and in fact, the opposite is true at NYU.
The more engaged people are, the more questions they ask,
the more they push you, the smarter they think you are,
the more interested they are.
And so I actually was attending to the right information,
but I was interpreting it in the opposite way.
And you know, had I had known that about the culture
that lots of questions means great things.
It means they think you're smart,
they're interested in the work.
I would have made a very different attribution
for that behavior.
And I think this actually can happen a lot.
We walk in a different culture as we're aware
that we have this cultural bias
that maybe we're not totally exposed to what those behaviors mean.
And we often make incorrect inferences
when we form those impressions
because we're anchored so much on our own culture,
on our own experiences.
You've also studied the phenomenon of something called egocentric bias. What is this, Tessa?
Ego-centric bias is probably one of the toughest things to get people to overcome when it comes to making accurate impressions.
So this is the idea that what goes on in our own minds is the rich of source of information that we have.
And we anchor what we think other people are thinking
and feeling, experiencing based on our own experiences.
So in the case of the bad interaction you had
with your work colleague at the psychology conference,
you came into that interaction with so many anxieties.
You know, when NYU said they wanted to hire you,
you questioned if they had called the right person, if they knew what they were doing, when you wanted to hire you, your question, if they had called
the right person, if they knew what they were doing, when you got to the conference, you felt
like you didn't really belong in a crowd of senior scholars. So in some ways, it seems
like you may have been projecting your own internal anxieties when you started reading the
mind of your colleague.
Absolutely. I was definitely projecting my own internal anxieties. And I think, especially
when the situation is a little bit unclear what the
causes, those ambiguous situations are perfect breeding grounds for egocentric bias. We're not quite
sure why someone's doing what they're doing. So we're just going to assume it's because of all
this stuff going on in our own heads. And I think the funny thing about that example is the person who
engaged in those behaviors was also had an egocentric bias. He was doing these things because he thought
everyone was staring at him and this is what he ought to do.
And so the egocentric bias is actually
going in both directions between the perceiver, me,
and also the actor, the target, that person.
There's been interesting research that
shows that we are more likely to deploy projection
when we see the other person as being similar to us.
But when we see people as different from us,
we tend to fall back on a different mental error
that produces its own form of misattribution.
Can you talk about how stereotypes
might be a form of misattribution?
Stereotypes are one of the biggest sources of information
we rely on when it comes to judging other people,
especially if we don't know them personally.
So this influences person perception
and accuracy in particular,
because these expectations just serve as a lens
through which we perceive and attend to
all the information around us.
If we expect it to happen,
we're more likely to attend to information
that's consistent with those expectations.
We forget the stuff that's not consistent
and we completely write it off.
You know, don't pay attention to it at all or forget it completely.
The problem is not limited to our errors in perception.
We constantly give off inaccurate signals to other people, making it more likely,
they will read a strong.
to other people, making it more likely, they will read a strong. In one study, Tessa found people went out of their way to communicate the opposite of
what they were really thinking.
We found in this study, so we brought people in, we had them do a negotiation task, there
was a winner and a loser of the negotiation, and we had them give feedback to each other
on how they performed, what they could do better.
And we manipulated whether that feedback was asked for or not.
And what we found was when you're giving feedback to someone that they didn't ask for,
people actually smiled more, they were friendly and they're delivery,
and despite the fact that they're often talking to someone who just lost to them,
the only thing they could come up with were great things that they did well in the negotiation.
So even in context where constructive feedback makes the most sense, they're like, it was
wonderful when you did XYZ.
So they're intentionally giving information to the world that isn't reflecting what they're
actually thinking.
It's like this deception in an effort to create rapport, to build a relationship with
someone.
Tessa and other researchers have also found that power dynamic shape, whether we accurately
reveal what is happening inside our minds.
Low-power people tend to mask what they are thinking and feeling.
High-power people are more likely to broadcast their internal thoughts.
Things get flipped when it comes to perception.
People with less power are better at reading the mind states of the powerful.
Children, for example, often have their parents number.
Absolutely.
My son has figured out that if I'm typing on my phone is the best time to ask to purchase
some Pokemon card on Amazon.
And that, you know, and if I'm making dinner, it is a bad time, because I hate making dinner, and I'm in a bad
mood.
So, yes, kids learn these things, you know, kids in classrooms learn these things.
I think people pick up on these cues when they're outcome dependent upon others.
Some of the examples we've discussed can sound humorous, but errors in mind reading can
have serious consequences.
A black patient who feels a doctor is being inattentive or hostile might be less likely to reveal a medical
problem. A doctor who falls back on stereotypes can miss
something important. Tessa has also found that mistakes in
reading the minds of others leads to worse performance in
teams. So there is something kind of unique and special about
your ability to read, you to read how people are relating
to one another on teams, and that ability is distinctly related to your ability and your
own teams to perform well, and also to not have status conflict, to not be jockeying for
status with another person, to know when it's time to step back, and when it's time that
you can actually take over a position of power.
And we have found that this ability to read the room predicts how well you do on a creativity
task.
It also predicts what we call status conflict.
So this is the idea that people were fighting the entire time.
We couldn't figure out who should be in charge.
No one really agreed at the end of the day who's in charge, what people's roles are.
And I think what's interesting about this is,
you know, status hierarchy's often
get a bad reputation.
But figuring those out, conferring status really early on
is actually critical for group success.
And this is, you know, the ability to read the room
is directly related to the ability to just get that process
over with, move on with things, and then work well together.
And we also found that there's a bad apple effect.
So if you put one person on a team who's really bad at this, they can completely disrupt
the group process.
They're interrupting the wrong people.
You know, they're taking over when someone important is saying something.
And so even just one person who's bad at this kind of person perception skills can completely
throw off and disrupt group processes.
When we come back, the magic trick to discovering what is really going on in someone else's mind.
You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedantam. This is Hidden Brain.
I'm Shankar Vedantam.
Tessa West is a psychologist at New York University.
She studies the benefits of interpersonal accuracy, coming up with the right attributions for other
people's behavior.
Tessa and other researchers have tested a variety of techniques to induce people to become more accurate as
they read other people's minds.
Testar figure that one reason people might be bad at reading other minds is because they're
not motivated to do a good job.
She thought, why don't we fix that?
She offered volunteers as much as $100 to accurately guess what their romantic partners
were thinking. If motivation was the problem, cold, hard cash ought to correct the problem.
We found that by and large, it doesn't really work. So this was our attempt at really testing this
idea that, you know, inaccuracy is really about motivation. And if you give people money, you can get them to be right.
There's a little bit of evidence to support it, but nothing that I would be too confident in.
The fact that monetary incentives fail to improve people's accuracy in reading the minds
of their romantic partners, told Tesla something important.
A lack of motivation might not be the problem.
So you can put people in an experiment where they're not motivated to be accurate, they
have no reason to care, and you can give them money and that basically just makes them
pay more attention. But I think in the real world, in naturalistic interactions, we're
all motivated to be accurate. We all actually care. You know, there's often a lot on the
line. And so offering that incentive just doesn't really get us there.
So another intervention has involved asking volunteers to engage in perspective taking,
to imagine putting themselves in other people's shoes as a way of getting a better understanding
of what's happening in the minds of other people.
How do these experiments turn out and what do they find, Tessa?
You know, the shorter answer is they don't work.
What they do is they actually increase your confidence in your ability to read people.
And they can reduce that egocentric bias we talked about.
They do not increase accuracy because they're not actually giving you any information
that you can use to make judgments.
And in some cases, they actually backfire.
So if you're engaging in an interaction with someone from a different race, a different culture,
and I tell you to perspective take, what that can do is make you feel very anxious
and uncomfortable.
Are they going to think I'm prejudiced or racist or uncomfortable around them?
It actually leads you to be even more egocentric and in your head than if I hadn't given you
that manipulation at all.
So here we are again with another intuitively interesting potential manipulation that just simply doesn't work
and often actually leads people to be less accurate.
I mean, that's kind of astonishing, isn't it,
that we still hear the admonishment
to take other people's perspectives
as the cure to mistreating their minds?
I mean, that advice, I think, is still pretty widespread.
It's very widespread.
You hear it from everything from romantic relationships
to improving workplace relationships
and intergroup contact, but it does nothing for accuracy.
In a third approach to try and boost people's accuracy in terms of their attributions, you offer study subjects, alternative explanations for the behavior of their conversation partner. So this was the idea that if you give people benign explanations for
other people's behavior, that will allow people to settle into the interaction, make fewer
mind-reading errors, generally get along better. Was your hope born out?
It was not. This was one of the more frustrating academic experiences I had. I woke up in
the middle of the night and I thought I had this brilliant idea, which was this.
We know that anxiety can be very disruptive to interracial interactions.
And one of the reasons why is when we're interacting with someone who comes across as anxious,
we think it's because they don't like us because of our race.
So therefore, the solution might be, well, let's just fix that part.
Let's give them an attribution for anxiety that has nothing to do with race.
So what we did was we brought people in and we told them that their partner was a little
bit jittery because they had a couple cups of coffee too much that day and that can actually
lead them to appear anxious and uncomfortable. They then interacted with someone who was
the same race or a different race than them and we are convinced that this information
and interracial interaction would lead people to be more engaged and more accurate
and mind reading and all of these things.
And when I came to analyze the data,
I got this huge effect.
It was one of the biggest effects I've ever gotten
in the opposite direction.
So it actually made things much worse.
The minute we told them anxiety was at play,
that's all they could think about.
And they actually ended up seeing anxiety
in their partner that wasn't even there.
So that attribution just heightened the attention to anxious related cues.
So imagine walking into a job interview and you say to the person, I'm sorry, I'm a little
bit fidgety right now, but I just had like too much coffee this morning.
You might think, okay, well, that's going to make them think I'm actually comfortable.
All they're thinking now is great.
I'm interacting with a super anxious person and they they're only gonna attend to your anxiety-related cues
into nothing else.
You know, in popular culture,
people have long believed that there are ways
to read other people's body language.
Can you talk about this idea,
the enduring popularity of reading people's body language
to understand what's happening in their minds,
and what science has found about the accuracy of this technique.
You know, there's so much intuitive appeal to if I could just learn the behaviors, the
cues I could figure out what's going on in people's minds.
And I think, sure, there are some shared cues.
If I'm smiling, I'm probably happy.
But by and large, I think people behave really idiosyncratically.
What I look like as hungry, another person looks like as upset or angry, you know, and really
just trying to learn generalized cues to read people's behaviors never really works because
it's a lot like lie detection.
Individuals might have tells, but there's no such thing as a shared tell what everybody
does when they're lying.
And the same thing is true with human mind reading and perception and behavior.
There's just no shared set of behaviors everybody does when they're feeling angry or happy
or irritated and so forth.
So we're really looking at a litany of failures here, Tess.
If you've mentioned several different techniques to try and get people to be more accurate, have
you found anything that does help us accurately get inside the minds of other people?
We have.
So the good news is we're not all doomed.
I think, you know, social science has really been in the business of understanding how
can we change the way people are thinking to improve accuracy?
It's really putting the burden on the perceiver to become more accurate.
But what we actually know from social science is we need to stop trying to read people and
actually get the world to tell you what it thinks and feels.
Don't try to read the world.
And so the manipulations that work are all about clarifying the information that other people
are giving to you, seeking that information out. I think Nick Eppley and his work talks about
perspective getting, explicitly asking people,
what are your thoughts and feelings?
Tell me about your opinions.
And this seems sort of overly obvious and intuitive,
but people just simply don't do it.
Instead, they focus on what's going on in their own heads,
but really if you can get the world to tell you
what it's thinking and feeling,
that's your best bet at improving your interpersonal accuracy.
So you're saying the best way of figuring out what's inside someone else's mind is to
ask them?
That's right.
And you know, it's hard.
We have a lot of reasons why it's hard, but asking is really the only thing we found
that truly works to improve in a personal accuracy.
So, in some ways, that advice is sort of so comically obvious, but many of us don't realize
it's the most effective way to understand what's happening inside other people's heads.
And surely some of this is because we widely overestimate our ability to read other people's minds.
We don't believe that we actually need to to ask them we can just watch
you know how they're crossing their arms or we can infer from you know how they look at us in an
elevator what's happening in their minds or we can deduce from how they behave in an interview
what they really think of us that we are so confident about our mind reading abilities that we don't do the most obvious thing.
That's right. I think you think people tend to be very confident,
but unfortunately, their confidence and their ability to mind read
is correlated almost zero with their actual ability to mind read.
They're rarely actually getting the feedback
that these two things are completely misaligned.
Is it possible that our overconfidence in our ability
is to read other people's minds is exaggerated
when it comes to people who
are close to us.
In other words, if I know my partner, my spouse, my parents, if I know these people very
well, it almost seems as if, well, I should know what's happening inside their minds because
I'm so familiar with them.
You know, one of the funnest findings, I think, in social science is how in marriages, we're
actually the most accurate at the newlywed stage.
And as your marriage progresses by, you know, your later years,
you tend to actually just completely
base your judgments of that person on yourself
because you are so confident.
And you stop paying attention.
And then you end up in these conversations of like,
I always thought you loved Lobster and your partners.
Like, I haven't liked Lobster in 30 years.
You should know that.
But this is common, we're overconfident,
we can get a little bit lazy as relationships progress.
If the advice to ask people what they are thinking
instead of trying to guess what they are thinking
sounds comically obvious,
there are some specific techniques that Tessa and others
have found that are less obvious.
The first is, if you want to know why someone did
something or said something, don't wait for a week or a month to ask them.
Yeah, I think this is a common approach a lot of us use and I think, you know,
that the main issue here is that memories are fallible. We can barely even remember
what we had for lunch yesterday, let alone what we were thinking about, you know,
weeks and sometimes months later. And so you're just not going to get accurate information when you're using
fallible memories plus people just filling in the gaps with current information
that wasn't relevant to the event of the past.
Can you also talk about the difference between asking global questions and specific questions
because it turns out this is another error we make when we ask people what's going on inside their minds. Yeah, I think most of us have this intuition that if we ask
someone something global, we're going to get more information from them. So for instance, I could
ask you, you know, do you trust me? Are you happy in this relationship? Or, you know, do you like it
when we, you know, go to movies? Or do you like it when we go to movies?
Or do you like it when we order Thai food?
The latter two are very specific questions.
The former are very global.
It's very hard for us to get accurate answers about those things.
We don't even know what the person who's answering that question
is basing their answer on, for instance,
because it's so global, it's so kind of nebulous in general.
It's very
tough for us to kind of learn how to read people when we're just asking these very kind of
non-specific questions. So instead of asking a question like, do you love me? Maybe it's more
effective to say, tell me about the time we went on this date and you know whether, what is it
about this date that you actually enjoyed? That's right. What about this date? Did you enjoy,
you know, don't ask people whether you're good and bad, ask what about you in bed? They about this date that you actually enjoyed. That's right. What about this date did you enjoy?
Don't ask people whether you're good and bad.
Ask what about you in bed they like or dislike
and what about that you're cooking.
They like or dislike specifics are better.
And I think if we go back to the previous finding
that we tend to give people positive feedback
when we're uncomfortable, the more global the feedback,
the more positive it's going to be.
And that, by definition, is just going to be biased towards inaccurate feedback,
especially when you want, you know, even critical information from someone.
Can you talk about the idea that besides asking questions of other people to elicit what's
happening inside their minds, we should go to some lengths to become clearer in what's going on inside our minds.
So people are trying to read us, we're often giving model signals to other people.
If we want better communication all the way around, it's not just that we should get better
at reading other people's minds, we should make our signals clearer so that other people
can read our minds better.
That's right.
We often assume that we have a transparency bias.
We assume that what's going on in our thoughts,
what's going on in our emotions, our minds and so forth,
is just readily apparent to people and it's not.
And I've actually had to be extremely explicit
in what I'm thinking and feeling.
And I learned this in the classroom, in the pandemic,
that if I don't tell people exactly
what I'm thinking at that moment, they're not going to know they don't have access to
cues.
If I'll awkward at first, but I think telling people, I'm sorry, I'm not frustrated with
you, I'm frustrated with my computer, or that side didn't mean I'm irritated with you,
that side meant I'm a little bit tired or whatever, I think those little kinds of pieces
of information, people can pick up on and learn how to read you more accurately in the future.
So those are really critical to express not just to perceive in others.
And I'm wondering during the pandemic when so many of us were wearing masks, whether it became even harder now to pick up on cues from other people and whether we needed to go to even greater lengths to actually clarify what was going on inside our heads.
whether we needed to go to even greater lengths to actually clarify what was going on inside our heads.
Absolutely, I think a lot of us lost those,
nonverbal behaviors that were used to relying on.
We couldn't tell if someone was smiling under there
or frowning, and now that I've done
this sort of explicit thing and I've taken the mask off,
I'm still doing it, and I've realized there's utility
to it even when those cues are available.
And after the elevator, I've learned that my face doesn't reflect what I'm thinking anyway.
So I should always be super clear with people what I'm thinking and feeling.
You know, I'm wondering, Tessa, that, you know, when I think about people who are on the autism spectrum, for example,
these are people who sometimes have trouble reading the minds of others and have to learn with care and deliberation
how to understand others' intentions, how to communicate their own.
But as this conversation unfolded, I've really come to think that maybe all of us might benefit in some ways from some of the skills training that people who are on the autism spectrum receive.
I absolutely think that's true. I think kind of what's fascinating about people who are diagnosed on the spectrum
is the minute they receive that diagnosis,
they're sort of told that they're not good
at giving off signals, they're not good at reading signals.
And so they have to be very deliberative
in expressing what they're thinking and feeling
and also asking information from people.
And I think because they're getting that skills training,
you know, really out of a fear
that they're going to get these social interactions wrong, they get much better at this and you know they're much more rehearsed with it
than people who aren't diagnosed who are neurotypical even though I'm with you
100% I think we could actually all benefit from some of this training.
I'm wondering if there are limits to this advice of trying to be explicit and
trying to ask people explicitly about what's happening
inside their minds. Are there situations and relationships, for example, as people are
getting to know one another or in work situations where in some ways it might be inappropriate or
problematic to make things too explicit?
Yes. As you join a new workplace, for instance, there are norms about things you can and can't
ask about. And I think there's certainly social situations where you're just simply not allowed because it's
non-normative to ask someone what their behavior means. I often think about this in the doctor
patient interaction context where if a patient was to ask a physician, do you just not trust me?
Something like that, they would get a very strange response back.
So I do think there's a little bit of a learning curve
for figuring out how and when to ask
these kinds of questions.
And I think the advice I give people
is be very careful about things like over disclosure
or asking about people's personal lives
and context for them are not appropriate.
Specific feedback tends to be context-specific,
it tends to not be inappropriate.
So the more that you can kind of tailor the question
to the moment, the less likely you are
to kind of step into uncomfortable grounds.
Are there situations where you think
leaping to conclusions about people
could actually be good for us?
I mean, there's some research that suggests that when people have, you know,
overly optimistic or positive views of their romantic partners, for example,
they might end up in happier relationships than people who have more realistic
perspectives of their partners.
Are misattributions always a problem?
The question isn't, is accuracy good or bad?
It's what type of accuracy is good and bad and when.
And I think what we actually know there is that you want to have
these global positive judgments of your partner.
You think your partner is really attractive and fun and interesting.
We call this the rose colored glasses effect,
but you want specific accuracy.
So you want to be able to tell for instance that your partner is
annoyed with you for not emptying the dishwasher,
or that they're too tired to go to the movie tonight.
You can read those cues.
So the specific things you want to be accurate about,
but at a global level,
you actually want to be overly biased
in a positive way.
You want to see them more positively
than others see that person.
I want to return to that interaction we spoke about earlier, the one where your NYU colleague
was abruptly dismissive of you and this was behavior that you initially attribute it
to his feeling superior to you.
You were unexpectedly thrown together when you were asked to co-teach a professional development
course.
How did you get along given your Rocky start, Tessa?
You know, so Javon Bable, who the story is about, he and I taught a professional
development course when after several years of not really interacting with
each other, we both had to kind of step in last minute and find a course to
teach. And I was like, oh, this is gonna be awful. He's this guy. He's such a
social climber. And I immediately realized that I was wrong.
My perceptions of him, my reading of him was wrong.
He was actually super friendly and super engaging.
And happy to engage with me with this course.
And we immediately became very close friends.
But I had hung on to those wrong perceptions
for many years before that moment.
So you teach this course together and you get along famously and often sometimes you realize
that you actually like each other quite a bit and you might feel that even something might
be brewing between the two of you, but your colleagues in the same department, so this is
a high-risk proposition, I understand that the two of you sat down and had a heart to
heart.
Can you tell me about that conversation, how it went, what he said, and what you said
in response?
You know, one thing I really learned in this experience is when you decide to date a
colleague whose office is two doors down from yours, and you both have ten years at the
same institution, and you're likely to stay there forever.
It behooves you to learn how to read each other accurately.
And I obviously didn't have a
great history with him and when it came to person perception. So we sat down together and he said,
is it just me or is there something going on between us? Is there feelings between us? And I can
remember seeing his handshake, he was kind of ringing his palms from the sweat and he was so
uncomfortable, but it was really critical for him palms from the sweat and he was so uncomfortable, but
it was really critical for him to get this right and to not assume that, you know, his
colleague had feelings for him that I didn't have.
And so we had a very blatant conversation about explicit feelings for each other.
And I think that's a funny thing to do with someone that you're working with and you're
not dating to just have this very explicit, let me tell you exactly when I'm thinking
in feeling conversation, over lunch and just hoping
it doesn't completely blow up.
And it went well, but it was anxiety provoking.
I understand that you had both been married previously
and you decided that you were gonna have a very candid
conversation about what it meant to get into a relationship.
What kinds of things did you discuss, Tessa?
You know, we gave each other a list of 100 questions
and we being scientists had this kind of
null hypothesis testing approach
where we assumed we were not compatible
and we had to prove ourselves otherwise.
But, you know, we asked each other everything.
Nothing was off the table.
You know, what are your 401K plans?
Where do you like to go on vacation? What are, you know, we both had small children. What are your 401K plans? Where do you like to go on vacation?
We both had small children. What are your parenting strategies? How do you handle the following
potential conflicts? What is your relationship like with your ex? How are we going to
communicate this to colleagues? All the tough stuff, there was no stone left unturned,
where we wanted to prove to ourselves that we were in the compatible before we even started dating because the stakes were so high.
So you had this very detailed, I don't know if the right word is conversation or interrogation,
but you had this very detailed interaction before, really, you became romantically entangled.
Before we even held hands, you know, before even testing the chemistry part out,
honestly. But we had to, the accuracy in center was so high. And we both were just very honest
and didn't lie and didn't say the thing we thought the other person wanted to hear, because
if the relationship blew up, it would have been very bad for a lot of reasons. We would have
been miserable at work for 30 years.
I mean, in some ways, it's a testament to what you've been describing in terms of the arc of your
research career, which is the value of actually being really explicit and really specific and
putting your cards on the table. So what was the upshot of this very deliberate effort to have
this conversation up front? How did things go? We had the conversation and now we're married.
So, the upshot I think for us, aside from it being a success
and our families really merging well
and us being very much in love,
was that it set the stage for everything else
in the relationships.
So, people often don't want to be good targets.
They don't want to be honest because they're afraid of what it's going to do the relationship.
They want to have positive perceptions of the partner so they don't ask negative things.
But we got all of that off the table from the get go and we're extremely honest with each
other.
And full disclosure, I talked to Jay.
It length about this before I discussed it with you just to make sure he was comfortable.
So we're always just keeping each other in the loop. And the know, the other thing I know is I'm not a great person
perceiver and neither is he, but I know how to be a good target and I know how to elicit that
information from other people. And I think that really has an upside in relationships.
Tessa West is a psychologist at NYU. She's also the author of the book, Jerks at Work,
toxic co-workers and what to do about them. Tessa, thank you so much for joining me today
on Hidden Brain. Thank you so much.
Hidden Brain is produced by Hidden Brain Media. Our audio production team includes
Bridget McCarthy, Annie Murphy-Paul, Kristen Wong, Laura Quarelle, Ryan Katz, Autumn Barnes,
and Andrew Chadwick. Tara Boyle is our executive producer. I'm Hidden Brain's executive editor.
For today's on-sunk hero, we turn the mic over to you, our listeners.
It's a story from our show, My Ansan Hero.
Today's My Ansan Hero is brought to you by Anstar.
Onstar advisors are now with you everywhere, on the app, in your car, and at home.
Anstar, be safe out there.
Our story comes from Joe Arigoni. Joe and Joni have been married for more than 30 years.
Three of their four kids are out of the house and they are always imagined spending this
phase of life working and planning for retirement. But all that changed at the end of 2018 when
Joni was diagnosed with early onset Alzheimer's at just 51, her condition has been devastating.
And for Joe, it has tested the limits of his compassion and understanding.
He remembers one morning that was especially frustrating.
It was in March of 2022.
Joni was insisting that she needed new shoes, even though she did not need them.
So after he got tired of arguing about it with her,
he agreed to take her to the shoe store.
And I'm like, okay, here, here's all the shoes here.
There's, you know, aisles and aisles and shoes.
You know, which ones do you like?
Is there a particular color or a particular brand?
And I don't think she really understood.
And she goes, I want to talk to an associate.
And so I'm like, all right, fine,
you just go find an associate.
I'm just gonna sit down over here
because this whole knee that you have for shoes,
I just need a break from it.
So she starts wandering off and going around the store.
I don't see her then for a while, and I finally hear her voice, and she's talking to the
salesperson that she found.
And I peek around the corner, and I see them engage in this conversation. And the sales woman, Michelle,
is trying to figure out what size of shoe Joni needs.
She's gently helping to get the shoe off her foot
and then asks her to place her foot
in the little metal slide that they use
to get the width in the size.
And because my wife has a hard time following directions because of her illness,
she puts her hand down on the measuring tool for feet.
And Michelle says, that's okay. Don't worry. Why don't you just stand up and I'll put the tool
underneath and we'll measure your foot that way. And Michelle asks her, do you have some anxiety?
She says, because I have some anxiety. And I also have autism.
And when I heard that,
it just broke my heart
because here I couldn't generate the patience
and the compassion.
And here this was the salesperson
who my expectation was that, you know, they're not
going to be able to understand Joni.
They're not going to be able to engage with Joni, and I'm going to have to step in and
do everything anyway.
And they had just created this beautiful moment and still chokes me up a little bit.
When you're a full-time caregiver, 24-7, sometimes I can take a toll on you and your level of compassion or hope can get depleted. And so you can be
just desperate for some type of relief from that responsibility. And when someone who
themselves already has a difficulty navigating our world is caring for your loved one
with more patience and compassion than you can bustle at that moment. It's doubly impactful,
it's beyond words and it's a beautiful thing.
thing. Joe Erigone of Orland Hills, Illinois.
By the way, they did find Joni a pair of shoes, and Joe was able to go back into the store
and tell Michelle, thank you.
This segment was brought to you by OnStar.
OnStar believes everyone has the right to feel safe everywhere. That's why
their emergency advisors are now available to help not only in the car but wherever you are.
On your phone, in your car and at home. OnStar, be safe out there.
If you like this episode and would like us to produce more shows like this, please
consider supporting our work. Go to support.hiddenbrain.org
Again, if you would like to help support the show you love, go to support.hiddenbrain.org
I'm Shankar Vedantam. See you soon.
you