Hidden Brain - Innovation 2.0: The Influence You Have
Episode Date: May 13, 2024Think about the last time you asked someone for something. Maybe you were nervous or worried about what the person would think of you. Chances are that you didn’t stop to think about the pressure yo...u were exerting on that person. This week, we continue our Innovation 2.0 series with a look at a phenomenon that psychologist Vanessa Bohns refers to as “egocentric bias.” We explore how this bias leads us astray, and how we can use this knowledge to ask for the things we need. Did you catch the first two episodes in our Innovation 2.0 series? You can find them in this podcast feed or on our website. And if you're enjoying this series, please share it with a friend or family member. Thanks!
Transcript
Discussion (0)
This is Hidden Brain.
I'm Shankar Vedanta.
Philip Zimbardo grew up poor in New York City,
in the South Bronx.
As he went to school and played in his neighborhood,
he noticed something.
There were lots of ways for kids from poor families
to get into trouble.
One of the things about growing up poor
is you're surrounded by evil,
meaning people whose job it is to get good kids
to do bad things for money.
And even as a little kid, I was always curious about why some kids got seduced and other
kids like me were able to resist.
Was some kid smarter?
Tougher?
Lots of people might draw such conclusions, but from an early age, Phil found himself
interested in another explanation.
The context in which a good kid would do something bad.
The situation.
At school, James Monroe High School, also in the Bronx, Phil got close to a classmate
who was interested in the same questions.
And it was a little Jewish kid named Stanley Milgram.
We were in the same class.
We sat side by side.
He was the smartest kid in the class.
He won all the medals at graduation
So obviously nobody liked him because we were all envious of him, but he was super smart and super serious
If you know anything about psychology
You will know that these teenagers went on to become two of the most influential psychologists in history
Phil became famous for conducting the
Stanford Prison Experiment where he turned the university's psychology
department into a makeshift prison. Stanley Milgram made his mark with a
study that examined the power of situations to seduce good people to do
bad things. It involved asking a volunteer to administer a memory test to
another person. If the answers were wrong, the volunteer was volunteer to administer a memory test to another person.
If the answers were wrong, the volunteer was told to deliver a series of electrical shocks
as punishment.
The study has invited a great deal of admiration and a great deal of criticism over the years.
We're going to begin today's show by taking you through this famous experiment.
As you listen, pay attention to how you're responding to the scene that unfolds,
what you think about the different characters, and how you relate to them.
Once it's done, we're going to talk with a psychologist who realized
that most people overlook something in the experiment.
We so often sort of simulate, if I was in that Milgram Shock experiment,
what would I do if I was the the study participant, right?
Would I actually stand up and go against these directives and say no, but we kind of flipped that idea on its head
Today on the show we continue our innovation 2.0 series by looking at the underappreciated power we exert on
others and how this knowledge can transform our relationships both at work and in our
personal lives.
Flipping the script, Japanese attack on Pearl Harbor.
1941, the U.S. was swept up in the conflict.
Stanley Milgram grew up in a world that seemed bent on destroying itself.
World War II was raging in Europe and Asia, and by the time he was eight, the U.S. was swept up in the conflict.
We interrupt this broadcast to bring you this important bulletin from the United Press.
The White House announces Japanese attack on Pearl Harbor.
1941, a date which will live in infamy.
By tomorrow morning, the members of Congress will have a fore-report and be ready for action.
The fields of battle were far from Stanley's home,
but as he grew older, he couldn't stop thinking about the war and its implications.
Stanley was consumed by some big questions.
Why did so many people willingly kill Jews in the Holocaust?
Was everyone who followed Nazi orders inherently evil?
Here he is in an educational film.
How is it possible, I ask myself, that ordinary people, who are courteous and decent in everyday life,
can act callously, inhumanely,
without any limitations of conscience?
Phil Zimbardo remembers his classmate
asking those same questions at James Monroe High School.
As a high school student, he was worried
that the Holocaust could happen again in America.
And everybody said, Stanley, that was Nazi Germany,
that was then, we're not that kind of people.
And he would say, I'll bet they thought the same thing.
And the bottom line, he says, how do you know
how you would act unless you're in the situation?
How do you know how you would act
unless you're in the situation?
Stanley's theory was that the context
that people found themselves in shaped their behavior.
This went for Nazis, but it went for ordinary people too.
Most of us never get to find out if we will behave like Nazis
because most of us never find ourselves in situations
where we are asked to behave like Nazis.
By the early 1960s, as a psychology professor at Yale, Stanley decided to test this idea.
Stanley wanted to put volunteers in a situation where they would be asked to do something
that was clearly wrong. Would they do it? Follow instructions? Obey orders?
He came up with a scenario that was simple, ingenious, and wildly controversial.
An experimenter wearing a lab coat invited volunteers into a room.
The volunteers were told they were part of a study about learning and memory.
Some would play the role of teacher, while others would play a student.
What you're going to hear next is a recreation of the study using voice actors. The dialogue
is drawn from a 1962 documentary that describes the experiment.
Before we begin, we should know that some listeners may find this section upsetting
because it involves descriptions of someone inflicting pain
on another person. Also, there are two liberties we've taken in this recreation.
First, in the real version of this experiment, the student responded to the
teacher's questions
by silently flipping a switch. We've given voice to those actions.
Second, we've imagined the internal monologues
of some of the people in the experiment.
Those inner voices sound different
from the things they say aloud,
and you'll hear them both throughout the scene.
So, that's the setup for the experiment.
Remember, there was an experimenter and two volunteers, one playing the role of teacher and the other playing the role
of student. The experimenter began by explaining the purpose of the memory
test. We want to find out just what effect different people have on each
other as teachers and learners and also what effect punishment will have on
learning in this situation. The experimenter told the person playing the role of student
to sit in a chair. The experimenter strapped down the student's arms
and attached an electrode to his wrist. The electrode, the student was told, was
connected to a shock generator. Then the
experimenter explained, the teacher will read a list of word pairs to you
like these.
Blue, girl, nice day, fat neck, and so forth.
You are to try to remember each pair.
For the next time through, the teacher will read only the first word,
or the first half of the word pair.
The student was asked to remember the second half of the word pair.
The experimenter made sure to ask,
Do you have any questions now before we go to the next room?
No, but I think I should say this. About two years ago I was at the
Veterans Hospital in West Haven and while there they detected a heart
condition. There's nothing serious, but as long as
I'm having these shocks, how strong are they? How dangerous are
they? Well no, although they may be painful, they're not dangerous. Next, the experimenter ushered the volunteer playing the role of teacher
into another room. He gave him a set of instructions.
You will read each pair of words in this list once to the learner
until you've read the entire list. Direct your voice toward the microphone
as the rooms are only partly soundproof. Now, if he gives the correct answer,
you say correct and go on to the next line. The correct answer is...
Okay, here goes.
...indicated in the right margin.
I see. All right. Looks easy enough.
The experiment got underway.
Attention, learner. Your teacher is about to begin the test. Try and remember the word
pairs. Ready? Begin.
Blue. Girl.
Right so far.
Nice. I think day. Fat. But when Right so far. Nice. Um, I think day.
Fat.
But when the person in the other room made a mistake...
Fat. Was it hat? No. Wet.
The volunteer playing the role of teacher would tell the learner that he was wrong.
As punishment, he would administer an electric jolt.
Incorrect. You'll now get a shock of 75 volts.
Oh!
Seems nervous.
The experimenter in the lab coat, meanwhile, was observing the process.
Please continue.
Cool.
Okay, I'm pretty sure it's day.
Wrong. It's head. 105 volts.
Ow!
Come on, get it right. I don't want to shock you.
Teacher, please continue.
What should I do?
Boat.
Grass. Wrong. Teacher, please continue. What should I do? Boat. Grass.
Wrong.
135 volts.
Answer.
The students trapped to the chair in the other room kept making mistakes.
Each time, the experimenter urged the volunteer playing the role of teacher to keep going,
to administer a stronger jolt of electricity.
Wrong.
It's harsh.
150 volts.
Ow! Experimenter, get me out of here. 150 volts ow experiment or get me out of
here that's all he wants to quit get me out of here I told you I had heart
trouble my heart is starting to bother me now get me out of here please my heart
teacher please go on let me out you refuse to go on you want me to keep
going experiment requires you to continue teacher please continue the I don't want to do this anymore. You're gonna stop. You're gonna stop. You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop.
You're gonna stop. You're gonna stop. You're gonna stop. You're gonna stop. You're gonna stop. And so did the protests coming from the next room. He's got a heart condition in there.
I'm gonna go.
Your choice.
Please continue.
Now, I got a shock.
180 volts.
Ow!
I can't stand the pain.
He can't stand it.
Let me out of here.
I'm not going to kill a man.
So are you going to keep shocking him?
Poor guy.
He's in there screaming.
I said before, the shocks may be painful, but they're not dangerous.
But he's in there hollering. He can't stand it.
What if something happens to him?
You don't have to keep going.
The volunteer being asked to administer electric shocks
is in a difficult position.
The experimenter is urging him to continue,
even as the person in the next room begs to be spared.
Should he keep going or stop?
The experiment requires that you continue, teacher.
Whether the learner likes it or not, we must go on until he's learned all the word pairs.
I refuse to take responsibility of him getting hurt.
So you don't want the responsibility?
It's absolutely essential that you continue, teacher.
There is too many of them left. Please, go on.
Who's going to take the responsibility if anything happens to the gentleman?
I'm responsible for anything that happens here.
Please continue.
As the experiment progressed, the memory test became more demanding.
Next one.
Slow, walk, dance, truck, music.
Come on.
Answer please.
Come on, get this right.
I know it's for science, but I don't want to hurt you.
Wrong.
195 bucks.
Ow!
Let me out of here. Continue please. Let me out of here. I don't know to hurt you. Wrong. 195 volts. Ow! Let me out of here!
Continue please.
I don't know.
The experiment requires...
I know it does sir, but I mean you do know what he's getting in for.
He's up to 195 volts.
Yeah, 195.
That's pretty high.
After the study reached about 330 volts, the screams from the next room went silent.
If the learner doesn't answer in a reasonable time, about four or five seconds, consider the answer wrong. And follow the same procedures you had been doing for wrong answers.
Say wrong, tell them the number of vaults, give him the punishment.
Go on please with the experiment. Please continue.
Is he breaking?
Soft, rug, pillow, hair, grass.
Answer, please.
Go on, teacher.
360 volts.
I think something's happened to that fellow in there.
I didn't get no answer.
He was hollering at less voltage.
Can you check to see if he's all's alright please? Not once we've started. Please continue teacher.
In all, Stanley Milgram ran about 20 different iterations of this study over a span of several
years. In this version, many of the volunteers playing the role of teacher showed discomfort,
but continued with the experiment. More than half went
all the way to 450 volts even when the screams from the next room went silent
and the student was presumably unconscious. Why didn't the volunteer stop?
Stanley later debriefs some of the volunteers.
I did stop but he kept going and keep going.
But why didn't you just disregard what he said? He says it's got to go on the experiment.
If you're familiar with the study, you already know that the student in the other room was an actor
and not actually given electric shocks. The screams and cries of protest were carefully timed recordings.
The only target of the experiment were the volunteers
who played the role of teacher,
the people who had to administer the shocks.
Stanley Milgram's study generated enormous attention
and controversy.
Admires drew parallels between the experiment
and what happened in Nazi Germany.
They said, look, people are sheep. admirers drew parallels between the experiment and what happened in Nazi Germany.
They said, look, people are sheep.
They can be easily misled by demagogues and dictators.
Critics of the study said, no, those conclusions are vastly exaggerated.
They questioned whether the volunteers actually behaved the way the experiment suggested.
Some critics said that many volunteers
simply refused to go along.
Beyond the academic debates,
the study prompted an entire sub-genre
of books and movies.
Even today, people find the study fascinating,
and they find it fascinating for one reason.
How, they ask, could people who know
that something is wrong go along with it?
Are such people typical? Is everyone susceptible to such influence? Am I?
As we listen to the details of the study, we can't help but ask, what would I do?
Would I follow orders and zap the person screaming in the other room?
But Vanessa Bonds, a psychologist at Cornell University, realized there was something no
one was paying attention to.
Everyone was asking what was going on in the minds of the volunteers and how difficult
the situation was for them.
We so often sort of simulate if I was in that Milgram shock experiment, what would I do
if I was the study participant, right?
Would I actually stand up and go against these directives and say no?
No one was asking whether it was difficult for the experimenter wearing the lab coat
to tell the volunteers to administer electric shocks.
To the extent we think of the experimenter at all, we might imagine someone who enjoyed
putting people in difficult situations, a sort of mad scientist.
Vanessa asked a deceptively simple question.
Was he surprised to see these people going along with this crazy request he was making
of them?
Vanessa's insight was radical. What if you looked at the experiment, not from the
point of view of the students screaming in the next room, and not from the point
of view of the volunteer administering the shocks, but from the point of view of
the person giving the instructions?
Teacher, please continue.
Is he going to keep going?
What if you treated the experimenter as the object of study once to quit? Please continue. I said before the shocks may be painful, but they're not.
So are you going to keep shocking him?
So stop. You don't have to keep going.
Why do so few of us put ourselves in the shoes of the experimenter?
Why don't we ask how difficult it was for him to
issue those instructions? Why is it when we hear the story we automatically put
ourselves in the shoes of the volunteers, the people receiving the instructions?
Vanessa realized that we all naturally gravitate to the point of view of the
volunteers and not the point of view of the experimenter because we all
instinctively
know what it feels like to have other people put us in uncomfortable situations.
We think of our bosses, our partners, our co-workers, and how they affect our lives
and change our moods.
We think of the aggressive driver next to us, or the other patrons at the restaurant
who are so loud and obnoxious that they ruin our meal.
We feel buffeted and pushed and pulled by those around us.
The one thing we don't ask?
What effect do I have on other people?
There's been a long history of research on social influence and persuasion, so we do know a lot about
how other people influence us, but we don't know so much about how we experience our influence of other people.
When we are intensely focused on how the world affects us and not how we affect
the world, this can have profound consequences for both good and evil.
for both good and evil.
You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta.
This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. When Vanessa Barnes was a graduate student at Columbia University, she walked on a study.
Every day, she would leave her apartment in the Morningside Heights neighborhood
and take the subway from 116th Street to Penn Station.
Once she was there, she had to do something she found very difficult.
Basically, I would just go up to random people in Penn Station, say,
hey, will you fill out this questionnaire?
Vanessa no longer remembers what the questionnaire was about,
but she can still recall what it felt like to make such requests of total strangers.
Yeah, I mean, I still have flashbacks of going down to Penn Station
because it was so distressing.
I would walk in, there'd be people kind of walking all over the place and then there'd be people just
sitting down waiting for their trains. So I'd usually go up to the person who was
sitting there waiting for their train, you know, doing whatever they do to kind
of occupy their time and I would say, excuse me, will you please fill out the
survey. It felt incredibly awkward stepping into someone's space, disturbing
them, asking them to stop doing what
they were doing and to do something she wanted them to do. As Vanessa asked for help and waited
for an answer, her palms began to sweat. Her heart started beating faster. It was a really sort of
palpable fear that they were going to reject me or worse, right, say something mean. I don't even know what, but I expected them to say something terrible.
Looking back on the moment now, it reminds her of another Stanley Milgram study,
one that's less famous than the obedience experiment.
He had his research assistants go onto New York City subways and ask people for their seat.
Many of his students couldn't complete the task.
His students started coming back to him saying, I can't do this, this is just so upsetting,
this is the most, you know, distressing thing you've ever asked me to do.
And he was like, you guys are being babies, I don't understand why this is so upsetting.
And so, to prove his students wrong, the famous researcher set out for the subway himself. He would do
what his students couldn't, walk up to strangers and ask them for their seats.
He found the experience so much more distressing than he expected it to be
and all of a sudden he understood why they have been complaining so much.
Why is it so hard to make such requests? Well one obvious explanation is that we
know that people will reject us
and that rejection is painful. Vanessa remembers being hugely relieved when she was done giving
out questionnaires at Penn Station and could head back to her lab at Columbia University.
Once there, she and her professor, Frank Flynn, analyzed the responses to the questionnaire.
They noticed something intriguing.
Frank was like, I can't believe how many people are actually saying yes to you.
Total strangers, disrupted from reading their newspaper or eating a sandwich or watching
the crowds of people in the busy station, they were like, sure, I'll respond to your
questionnaire.
We were really surprised by how many people were agreeing in New York Penn Station to
do this survey.
What began as a simple observation turned into something much more important, an insight
about our minds.
Here's the chain of thought that led to the discovery.
The reason Frank and Vanessa were surprised that so many people said yes is because they
expected people to say no.
If lots of people said yes, that meant that Vanessa's fears about rejection were misplaced.
Her perception of the influence she actually had on other people was wrong. Like most of
us, Vanessa had long felt that others had a big effect on her.
As she gazed at the data, she realized that she had a big effect on other
people. If she was blind to this power, what consequences could it have on her
behavior? As researchers, the first thing that Vanessa and Frank decided to do was test if their personal experience was generalizable.
We decided to bring participants into the lab and have them do basically what I had done on those number of days.
So we brought them into the lab. We said, hey, we're going to have you go out and ask people to, as our first step, fill out a survey, just like I had done.
And how many people do you think are going to say yes to you?
We made them estimate how many people they thought
would agree, go out and actually ask people.
And what we found was that they really underestimated
the number of people who would agree to that request.
So it wasn't just Vanessa and Frank.
People in general seem to have a poor assessment
of their power over others.
People thought that others would find it easy
to turn down their requests.
Vanessa connected the seeming blind spot in our thinking
to Stanley Milgram's famous obedience study.
She realized this might be why everyone
always saw the experiment from the perspective
of the volunteers asked to administer shocks,
the people being influenced.
No one saw the experiment from the point of view of the experimenter, the person exercising
influence.
We don't ask, was it hard for him to issue those crazy instructions because we don't
identify with people exercising such influence.
We think that kind of person must be very different from us Because we don't feel we have such power was he surprised to see these people going along with this crazy request
He was making of them. So it's interesting when people think about the Stanley Milgram study
And I think this is true for myself as well
I always imagined myself being in the role of the volunteer in the experiment hearing the instructions from you know
The experiment are saying, you know, you must shock this other person. I never put myself in the shoes of the experiment, hearing the instructions from, you know, the experimenter saying, you know,
you must shock this other person.
I never put myself in the shoes of the experimenter.
Exactly, so that was something that
we started to wonder about.
So we so often sort of simulate,
if I was in that Milgram shock experiment,
what would I do if I was the study participant, right?
Would I actually stand up and go against these directives
and say no? But we kind of flipped that idea on its head.
Vanessa went back to her experience at Penn Station. It felt difficult because she had
seen the interaction only from the point of view of her own insecurities. She hadn't
seen the encounters through the point of view of the people she was asking for help. From
their perspective, an anxious young woman was asking for something trivial.
They had to weigh whether to put aside what they were doing
and help her for a few minutes.
If they said no, it could make them look like jerks.
It's this really interesting phenomenon where you have these two people interacting with one another
and they're both so focused on their own personal anxieties and insecurities and concerns with embarrassment that they don't realize that the other person
is feeling that way too. So it's this really interesting situation where being so inwardly
focused on your own anxieties makes it so difficult for you to recognize what the situation
really is for itself.
People in these encounters experience what psychologists call an egocentric bias. They are so consumed with their own perceptions that they
fail to see what the interaction feels like for the other person. It's
absolutely true that many of us are influenced by situations that many of us
will do things because the situation prompts it. But there is another problem
too and it
might be a deeper problem. The people who put us in those situations, it's not like
they are all-powerful gods. They are humans just like us and they may not
realize the extent of the power they have over us. In fact, they may be
thinking, I'm sure this person is going to turn down my request. They might assume, falsely, that it's easy to refuse instructions.
Vanessa realized that this bias could have all sorts of important consequences.
So what we started looking at about over a decade now,
ago, we started to look at whether we recognize when we're the ones
who are influencing someone else,
when we recognize that someone else, for example, can't say no to something that we've asked them.
Vanessa is now a psychologist at Cornell University. In a series of experiments,
she has demonstrated how people are often oblivious to the power that they have over others.
In one study, she asked volunteers, mostly college students, to make a simple request of others.
We brought people into the lab and we told them, you're going to go out into campus and ask people to borrow their phones.
She walked them through how to approach someone and gave them instructions for what to do once people agree to let them use their phones. They would call us back at the lab and say I
have this person's phone, this is where I'm located, we'd mark it down and then
they go on and ask somebody else. Before the volunteers went out to begin the
study, Vanessa asked them a question. How many people would they have to ask to
get three people to say yes? And at this time, participants are kind of freaked out
by this whole thought.
They are convinced everyone's going to say no.
They're not going to be able to do the task.
And before they actually go out onto campus to do the task,
they often would ask us, well, what if no one agrees?
Do I come back?
What do I do?
They have all these concerns about not being
able to complete the task.
What Vanessa found was similar to her own experience
at Penn Station.
Many more people said yes than the volunteers expected.
They thought they had to ask a little over 10.
They actually had to ask more like six.
In fact, every other person was agreeing to this request.
Maybe you think the students had a high success rate
because they were requesting something trivial.
But Vanessa has also conducted a version of the study
where volunteers had to ask
for something more consequential, money.
For that study, she enlisted the help of the leukemia
and lymphoma societies team in training program.
What people do when they participate
in a fundraising activity for team in training
is they ask people for donations so that they can
participate in some sort of race like a triathlon or a marathon.
They get some training and some travel money to be able to do that and the rest of the money actually goes to the
organization. Vanessa asked participants how many people they would have to solicit to meet their fundraising goals, which were typically thousands of dollars.
They estimated they would need to ask about 200 people to meet the goal.
What we found is that they actually only had to ask about half that.
So they only had to ask about 100 people in order to reach their fundraising goals.
Just as in Vanessa's phone study, her participants doubled the number of people they thought
they had to ask to reach their goal.
Their egocentric bias caused them to
focus so much on their own anxieties that they ignored the influence they
actually had over other people. You're thinking about what you're asking. I'm
asking this person for money. Will this person give me money? What you're not
doing is thinking about what if you were sitting there, you know, potentially in
your cubicle and a co-worker came up to you and said, hey, I'm participating
in a race, would you be willing to sponsor me?
If you were sitting there, it'd be really hard to say no to your coworker, right?
It'd be really hard to let them down, it'd be really awkward, what would you even say?
And so people are kind of put on the spot and they find it really difficult to say no,
so they go ahead and agree.
At the University of Chicago, economist John List has also studied the relationship
between social pressure and charitable giving.
John ran a study where experimenters knocked on the doors of some 8000 houses in the Chicago area.
They were trying to raise money for a children's hospital.
John asked me to imagine the scenario from the point of view of the person receiving
the request.
Let's say it's a Sunday afternoon.
I've just made myself something to eat.
I'm relaxed.
You're sitting on the couch watching a football game and you hear somebody knocking on the
door and you think okay
should I get up or should I stay watching the football games of course a
lot of people get up and answer the door but once they see that there's a
solicitor at the door they say oh my god I wish I would have stayed on the couch
watching the football game too late if they tell the solicitor no, then they have this very
negative or disutility from letting someone down. So they're weighing that
off versus just giving them $20 and having them go on their way. John added
a very interesting twist to the study. Some households were told ahead of time
that a fundraising volunteer would come and knock on the door.
Others were not told ahead of time.
They just received an unexpected knock.
What we find is that when we warn them, of course, many people just stay on the couch
or they leave the house.
They never answer the door.
The people who do answer the door, they do tend to give money.
And much of that is because of altruistic reasons.
But the people who we do not warn,
they end up answering the door more often, and they give more.
Put another way, people understand
how they are going to feel when they're put on the spot.
They often will go to great lengths
to avoid getting in such situations. What this also means is that some significant portion of the money that
charities raise
might not come from altruism. In the case of the Children's Hospital fundraiser,
for example,
what you find is that roughly three-quarters of the dollars given
are due to social pressure and a quarter of the dollars given is actually
due to altruism. So a very small component of what we observe in our door-to-door fundraising
drive is actually driven by altruism.
John's research reminds Vanessa of a classic study where researchers set up two booths
on a college campus. One booth was clearly asking people for something, while the other did not ask for anything.
What the researchers found was similar to John's donation study.
They measured how far away people walked from the booth as they walked by this path.
And if people knew that they were going to be asked for something, their distance from
the booth was much further than if they didn't think they were going to be asked for something.
We just kind of avoid any chance of having to say no to somebody.
We've seen how egocentric bias can cause us to act in helpful ways to others. We lend phones to
people who need them or donate money to charity. Unfortunately though, there's another side to the story. They grabbed that headset and they threw it across the room.
When we come back, the sinister side of our inability to recognize our power over other
people. You're listening to Hidden Brain. I'm Shankar Vedantam.
When we interact with others, we are often intensely focused on how we feel.
Our anxieties, our embarrassments, our fears.
As a result, we are often blind to the effect we have on others,
their anxieties, their embarrassments, and their fears.
Psychologist Vanessa Bonds has studied how such egocentric bias can keep people from asking for help.
But that's not the whole story.
There's kind of the happy story, which is that people will help us more than we think,
and then there's kind of the darker story that people will do a lot of other things for us more so than we think.
So we've run some studies where we started out
kind of asking people if they could get someone
to lie for them.
So our original studies involved just filling something out.
We said, what if we just have them ask
if they'll sign their name to something
saying that you gave them a pitch
that you didn't actually give them,
just kind of a white lie.
And so once again, we had people guess
how many people they would have to make this request of
before a certain number said yes.
They went out onto campus, they asked people,
you know, I'm supposed to be doing this pitch.
I really don't feel like doing it.
Will you just sign this saying that I gave you the pitch?
And again, most people wound up signing it,
even though our participants thought
that most people would say no.
As Vanessa says, the volunteers were asking people to tell a trivial lie.
And perhaps you could say, what's the big deal in signing a note that says, someone gave you a pitch that they didn't?
There are no real moral consequences.
So Vanessa raised the stakes.
So what we did is we created these fake library books.
We took a bunch of books off my bookshelf and just, you know, put some library codes on them.
And we gave them to participants and we said,
we're going to have you go into the libraries on campus and ask people to vandalize these library books.
And so they were to tell people, I'm playing a prank on my friend, but they know my handwriting. Will you please just write Pickle in this library book in pen?
And they left it at that and looked at whether or not people agreed.
And what we found is that the people they approached,
so they kept track of sort of the things that people said when they made this request of them, and
people would say things like, this is wrong.
You shouldn't be doing this. We could get in trouble.
They were clearly uncomfortable with the prospect of vandalizing this purported library book. People would say things like, this is wrong, you shouldn't be doing this, we could get in trouble.
They were clearly uncomfortable with the prospect of vandalizing this purported library book,
but they still did it.
They still did it.
And again, that finding went completely against the intuitions of the volunteers doing the
asking.
People significantly underestimated how much influence they possessed to get others
to do something unethical.
So our participants, before they went out and started asking people, they thought about
28% of people would agree to do this, right? So they thought the vast majority of people
would say no. But when they actually went out and made this request of people, 64%,
a majority of the people they asked actually agreed to vandalize this library
book.
I mean, that's actually pretty astonishing that 64% of people would say yes.
I mean, I would not have predicted it would be as high a number as that.
Yeah.
I mean, this was a task we designed and we were like, this is never going to work, right?
There's no way people are actually going to agree to do this.
And we ourselves were completely surprised that people did agree.
As much as it was uncomfortable
for them to do this unethical thing and vandalize a library book, it was way more uncomfortable
for them to say no to the person who was asking.
The scenarios in which egocentric bias could play a role in our behavior seem endless.
We share the answers to our homework with a friend who asks to
see our work. We don't push back when a colleague suggests bending the rules on a time sheet.
We agree to keep a friend's infidelity a secret, even when it makes us uncomfortable.
Listening to Vanessa made me realize why there is a vast gulf between what we predict we
might do and what we actually do
when we are confronted with problematic behavior.
We look as outsiders at the situation and we say,
why would you tolerate this?
Why wouldn't you just stand up and say,
I'm not going to do this, I'm not going to hurt another person?
I need to be able to do my job
and you're affecting my ability to do my job.
But in fact, the social pressure,
the concern about offending another person,
the social anxiety in that situation is so palpable to that individual that it
feels almost impossible for them to stand up and say something about it
and reject the sort of behavior that they're encountering.
Hidden brain listener Anna Aburuz called in with a story that illustrates how
egocentric
bias can affect workplace behavior. She was training to be an air traffic controller and
saw examples of bullying and harassing behavior all around her. She says the trainers had a clear
message for trainees. Don't be soft or you know you got to have thick skin to survive in air traffic.
That was a common one for sure.
You have to have thick skin to survive in air traffic.
I've heard that over a hundred times.
Anna recalled one painful incident.
There was a trainee that was trying to clear an aircraft for landing.
And the trainer in that moment grabbed the headset of the trainee and this headset is
plugged in to the radar and they grabbed that headset and they threw it across the room which
would fly off of the head of the trainee. They would actually tell them, hey hurry up and go grab it so that you can
plug back in and clear this aircraft for landing.
Anna says seeing such incidents made her fearful. She didn't feel she could complain, since
such behaviors appear to be the norm. Who could she complain to? The people who were
themselves acting badly. They would just say things like what are you doing if you do that again I swear to God. One time when she was
directing aircraft this was in real life not a simulation she found herself
sitting next to one of the trainers who she says had acted abusively to our
trainees. By this point Anna was no longer a trainee. She was directing two
aircraft. One was a thousand feet above the other. I got the names of the of the
aircraft mixed up and I because I was so nervous I think and I descended the
wrong aircraft. I descended the one on top instead of the one on the bottom because
I got those call signs messed up. She told the aircraft that was at a higher
altitude to descend directly into the path of the lower altitude aircraft.
Luckily the pilot could see the aircraft so the pilot was just, no, we're not going to descend. And I immediately knew what I had just done.
And I thought, today was a clear day.
It was clear skies.
There was no clouds in the way.
There wasn't any storm clouds in the way.
But had there been storm clouds, or had there
been some other kind of visual obstruction,
this plane would have descended
and they would have hit that aircraft and I would have been responsible for hundreds of deaths.
And it wasn't because I didn't know
how to control the traffic, I did.
And I had done this a million times.
It was because of the social stress
that I was in at that time
that didn't make me think clearly.
I told Vanessa Barnes what Anna described, how the mere presence of the trainer had disrupted her
to the point where she made a mistake that could have been catastrophic.
Vanessa said, look, it's certainly the case that there are lots of unethical people who know they are unethical and lots of bullies who know they are bullies. Maybe that was the
case here. But there is a deeper problem in the workplace that we often forget. The bullies
and harassers who don't know that they are bullies and harassers.
Often, when we're the person causing someone else distress, we can't see that distress.
It's invisible to us.
And it's not to let anybody off the hook, because clearly it's the people creating this
toxic culture's responsibility to kind of fix it and to not cause these things to happen.
But there's also this cognitive bias there where we may not realize the extent to which we're interfering with somebody else's performance.
These same dynamics play out in another common occurrence in the workplace, unwanted romantic attention.
We ran a couple of studies where we asked people about their experiences being asked out at work or asking someone out at work.
And we ask people to imagine situations
where they weren't interested in the other person
or the other person wasn't interested in them.
And what we found is that people who asked somebody out
at work and were rejected thought that it was pretty easy
for that person to reject them, right?
They didn't think that that person experienced
a whole lot of distress and they didn't think
that they changed their behavior very much after being asked out.
But when people recalled situations where they were asked out by someone at work who
they weren't interested in, they described feeling obligated to say yes, feeling much
more uncomfortable saying no to the person, and they reported doing all sorts of things
to try to avoid that person that the other person didn't realize that they were doing.
So in fact, this little request, we tell people to just go for it and
ask this person out.
It actually puts a lot more pressure on the other person than we tend to realize
when we're the ones doing the asking.
In some ways, we underestimate the pressure that we exert on other people.
In some ways, that's the moral of the whole story here, isn't it?
Absolutely, yeah. We underestimate the influence that we have over other people, and we underestimate
the extent to which asking them for something really puts them in an awkward position, because
now they have to say no, and that's just a really hard thing for people to do.
Like many psychological biases, the tendency we have to downplay the
influence we have on others can have far-reaching consequences. It can keep
us from asking for help that would be forthcoming. It can keep us from
reaching out and making friends with strangers. And it can also lead us to
give in to unethical demands or make improper demands of other people. I asked Vanessa how her research had prompted her
to do things differently in her own life.
It has made a huge difference in the little things.
So for example, when I was pregnant,
if I needed a seat on the subway or on a train,
I would kind of stand there and look around
and try to look my most pathetic
so that someone would give me their seat, thinking that someone would step up and do it because they were
nice, right?
But in fact, everyone's all involved in their own stuff.
They're not necessarily looking around and paying attention.
And maybe they'd be perfectly happy to give up their seat, but they're not going to think
of it unless you actually ask.
And so I tried to take that into account.
So when I was pregnant, I would go up to people and be like, hey,
can I sit down?
I'd really love to sit.
And then, of course, people are incredibly happy to just pop up
and say yes.
And what's interesting, of course, is when that happens,
you're actually giving people an opportunity to do something nice.
It's not just that you're imposing on them.
Presumably, some of them are actually happy to say,
I was just writing to work
and now I actually got to do this nice thing
for this other person.
I feel this little warm glow.
Yeah, absolutely.
So a lot of people wonder about the takeaway.
So if people agree to help us out of obligation
because they feel like they can't say no,
then do you really want to ask them for things?
But people are really good at justifying their behaviors
and ways that make them feel good about themselves.
So they may agree to help
because they feel like they can't say no,
but pretty quickly after that,
they're going to be convincing themselves that they helped
because they're a really wonderful person.
And so everyone's going to walk away
feeling good about the interaction.
You got the help that you needed
and the other person gets to feel like a good person.
Psychologists once conducted a light-hearted version of Stanley Milgram's Obedience Study. In the 1970s, they had research assistants stand on the streets of New York City.
Their jobs?
To look skyward.
At nothing.
The question the researchers wanted to know was whether innocent passers-by would also stop to look up to see what was going on.
They found that when more people were in on the gag, more pedestrians stopped and looked up.
I've seen video of that study many times and always found this scene funny.
One to fifteen people just staring off into the sky. Recently I rewatched it
and this time I did what Vanessa had done. I flipped the script. Instead of seeing the
experiment from the point of view of the passersby and asking myself whether I would be similarly
influenced, I looked at the experiment from the point of view of the research assistants.
Did they expect so many people to join them in looking at nothing?
We've seen throughout this episode how all of us as individuals have great power to shape how others behave.
If each of us has this hidden power, then collectively, as groups, as communities, as tribes,
we are going to have even more influence.
How we choose to use that influence? That's up to each of us.
HIDDEN BRAIN is produced by Hidden Brain Media. Our audio production team includes Annie Murphy Paul, Kristen Wong, Laura Quarell, Ryan Katz, Autumn Barnes, Andrew Chadwick and Nick Woodbury.
Tara Boyle is our executive producer. I'm Hidden Brain's executive editor.
Tara Boyle is our executive producer. I'm Hidden Brains executive editor.
We end today with a story from our sister show, My Unsung Hero.
It's brought to you by T-Mobile for Business.
Our story comes from Bethany Renfri.
I was 20 years old. My baby girls and I were living in low-income apartments.
Most of my neighbors were single mothers like myself.
And I remember how overwhelmed I felt that morning. It was a cold day in the apartment.
I dragged myself to the sink and it was stacked with dishes with pots and pans that had been
soaking because I'd burnt them all. I didn't know how to cook back then, and I would always burn our pans.
And I looked at my twin girls.
They were 18 months old.
They sat in their high chairs.
The baby, the newborn was in her swing.
I looked back at the sink, and I just couldn't bring myself to do those dishes, and I couldn't
look at them any longer.
It was a reminder of how overwhelmed I felt in my own life.
So I grabbed a white garbage bag
and I stacked the dishes in there one by one.
I walked out in the rain and I placed it
on the edge of the apartment dumpster
because the dumpster was full.
And I came back in and the girls and I left for the day.
When I got back that evening, it was dark and my porch was dark because I didn't even
have the energy to change the porch light.
But as we were coming in, I kind of kicked something.
It was a box.
And so I brought it into the apartment and put it on the table and it was my pots and
pans and they were shining and sparkling,
and the girls' Blue's Clues plates and their sippy cups,
and a little handwritten note popped out
on a yellow piece of paper,
and it said,
I've been there before, you will make it, I promise you.
I don't know which of the single mothers
went out there that day and saw that garbage bag
and understood what was happening.
But if I saw her today, I would thank her for showing me
that we are not alone and we are not bad mothers,
even in our hardest moments.
We are surrounded by kindness and understanding and I'm so grateful to have learned that lesson
so early on in motherhood.
Bethany Renfrew lives in Sutter Creek, California. When she was 27, she went back to school and earned undergraduate and master's degrees.
She is now a legislative director in the California State Senate.
Today's My Unsung Hero story was brought to you by T-Mobile for Business.
If you enjoy Hidden Brain, please take a moment and share your favorite episode with two or three people in your life.
Word of mouth recommendations really make a huge difference
in introducing new listeners to our work.
We truly appreciate your support.
I'm Shankar Vedantam.
See you soon.