Hidden Brain - Revealing Your Unconscious: Part 2

Episode Date: March 14, 2023

In the second part of our series on implicit bias, we explore the relationship between beliefs and behaviors. We also talk with psychologist Mahzarin Banaji about whether research on implicit bias tel...ls us more about groups than it does about individuals.To learn more:Project ImplicitOutsmarting Implicit BiasHow do your beliefs about the world shape your reality, and your well-being? Be sure to listen to our recent episode about primal world beliefs for insights on that question. And if you enjoy our work, please consider supporting it. Thanks!

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. All of us know what prejudice looks like. We've seen news stories about swastikas pre-painted on synagogues or news is drawn on classroom walls to terrorize black students. We have heard xenophobic speeches from politicians and watched in horror as ethnic groups around the world have exterminated their enemies. In the late 1990s, Harvard psychologist Mazurine Banaji and her former PhD advisor, Tony Greenwald
Starting point is 00:00:40 of the University of Washington, developed a test of hidden bias called the implicit association test or IAT. Unlike the very public spectacle of a burning cross on someone's front lawn, the picture of bias painted by this test was rather subtle. By measuring the speed of people's associations, the tests showed that large numbers of Americans found it easier to associate white faces with positive concepts than to associate black faces with positive concepts. Many people were similarly quick to associate men with professional activities and slow to associate women with such activities. Lots and lots of Americans appear to have negative associations about the elderly,
Starting point is 00:01:29 the overweight, and the disabled. Crucially, large numbers of the people taking the tests didn't think of themselves as being prejudiced, many prided themselves on having egalitarian beliefs. Their test results often came as a shock. The feeling was a feeling of dread. I would say a feeling of having had the rug pulled out from under you. You have to start from scratch to now rebuild a view of yourself
Starting point is 00:02:10 that will forever be a different view of yourself. In part one of the story, which I strongly recommend you listen to before you listen to this episode, we explored the origins of the psychological test. Today on the show, we explore what it means that so many people have subtle biases when it comes to their mental associations. Are these biases benign and just inside people's heads, or do they cause people to act in biased ways?
Starting point is 00:02:43 Is there anything we can do to fight our biases? The surprising connection between our biases and our behavior, this week on Hidden Brain. In George Orwell's dystopian novel 1984, there were lots of ways to get in trouble with a totalitarian state. Protesting in the street was a quick way to get seized by the authorities, but you could also get in trouble for subtler things, like reading the wrong book or having the wrong opinions. The eyes and ears of the state were everywhere, and subjects were expected to not just do the right things,
Starting point is 00:03:25 but to think the right thoughts. After Harvard University put the implicit association test on its website, you can find it at implicit.harvard.edu. Interest in the test surged. Many companies began mandating their employees take the test during diversity training exercises. As we saw in part one of the story, early studies that Mazarin and others conducted suggested a connection between implicit biases and the behavior of individuals taking the test. In one study she conducted with Physician Alexander Greene, Physicians with higher bias scores on the IAT
Starting point is 00:04:10 were less likely to prescribe clot-busting treatments to a black patient, relative to a white patient. But soon, other studies started to come out that showed no association between the two. People showing higher levels of bias on the test did not act in biased ways. Critics of the test, including Phil Tatlock at the University of Pennsylvania, started to argue that the test was measuring the equivalent of Orwellian thought crimes instead of judging people on their words and actions. I think the IAT is grounded in a reductionist view of human nature.
Starting point is 00:04:50 I conducted this interview with Phil some years ago in 2017, as we featured him on another episode of Hidden Brain. It depicts people somewhat as association driven automaton. I'm not temperamentally all that comfortable with that format reductionism, but I wouldn't reject the test on that basis. And I don't reject the test. It's a little bit different. I mean, it's a test that is enormously intuitively appealing. I mean, I've never seen a psychological test take off the way the IAT has and the way it's gripped the popular imagination, the way it has, because it just seems on its surface
Starting point is 00:05:25 to be measuring something like prejudice. You've got the differential reaction times, right, between the black and white stimuli, say, and it just seems to be a bullseye. And anybody who denies it is engaging as some kind of scholastic quibbling. But there is the question of whether or not people who score as prejudiced on the IET
Starting point is 00:05:44 actually act in the discriminatory ways toward other human beings in real world situations. And if they don't, if there is very close to zero relationship between those two things, what exactly is the IET measuring? Metaanalyses studies tracking a body of research not just an individual experiment are one way to tell if something is a fad or a fact. If you have a number of studies linking a medication with positive patient outcomes, for example, you become more certain that the medication actually works.
Starting point is 00:06:23 META analysis of the IAT test data found mixed results. Some studies showed a connection between implicit bias and real-world discrimination, but plenty of others did not. For many critics of the test, this was vindication that the test was useless and that the hype over the test was unfounded. Challenges to the usefulness of Ma Zareen's research were published in peer-reviewed academic journals. Soon, Ma Zareen and her colleagues decided to launch a comprehensive meta-analysis of their own. I asked her if she found there was a correlation between implicit biases
Starting point is 00:07:02 and real world behavior. The strength of the correlation is small. When scientists measure whether two things are correlated to each other, they use a scale from zero to one. Toler people tend to be heavier people, so height and weight are correlated. It doesn't mean every tall person is going to weigh more than every short person. All it says is that there is a relationship between the two. As height goes up, you're likely to weigh more. But there are lots of things that are not correlated. Taller people, for example, are not better at math. The closer a correlation between two things is to zero, the less likely the two things
Starting point is 00:07:45 are correlated. And when it came to Maserine's analysis of the IAT? We published a paper that probably places the correlation at the smallest it has ever been reported to be, around 0.1. And that's because we decided to include every single study no matter how poorly it was conducted. So we decided we will not throw any study out. There are studies in there that really don't belong. Somebody did a study looking at where the race bias would predict their degree of smoking.
Starting point is 00:08:18 There should be no correlation, but that study is in there and it counts as a lack of correlation. So we have gone, you know, overboard in making sure that no matter what the study we will include it, and so we show that even when you do that, the correlation is about 0.1, within that set, if you begin to use five criteria for what is a good study, that any scientist would agree, you know, The correlation jumps up to three times that size. I first read about Maasareen's work and the IAT around 20 years ago. It's fair to say I initially thought it would show that individuals with implicit biases
Starting point is 00:09:01 always act in biased ways. I wrote about the test in my 2010 book The Hidden Brain. As mixed data started to emerge in the last decade, I found myself having to question my beliefs. There was no doubt that large numbers of people around the world showed fast or slow associations on the tests. And the results of the tests generally matched our intuitions about the nature of prejudice. Across many countries, people more swiftly associated men, rather than women, with concepts related to science or leadership. Groups that were in the majority often had negative biases about minorities. The data coming in were voluminous. This wasn't about a couple of studies and a few dozen subjects.
Starting point is 00:09:47 There were hundreds of studies and millions of test takers. But if people's test results were only weekly correlated with real world behavior, what did these results mean? Did it really make sense for companies around the world to mandate their employees take these tests? Some were even using test results to figure out who should be involved with HR and hiring, or rising to leadership positions. As critics and supporters use the studies as ammunition for their pet beliefs, Maserine and her colleagues kept doing research.
Starting point is 00:10:21 The volume of data rolling in meant that they could do something that most social scientists working in other areas could only dream about. They could start to analyze relationships between test results and real-world behavior, not at the level of individuals, or even at the level of companies, but at the level of cities, regions, and nations. In 2009, they reported preliminary results on an unusual finding. We had data from a few countries, not as many as we have today, so we are in the process of replicating that to see if with so much more data, because we did this a long time ago, in which we knew that there was a standardized test that was taken across countries in eighth grade and we could get the data on the gender difference in performance on that mathematics test taken in eighth grade across many countries.
Starting point is 00:11:14 And so for each country, you compute the gender difference. What is the gender difference in country A? It could be big. If boys score a lot higher than the girls, that's a big difference. There might be countries where boys and girls score about the same. So you have a lot of countries with a bunch of variation in gender. And what you're looking to see is whether the countries that show the largest difference in math performance between girls and boys are also the countries whose people are carrying in their heads
Starting point is 00:11:47 a stronger association of boys with math rather than girls with math. And we reported that there is a robust positive correlation. Countries with higher gender bias are also the countries where the girls are underperforming compared to boys to a greater extent. I want to take a moment to sit with this finding. At the time she did this study, Mazarin didn't think much of it. But what she was finding was that when you give people an implicit association test, measuring how quickly the associate concepts in mathematics with men, rather than with women, they were
Starting point is 00:12:23 regional differences, national differences in test scores. When you averaged implicit bias scores across entire countries, people in some countries were faster than others when it came to associating men with math and slower in associating women with math. Now if you turn to how eighth grade students were doing in the standardized math test, girls in countries with high implicit bias were doing worse than girls in countries with low implicit bias.
Starting point is 00:12:55 The interesting thing is that if you look not at a nation but at an individual student or an individual school, you might see little to no correlation between implicit bias test results and student performance on the standardized mathematics test. Or you might find it in one school, but not in the next. It's only when you step back and looked at the big picture that you saw a robust correlation. It was like one of those pixelated images, or a painting that uses the style known as pointillism. Up close, you just see a lot of dots. It's only when you step back, you realize, oh, there's a picture here.
Starting point is 00:13:35 That paper, while I always thought that was an interesting result, I think I wasn't smart enough to realize that the reason we were getting a fairly substantial correlation is because we were wiping out lots of individual-level error. We were collapsing across many different people to come up with a much more stable score of what is going on in the larger system, in the larger environment in which people are sitting. And when you take the average of that, the average of a whole bunch of people, you're likely to pick up the actual or true correlation in
Starting point is 00:14:10 a much better way. When someone gives you a test, you feel the test is saying something about you. That is true, but also not completely true when it comes to the implicit association test. Yes, at one level, the tests are telling you about something that is inside your head. But the tests might be telling you something much more important about the culture in which you are living. When we come back, what happens when we look at the implicit association test not at the level of individuals, but at the level of cities, counties and nations? You're listening to Hidden Brain.
Starting point is 00:14:58 I'm Shankar Vedantam. The implicit association test became very popular after psychologist Mazurine Banaji and her colleagues placed the test on the Harvard website. Millions of people took the test, hoping it would give them a glimpse into their own minds. I took the race win and to be honest with you I was really surprised at how insightful it was. Does that score mean that I do not like European Americans? No. It's my subconscious aware of the condition that African Americans are in in this country at this particular point.
Starting point is 00:15:47 Is it because I can't come just to say that I'm bad? And is it just in our nature that there has to be an us in them? And them is going to be the bad guy. But as researchers evaluated whether test results revealed real world behavior, they found mixed results. Sometimes people who showed high bias on the implicit association test acted in ways that were biased, but at other times, they didn't. When you looked at all the studies together, the correlation between individual implicit
Starting point is 00:16:23 bias test results and individual real world outcomes was small. But the torrent of data from IAT test takers around the United States and around the world meant researchers could now start to analyze not just links between test results and individual behavior, but the correlations between average scores in an area and real world outcomes. Early on, Maserine and her colleagues discovered a curious result. The performance of boys and girls in an eighth-grade standardized math test appeared to be linked to average implicit bias scores in those nations.
Starting point is 00:17:02 Countries where people were quicker to associate men with signs showed a wider gap in test scores. Girls did worse on the standardized test. In time, other research along these lines started to emerge. So, Raj Chetty, for example, my colleague in economics, is interested in not only why upward mobility is so slow these days compared to what it was, why the American dream has vanished, but he's also interested in for whom is the American dream more likely and less likely. And so he took our data and said, well, we can look county by county at IAT. So now forget the individual. Instead, identify the county and take the average IAT of all the people in that county and give it just one score. So you collapse across all the people in the county and you come up with one I counties, and you look to see if that predicts upward
Starting point is 00:18:08 mobility for black Americans. In other words, he shows that the higher the race bias, average race bias in a county, the harder it is for black people in that county to be upwardly mobile. This is just one example. Now that we've understood what the correlation is, I can just rattle off for you. You know, now I think we're up to about 17 independent studies that have been published, that show that higher race bias in a county will predict greater lethal use of force by police against black Americans. The most recent study shows greater militarization of police departments in those counties, greater threats to maternal health and infant health in the
Starting point is 00:18:52 counties that have greater bias, school disciplining differences between white and black kids that are greater in counties that have greater race bias, traffic stops, tickets, et cetera. And these 17 studies that I'm just mentioning that look at average IAT scores by county or by state or by metropolitan region or by country, they're all averages by region. They are just predicting up and down the spectrum in a way in which I would never have predicted,
Starting point is 00:19:22 but it's really exciting to see because these dependent variables are not simple little things. It's not even how well you do on a math test. This is about whether you live or die. This is about whether you will get disciplined in school and get kicked out. This is about whether you will live as a baby. Can you talk a little bit about why it is we would see a stronger correlation at this aggregate level? So in other words, if you're analyzing my brain and saying, here's your implicit bias and then you're evaluating me to see, do I hire a black person or a white person?
Starting point is 00:19:59 Do I hire a man or a woman? Why is it that we would see a lower correlation at me and an individual? But when you step out and look at the aggregate, you have a higher correlation. How would that be the case? That's the power of aggregation. Any individual score is going to vary based on lots of things jittering around in that moment.
Starting point is 00:20:20 And more importantly, whether you behave in a way that is biased or less biased is going to be multiply determined by little things in the local environment. For example, my score on race bias may be quite high. I may be quite anti-black, but it may be that in the moment in which you're testing my behavior, a smiling person appeared in front of me who wiped out my bias and I responded positively to that person. Little things like that in the environment can make the behavior move around and not allow
Starting point is 00:20:53 the particular measure in which you're interested in to show itself. So as soon as you aggregate it for every person like me, somebody else's similar behavior will counter it. And so all you are doing the best way to understand it is that when we aggregate, we are removing individual level noise in the data. One analogy to this idea comes from the realm of polling. In the United States, lots of polling is done by groups that have either a conservative or a liberal bias. Unsurprisingly, polls that lean conservative are likely to predict conservative victories. Liberal polls are likely to predict victories for Democrats.
Starting point is 00:21:46 But something interesting happens when you average out the polls. The poll that leans too far right gets balanced out by the poll that leans left. When you average polls, you are likely to get answers that are much more accurate than individual polls. The same thing happens if you ask people to make estimates of something, say the size of the US economy. Low estimates and high estimates cancel each other out when you average the answers, leaving you with a better approximation of the correct answer. This phenomenon is sometimes known as the wisdom of the crowd, meaning the average answer across a group of people is often more accurate than
Starting point is 00:22:25 individual answers. Now there are two views on this. The two views are my view is that as we remove noise we will see higher and higher levels of correlation. In other words as the tests get, as studies are conducted more carefully, Ma Zerina saying she expects the correlations to get better at the individual level, not just at the level of nations. But she also cites a second possibility, that the IAT is really capturing a reflection in people's minds of something that is in the larger culture. Somebody I admire greatly and agree with in many ways,
Starting point is 00:23:06 and I'm not opposed to this, but this is Keith Payne. He argues that these things don't operate at the level of the individual. You will never, no matter how much error variance you remove, it will never get better at the individual level because we become of a certain place when we go into a certain area. So he did this remarkable study because we become of a certain place when we go into a certain area.
Starting point is 00:23:25 So he did this remarkable study that if I can just describe really quickly, I will tell you about it. Keith obtained a map that had been produced by Abraham Lincoln in the 1860s, in which Lincoln had his people plot county by county the proportion of enslaved to free people in every county in the southern states. And he did this because obviously he was this smart guy, a scientist almost, because he thought if I know that, if I know the proportion of enslaved people in a county, I will be able to make better military predictions about which counties are going to fall faster than other counties.
Starting point is 00:24:09 And the simple idea was, the greater the proportion of enslaved people, the harder they will fight and the more they will resist giving up slavery. So this map exists even to this day. You can look at the map, you can see the counties and little numbers that tell us what the proportions are. So Keith, you know, in the 21st century goes back to this map and he says, let's correlate these two things. IAT raised bias in that county today and the number of enslaved to free people, proportions or ratios in 1860. And lo and behold, the correlation is quite substantial and high. And he will say exactly as we've been discussing he will say,
Starting point is 00:24:55 well how can it be these are not the same people. Notice how surprising this is. Keith Bain, a psychologist at the University of North Carolina Chapel Hill, looked at implicit bias test results for people in the 21st century. Why would these results be connected to policies that existed more than 150 years ago? Everyone who lived in those counties in the 1860s is now very dead. The proportions of enslaved to free people are not the people whose minds were measuring. In fact, we can't. They're gone.
Starting point is 00:25:31 And it's not even the case that the descendants of those very people live there today. In the United States, enough migrations have happened that that is not the case. So Keats' point of view, which is very interesting, is that your mind reflects what is right around you. And that if you were somebody who lived in Seattle and Microsoft Corporation sent you to some place in Georgia and you arrive there, you become of that place.
Starting point is 00:26:06 If there are many Confederate statues in the town in which you live, your mind will move in the direction of more anti-black bias. Your children will hear certain things in school and they'll bring them to your home and as you talk about them, you too will start to acquire that. And these are the things that ultimately create these remarkable correlations almost unbelievable, and what it tells us is just the long shadow of history and how psychology is able to pick up this incredibly long shadow of history, that we can look at data from 1860 and we can predict today what that county's race bias is. Or we can look at the race
Starting point is 00:26:46 bias today and we can predict who those people were. I find this absolutely fascinating. In some ways, I think I'm hearing three different models. One says bias is produced by active animosity and hostility. People who act in biased ways mean to be biased. The second says, no, our minds are mirrors and when we go to different places, we are going to reflect what is out there. But I think there's also a third model, and I might call this the hypertension model. So if you would have measured my blood pressure right now and measured my blood pressure two hours from now or two days from now, it's
Starting point is 00:27:35 going to fluctuate because blood pressure is not super stable. It depends on what's going on, what's happening to me physically, my mental state. But if you find that I do have high blood pressure, it doesn't necessarily tell you I'm going to have a heart attack next week, or I'm going to have a stroke next month. So in other words, it's a useful measure, but at an individual level, it's a somewhat crude measure of determining short term risk. But if you would step back and say,
Starting point is 00:27:59 what's the average hypertension of all the people in California or what's the average hypertension of all the people living in New York. And let's say the average hypertension in California was significantly higher than the average hypertension in New York. You could very confidently say you will have many more heart attacks and strokes in California than in New York, even though you can't predict which individuals are going to be affected. You can say something meaningful about the group, even if you can't be very precise about individuals. I mean, the same thing goes for smokers and non-smokers. I might not be able to tell you which individuals
Starting point is 00:28:31 are going to develop cancer in any given week, but I know the group of smokers is going to have more cancers than the group of non-smokers. So both the mirror model and the hypertension model suggest that if you want to understand how unconscious biases caused by its actions, you need to look beyond the individual mind and look at larger systems and structures. I think you very much have it right.
Starting point is 00:28:57 And I like the example of hypertension for many reasons, one of which is that the way we measure hypertension shows that the machine is not terribly reliable. For all the reasons that you said, if my arm is up or down, if I've just eaten, if I've walked, it will vary in sometimes substantially. There is error variance in that measure. The measure is not as good as it could be, same with the IAT. The IAT is not as perfect a measure as we would think or like it to be. So there's error variance there. However, your blood pressure does fluctuate. My brain is not the same brain as it was two hours ago. You know, having talked to you, a bunch of connections have now been made and my bias on some topic because we've been
Starting point is 00:29:45 talking about it could be higher or lower than it was. In other words, the IAT is actually picking up the real state of your brain now, which was different than it was yesterday, and therefore the real, what we will say is the reliability is low. So I think when we put it all together, so this is one strand of wire like your hypertension example. The other reason I like the hypertension example is when I teach, when I say, you know, hypertension is called the silent killer because you don't feel it.
Starting point is 00:30:14 You know, it's not like osteoarthritis or something where the pain tells you that something is going on in your body. But wouldn't we want to know that we have it? And don't we want to invent gizmos that are not very reliable but can still save our lives? I think of this attempt, and I'm not speaking about the IAT here, I'm speaking about any attempt to try to get at this kind of implicit cognition, I think it's exactly the same thing that we're trying to do for our mind as we do for our body. We're trying to invent a measure that may not be very reliable, but could give us enough evidence
Starting point is 00:30:51 that we would say, you know what, knowing this, I will change my behavior. I will do things in a different way. And so I just love the hypertension example. You know, one question it does raise, though, is that to the extent that these measures are in fact telling us something more useful at the aggregate level than at the individual level, you know, whether that's the key pain idea that our minds are really reflecting what's happening around us. In other words, what you're picking up in the measure of me as an individual is really
Starting point is 00:31:19 my reflection of what's in the society and the culture around me. Or, in the case of the hypertension example, my hypertension is actually more relevant as a clue when you aggregate it with the hypertension of all the other people around me in terms of predicting where the heart attacks and strokes are going to be. In both those cases, does it not raise questions about the model of fixing these biases that seems to have become very popular, where so much of the efforts to fix implicit bias has been about trying to eradicate bias from individual brains. When you think about DEI efforts at various companies and corporations, so much of that
Starting point is 00:31:54 is we'll give you a test, we'll show you through the test that you have bias, and then we're going to try and train this bias out of you. If in fact the bias comes from the society, if in fact it's a reflection of the society, or in fact it's part of the larger systems and structures in which we're part of, is it a fool's errand to try and say we can actually just fix individual minds and hope to solve the problem? In one sense I couldn't agree with you more. I would say that it is a fool's errand to think that we can go into a corporation, especially as it is currently done, come and give a talk on implicit this or that, and then assume that we've checked off our box and now we don't have implicit bias. In Go Frank Dobin and others and say,
Starting point is 00:32:38 the people who did the NIH training, nothing happened there. Maasuring is referring here to work by the sociologist Frank Dobin and Alexandra Kallev, who find that mandatory diversity training, as practiced by many corporations today, is not only ineffective, but frequently counterproductive. So, that's not a surprise, because the intervention is not up to the task of actually changing anything real. I do believe, though, that that education is necessary. And it's necessary not because it will change an individual person's bias,
Starting point is 00:33:14 but it will make them open to structural changes their organization will want to make. So if I work with any group that comes to me and says, what shall we do? I will say, I will teach them about implicit bias in a scientific way. You can't do this if it's mandatory. I will only come if it's voluntary. And what I think we will achieve is that when you then go to them and you say, you know what, the way we run interviews is really bad. Interviews are a terrible way to make decisions. We are going to terrible way to make decisions. We are going to start to do something differently. We are going to get resumes with much harder good evidence.
Starting point is 00:33:51 We are not going to let people write their hobbies on their resumes. We are going to do these screenings. We are going to bring interns in for six months and we are going to pick from that instead of these silly ways in which we did. I believe that if that education has been done well, that you will be able to make all these institutional level changes that will ultimately change the level of bias because you will have fixed it by intervening in the right moments. So I'm very clear with organizations. If you want to change people, I'm not the right person for you,
Starting point is 00:34:24 I will, in fact, agree very much with you, Shankar, in saying that that would be a fool, Zaryn. But I don't say don't educate them because I do believe that the education plays the role of making individuals feel secure as to why we're going about changing our organization. If police officers in Cambridge who I've worked with haven't been in a session with me and haven't learned why what I'm saying is in their interest they will resist every little step of the way wearing a body camera wearing a bulletproof vest and every. But after a session like this and we have a paper in which we're going to summarize, the massive shift in attitudes that we've seen in police officers prior to an educational
Starting point is 00:35:11 seminar and post an educational seminar. Now they're saying, yeah, I see why this is good for me. So I believe in teaching and I believe it's necessary, but nowhere sufficient. in teaching and I believe it's necessary, but nowhere sufficient. When we come back, how change happens? You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. Psychologist Maserine Banaji had a formative experience growing up in India. She was a member of a minority group that was all but invisible. So the short version is that Zorastrianism is known today as the oldest monotheistic religion in the world. Its origins were in Central Asia, in particular in what was then the Persian Empire. And Zorastrianism was the state-religion.
Starting point is 00:36:18 It was a very successful religion. It spread far and wide until about the 8th century, when Islamic invasions of that part of the world began, and over two centuries, somewhere between the 8th and 10th century, Zorastrians who did not wish to be converted, took off in little boats looking for asylum, religious asylum. And I guess the first country that allowed them that religious asylum was India.
Starting point is 00:36:50 It was on the west coast of India that they landed in Gujarat. And the story goes that the local king met them and said to them, we are full. There are many of us here,, we can't take you in. And at least the apocryphal story is that the captain of this little boat asked for some milk and sugar because they couldn't speak the same language. And use this, put the sugar into the milk, stirred it and explained that we would just blend in and that we would
Starting point is 00:37:26 sweeten the milk. And the king was so happy with this demo that he apparently letters in and said, you can practice your religion, you can have whatever beliefs you wish, you have to speak our language and where are dress. Growing up, Mazarin often felt pulled in two directions. Ever the observer, she noticed when this happened and what it meant. I'll just give you one example. In my own community of Zorastrians, but particularly in my family, I was considered dark-skinned.
Starting point is 00:38:09 I was. People would say things to me like, oh, you know, Karice. Karice means she's black. But when I would step out of the house, I would be considered pale-skinned in South India. So what was I, very early? It was a conundrum.
Starting point is 00:38:25 Am I dark skinned or am I light skinned? I think it taught me that there was clearly nothing inherent in the physical aspect of my skin color that made me light or dark. it's the context that made it so. In India, the Zoroastrian community is known as the Parse community. Likely because the first Zoroastrians were associated with travelers from Pars, a region of Iran. Maserine said she always sensed the Parse community had to stand apart from the larger Indian society.
Starting point is 00:39:07 It was communicated in a very sort of sideways way. Nobody said to us, you cannot do X. But we just knew we couldn't. If you needed something fixed, you would call the Parseic neighbor who would call, you know, the Parseic neighbor who would call the Parcy friend who would come and fix it. You didn't participate in society. And I only learned this when I married somebody from the dominant group. And I watched how his family operated.
Starting point is 00:39:37 And I thought, wow, that's what it means to have access. It just normal stuff. My father wouldn't even collect the reimbursements of health insurance that would have come to him because he was a government servant. He just wouldn't participate in those sorts of things. We wouldn't feel we had access, but we knew that we would be safe. Nobody was going to come kill us or anything. But we had to just stay outside the mainstream in some way.
Starting point is 00:40:08 Maserine told me that while she was deeply enmeshed in Parse culture while living in India, it was only when she came to the United States that she really started to understand her family's faith. I learned a lot about my own religion when I came to Yale as an assistant professor, and I met Stanley Insler, who was a scholar of Zorastronism, and particularly of the Holy Book, which I can, you know, I can rattle off many thousands of lines of code in a language called Avastan, but I
Starting point is 00:40:35 don't understand it. And what I learned by reading Stanley's books is that Zorastronism takes as its core principle the recognition that the world is constructed of good and evil, and that the job of every Zorastron every day is to ask on which side am I going to be. And that when you review your life, that's what you look at. How many times was I on the side of good or not? And I think of that as both quite profound, but also somewhat ironic. I only noticed it years after we had worked on implicit attitudes that the fundamental dimension I study is the dimension of good and bad.
Starting point is 00:41:34 Some time ago, one of Maizarin students came to her with a research idea that had direct bearing on this question of good and bad. Was it possible Tessa Charles were asked that implicit biases were actually receding, that the United States was becoming less biased? I was completely confident, and I even said to her, yeah, you're not going to see any change, not in our lifetime. I mean, change will happen, but if you look at the IAT from 2007 to today, my prediction, no change, it'll be a flat line over time because implicit bias changes, but not fast.
Starting point is 00:42:10 So Tessa does these lovely analysis. And what the data show is something quite stunning. On the sexuality test, the anti-gay bias test, bias was quite high. Anti-gay bias was quite high in 2007. But with every day, every month, since then, it has slowly been coming down. So that in 2020, that bias has come down close to neutrality.
Starting point is 00:42:40 What Maserina Tessa found was that between 2007 and 2020, anti-K bias decreased by nearly two thirds. It's not yet neutral, but we're predicting our model predicts that in one and a half years, Americans will be at neutrality on that. There are also encouraging signs when it comes to biases based on race. Race bias has also come down on two kinds of measures, the black white test, and also the dark skin, light skin test, which is not race, but could be seen as another proxy for something akin to race.
Starting point is 00:43:20 Both tests show exactly the same drop in bias by 25%. So it is not nothing, but it is not 64%. Which we know it could be if we were doing things the way we're doing for sexuality bias. To recap, Mazurine and Tessa's data found that race bias has come down by 25% and anti-gay bias by a remarkable 64%. That's the good news.
Starting point is 00:43:49 The bad news? There are three other types of bias where the data haven't budged at all. Anti-eldrally bias, disability bias, body weight bias. These stigmas, I think, are going to be much harder to change. They're visible, they're on the body, and we don't talk about them nearly enough. We're not arguing about age, bias, or disability or body weight. In fact, body weight bias, people express quite explicitly. That may be a part of it, but also these are going to be harder to change. Right now if we do nothing, those biases are with us for at least 200 years. That's what our model predicts. Let me go back to sexuality and just say one thing about it that I think is incredibly interesting. We thought, okay, it's changing, but it must be young people only or
Starting point is 00:44:45 a certain part, a certain demographic group. Gay people only, things like that. And it turns out, no, everybody is changing. Conservatives are changing, and liberals are changing. Elderly are changing, and young are changing. Educated and less educated are changing. Rich are changing, and poor are changing and young are changing, educated and less educated are changing. Rich are changing and poor are changing. So I think these results are together really exciting to us. It tells us that change is possible at this societal level. As we put this episode together, Republicans and Democrats in the Senate
Starting point is 00:45:36 came together to pass landmark legislation and shining the right to gay marriage. I find it difficult to imagine this would have happened if it were not for a sea change in public attitudes, a sea change that the implicit bias test seems to have picked up. Mazarin points to the forces driving the change, there was change at the individual level as grandparents reconciled themselves with their grandchildren's sexuality, changes at an institutional level as companies began offering same-sex
Starting point is 00:46:06 benefits to workers, and change at the level of national policy in terms of laws and Supreme Court decisions. All three happened within a tight period of time, and when you have changed at this many different levels of society from the individual human to the Supreme Court, that's when you can get a 64% drop in implicit bias, anti-gay bias. To be clear, the fact that there has been a dramatic drop in anti-gay bias does not mean the pendulum cannot swing back.
Starting point is 00:46:37 There are jurisdictions across the US and around the world that are actively trying to curtail LGBTQ rights. There is a two-way street between what happens in our minds and public policy. Laws can change because of the biases in our heads, but our biases can also change as a result of laws and cultural shifts. I know that you don't think of yourself as being a religious person,
Starting point is 00:47:03 but again, I'm struck by something you told me about what it means to be a good par-sea. You know, the good par-sea recognizes that the world has good and evil and has to try and make a choice every day with side they're on. Do you feel like you do that in your own life? I mean, consciously, yes. Almost, almost like writing in a book. One of the things I teach about is that our ancestors had very clear evidence every day about the harm
Starting point is 00:47:34 that they did to people who were not like them. They would get on a horse, they would go into some neighboring village and loot it and bring their stuff back to theirs. So at the end of the day, if you asked, our ancestors on the Tundra, did you harm somebody who was not like you? They would say, damn right, I did. They would have direct evidence. You and I live in such a protected and privileged world that we don't have to do that every day. We don't have the experience of harming people who are different from us. So how is it that we discriminate and we do? We do it in a very paradoxical way.
Starting point is 00:48:10 We do it by who we help. And I think that this is where have I been a good person is no longer a simple question. Because if I help people from my own tribe, which I sure I do, I should not count that in the good column, until I've done a compensating behavior in the other column, which is very hard to do. And which is why institutions and governments have to enter? It is because you and I, as individuals, will help. If my friend calls me up and says,
Starting point is 00:48:47 my son is not doing well, can he come and spend a summer in your lab, I will say yes. And I don't think I want to be the kind of person who doesn't do that. But if that's happening, then my institution needs to have a program by which people who are not the children of my friend can visit my lab. And this is why I think helping can often be the way in which we keep the world unequal and yet we don't count it as something that we've done that is not something we should be proud of. So you see how it's complicated and yet every day, when I do the kind of work and see the data that come in,
Starting point is 00:49:34 I am being transformed in what I think is to go into the good and bad column of the Zorastrian ledger. Maasareen Panaji is a psychologist at Harvard University. Along with Tony Greenwald, she's the author of Blind Spot, Hidden Biasis of Good People. Maasareen, thank you for joining me today on Hidden Brighton. Thank you for having me. Always a pleasure. Some weeks ago, we ran an experiment, and we would like to do it again. We're exploring the possibility of regular follow-up conversations in which our listeners can pose their questions to our guests. If you have questions or thoughts
Starting point is 00:50:31 about our series with Maserine Banagji and are willing to have those questions shared with a larger hidden brain audience, please record a voice memo on your phone and email it to us at ideasathydenbrain.org. 60 seconds is plenty. Please remember to include your name and a phone number where we can reach you. Again, email the questions to us at ideasathydenbrain.org and use the subject line, implicit bias episodes. Hidden Brain is produced by Hidden Brain Media. Our audio production team includes Bridget McCarthy, Ani Murphy-Paul, Kristen Wong, Laura Quarelle, Ryan Katz, Autumn Barnes, and Andrew Chadwick.
Starting point is 00:51:19 Tara Boyle is our executive producer. I'm Hidden Brain's executive editor. Special thanks for this episode to Sound Designer Nick Woodbury. Our run sunk hero today is listener and Hidden Brain supporter, Brendan Smith of Oakland, California. Brendan says he likes to listen to Hidden Brain while walking with his golden retriever, Max. We're really glad to be part of your walks with Max, Brendan.
Starting point is 00:51:46 Thanks so much for your support. If you found this episode thought-provoking, and you would like to join Brendan in helping us to make more episodes like this, please do your part to keep us thriving. Help us build more shows for new listeners. Visit support.hiddenbrain.org. I'm Shankar Vedantam. See you soon.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.