Hidden Brain - Why No One Feels Rich
Episode Date: April 23, 2019If you've ever flown in economy class on a plane, you probably had to walk through the first class cabin to get to your seat. Maybe you noticed the extra leg room. The freshly-poured champagne. Maybe ...you were annoyed, or envious. Social psychologist Keith Payne says we tend to compare ourselves with those who have more than us, but rarely with those who have less. This week, we explore the psychology of income inequality, and how perceptions of our own wealth shape our lives.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta.
Let's say you make $500,000 a year.
That puts you in the top 1% of all Americans,
and far ahead of almost everyone else in the world.
That's out the right center field again at the scene.
What if that $500,000 salary comes from being
a professional baseball player.
For the Los Angeles Angels.
When you look over your shoulder in the locker room, you see this other guy, Mike.
Two potential news now of the biggest contract in baseball history.
Today, we learned that LA Angel's star, Mike,
trout is finalizing a deal to stay with that team for another 12 years and to earn $430 million
over that time.
430 million dollars over 12 years or 36 million dollars a year, which makes your $500,000 look measly.
As we walk through the world, we are constantly comparing and contrasting our lives with those of others.
We think about ourselves in terms of being on a certain wrong with some people above us and other people below us.
Where we think we stand on that ladder tells you a lot about a person's life and their life outcomes.
This week on Hidden Brain, the psychology of inequality.
How our predisposition to compare ourselves to others affects our minds and our bodies. Keith Payne is a psychologist at the University of North Carolina.
He is the author of the Broken Ladder, How Inequality Effects the Way We Live, Think, and Die.
Keith, welcome to Hidden Brain.
Thanks so much for having me.
I want to begin with a personal story that you've told
in the book and elsewhere, Keith. This goes back to your childhood when you were in the fourth grade.
You were standing in line at a cafeteria in your school
and you had your first visceral experience
with the awareness of inequality.
Tell me that story.
What had happened was we had a new cashier, a new lunch lady in the line that day.
When I got to the cashier's desk, she asked me for, I think it was $1.25.
That was the first time that anybody had ever asked me to pay for my lunch
because I had always been on free lunch and but I didn't know it because nobody
had ever pointed it out or talked about it. And previously the cashier had just
waved me on as part of the normal process but this new person didn't know how
things worked and so she asked me to pay for my lunch and there was this awkward moment.
I didn't have any money of course and I didn't know what to do about it and so that moment of
awkwardness made me suddenly realize that wait some of these kids have been paying for their lunch
all along and some of us haven't. And all of a sudden it dawned on me why that was, that we got free lunch, that meant
that we were the poor kids, and that that had never occurred to me before.
And so that awkward moment standing in the lunch line suddenly increased my awareness of
not only the inequality in my classroom, but the implications of what it meant to be one of the poor kids.
And so I started thinking about myself differently. I started seeing my friends and my peers differently.
And all of a sudden, this relative difference between
me and the other free lunch kids versus those who paid for their lunch,
all of a sudden loomed larger than it ever had before, at least for me.
And the interesting thing is, you know, that it's not like I was poorer the day after
that than I was before.
Nothing objectively had changed, but because of that subjective awareness, now everything
seemed different to me.
I want to talk about a wonderful analogy that you've explored in the book, and it really
grows out in some ways of this conversation we're having about what happened in your
school lunch cafeteria.
Let's say I'm boarding a plane, and on my way to seat 36J in economy as I make my way down the aisle of a traditional
plane, tell me what I see and how that affects me.
So usually you enter at the front of the plane and you're walking down the aisle past the
first class section with the large seats in the leg room and everybody's already settled in because
they boarded first.
And you're walking past that to go to where I usually sit, which is in coach, right?
So as you go to find your way, you're literally walking along this sort of status hierarchy,
this laid out in front of you.
And so there's the wonderful study that shows the psychological and behavioral consequences
of experiencing that kind of hierarchy embedded in the airplane.
The researchers looked at data from millions of flights to look at what predicted incidents
of air rage, that is cases where passengers were unruly or disruptive or violent in some
way.
And they found that in planes that had a first-class cabin,
incidents of air rage were several times more likely to happen
than in flights that didn't have a first-class cabin,
which suggests that to witness that inequality seems to have some kind of
psychological effect on people
that really ramps up the disruptive behavior. to witness that inequality seems to have some kind of psychological effect on people that
really ramps up the disruptive behavior.
Now it's worth noting that planes with a first-class cabin might be larger with more passengers
and longer flights.
More people and more time spent in the air could also increase the likelihood of air rage.
But there are some other indications that seeing the inequality between first class
and coach does affect passengers. For example, some planes don't start boarding with their first class
passengers. They board from the rear of the aircraft and Keith says that makes a difference.
Yeah, there's a difference in the sense that if you board a plane in the middle or at the rear and don't
have to walk past the first class cabin, there's a much lower incidence of air rage in the
coach cabin.
I remember a scene in the movie, Bridesmaids, Christian Wigg plays a woman named Annie.
She's an economy, but all her friends are flying first class.
Miss, you cannot be up here.
You little grandpa.
Mm-hmm.
I'm sorry.
I just want to be here with my friends,
because I'm with this group.
The sign just went off.
Can't she stamp a flick of it?
Absolutely not.
Coach passengers are not allowed up here in first class.
It's policy.
I'm sorry.
Oh, this is a very, this is a very strict blame that I'm on.
Welcome to Germany.
On futils in.
So Keith, of all of Annie's friends or an economy,
she would feel a lot better, wouldn't she?
Right, the feeling that she deserves to be in first class
because her friends are in first class is really powerful because you know
if she didn't have any connection to the people in first class, she might pay less attention to them.
And so it's that feeling that not only do I not have something that other people have,
but I deserve that thing that other people have that makes that relative comparison so much more painful.
Why is it that the comparisons we make are invariably upward comparisons?
So if the folks in coach are comparing themselves to the folks in first class, the folks in first
class are probably comparing themselves to the folks who fly private jets.
There's a pronounced tendency we have to make upward comparisons in all areas of life.
And it's not always the case.
Sometimes we compare downward or to other people who are similar to us, but we have this
pronounced bias to compare upward.
And the reasons for that upward bias are not fully understood, but it seems to have something to do with the fact that upward comparisons
on the one hand feel painful
because you're comparing to somebody
who has something that you don't.
But on the other hand, they're also potentially inspiring,
potentially motivating,
and so they can sort of get you moving
to work harder in some cases as well.
So there's something adaptive about it, but also something painful.
I remember whenever there's stories in the press about inequality
and you read the comment section,
you will invariably find someone writing in from New York's Upper West Side,
saying a million dollars isn't what you think it is.
It really doesn't get you all that much. I'm barely keeping up with all the people around me. But
this is how nearly everyone feels at every income level.
That's right. And it's incredibly frustrating for ordinary people to read those kind of
comments. But you can kind of see the psychology at work, right? Because if you're a middle-class
person being frustrated at that New Yorker saying that, just think Because if you're a middle class person being frustrated at that, uh, New York
or saying that, just think about how you're viewed by other people who are not middle class
Americans, either they're people who live in poorer countries around the world or they're
people who are struggling to make insmeat in the United States, that same dynamic happens
all up and down the income ladder.
Now, we increasingly live in a world where you have
extremes of inequality.
The richest, you know, 100 people in the world
probably have more wealth than the bottom,
maybe the bottom half of the people in the world.
Tell me about the title of your book,
what do you mean by the broken ladder?
One of the images that I used throughout the book to capture the relative differences between
people is this idea of a status ladder that we think about ourselves in terms of being
on a certain wrong with some people above us and other people below us.
And where we think we stand on that ladder tells you a lot about a person's life and their
life outcomes.
And as the scale of inequality gets larger and larger as it's been doing in the United
States and other advanced economies around the world, it becomes harder and harder to
climb that ladder for a number of reasons.
And so when the scale of inequality gets out of proportion to what we can psychologically
handle, the ladder is essentially broken. scale of inequality gets out of proportion to what we can psychologically handle.
The latter is essentially broken.
It becomes harder and harder to occupy the wrong that we think we ought to be on.
If we go back for a second to that moment in the school lunch cafeteria and something
you said back then, which is the moment after the incident was over, you were not objectively poorer than you were a moment before the incident took place, but you felt
poorer. In the same way, if you extrapolate that across the whole economy, it might actually
be that the poor are not necessarily poorer than they were 50 years ago. They might not
have gained very much or even gained anything at all, but they might feel poorer because
the rich have gotten so much or even gained anything at all, but they might feel poorer because the rich
have gotten so much richer. That's right. If you look at actual income changes over the last 40 or
50 years, the people in the middle group who might consider middle class incomes haven't made
any more or less on average than they did back in the 1960s and 70s.
The poorest groups and the middle class groups are basically staying the same,
but that stagnation feels like people are falling behind by comparison to
the top 20%, the top 10%, and the top 1%,
who have made so much more money over the last several decades.
So staying in place feels like falling behind if other people are getting so much further ahead.
In other words, feelings of opulence and poverty are not merely shaped by objective facts.
Feelings of opulence and poverty are not merely shaped by objective facts. An individual making around $50,000 a year is in the top 1% of all incomes globally.
In the United States, people learning that much don't feel like 1%ers.
When we come back, how these feelings affect our lives.
Keith, tell me what the experience of inequality does to our minds.
Tell me about the range of different effects
that you and others have identified in terms of what inequality does to the ways our minds work.
Inequality does a few different things to the way that both our minds and our bodies respond.
One is that it makes us focused on the short term. It makes us impulsive, focused on the here and now. And it makes us more willing to seek out risks
and engage in high-risk, high-reward sort of gambling
behaviors.
It also affects our bodies in ways that
are similar to physical threats.
So the social threat of feeling lower
on the status ladder than somebody else
provokes very similar reactions physiologically
to a physical stressor.
So we react as if we were about to be physically attacked.
We react with stress responses as if we were about
to face a literal physical challenge.
And the total of all of those effects
adds up to feeling that we're constantly in crisis.
Tell me about the difference in the homicide rate
between countries that are equal
and countries that are less equal.
If you look across countries,
one of the strong predictors of homicide rates
as well as other kinds of violent
crime, is the level of income inequality in those countries. Now, we have to make an important distinction
here between wealthy developed countries and poorer countries, because in poorer countries, the best
predictor of crime and lots of other bad health and social outcomes is actually poverty.
Because we're talking about countries in which poor people may not have their basic physical
needs met.
But when we look at wealthy countries like the countries of Western Europe and North America,
poverty ceases to be the strong predictor of things like homicide rates and inequality
becomes the stronger predictor. Because once people's basic physical needs are met,
it becomes the relative comparisons to other people that becomes one of the
major stressors that affect people. Now of course it's important to mention that
some of these findings are correlations. You're basically looking at patterns,
you're not actually conducting an experiment where you're varying how unequal
people are and then measuring the outcomes and things like homicide.
But tell me about another correlation that people have looked at that has to do with politics,
the relationship between inequality and political polarization.
If you compare either across countries or within the United States over time, you see a pretty
strong correlation between the level
of income inequality and the level of political polarization.
So it's not that inequality makes people more conservative
or more liberal per se, it's that people who are already
in those, on those sides, go further into their corners
when inequality is high.
What's driving the the fact that people are retreating into their separate camps?
There seems to be a back and forth dynamic between how much money people have
and what they see around them and how certainly feel in their own opinion.
So people who are wealthy tend to feel that they deserve it and they
earned it and that tends to make them favor more sort of free market policies
and ideologies and people who are struggling who are working hard to get ahead
but can't seem to do so tend to feel like the market system is rigged against
them. They favor more progressive taxation
and redistribution policies,
and both sides tend to think that the system is rigged
largely by the other side.
And so, the spreading of inequality in terms of money
has also spread apart our politics
and increased our confidence that we're right
and the other side are not only wrong,
but out to get us.
So, you know, as I was preparing for this interview, Keith,
I came by this interesting study that Daniel Zizo had conducted.
He gave money to volunteers, some got more, some got less,
and he found that volunteers were willing to spend their own money
If it allowed them to reduce the money that other people had, Zizzo called this burning other people's money
And he found that a substantial number of people were willing to reduce their own wealth if they could also reduce the wealth of other people
In other words, inequality has such strong
effects on us that we are willing to make great sacrifices to level the playing field.
And from an economic point of view, that's just crazy behavior, right? But from a psychological
point of view, it makes perfect sense because we're not judging these things the way an
accountant or an economist would, we're thinking thinking what do I have compared to what that other guy has and that relative
sense of entitlement and having enough compared to what other people have is so powerful.
Keith the researcher is Michael Norton and Dan Ariela once asked volunteers what kind
of a country they would like to live in? The first option was a country where the top 20% owned a third of the wealth and the bottom 20% owned 10% of the wealth.
Option 2, the top 20% owned 84% of the wealth, the bottom 20% owned 0.1% of the wealth.
What do the volunteers say?
Most people chose a an overwhelming margin,
the more equal option. And of course this was not even a small difference. 92% of Americans
apparently chose the first option and that first option doesn't look a lot like America,
does it? No, in fact, those pie charts that you just described were taken from actual
data, the first one which describes Sweden and
the second very unequal one which actually describes the United States.
Almost universally, people preferred the first option to the second.
The survey, which was conducted in 2005, found that of those who had voted in the last presidential election, 90% of those
who voted for a Republican and 94% who voted for a Democrat, said they preferred option
1 to option 2.
I ask Keith why this preference for equality doesn't get translated into policy.
What explains the mismatch between what we say we want and what we do?
Well the important thing in that study was that the charts demonstrating those
different levels of inequality were not labeled right and so if you were to add
the labels that this is the United States and that Sweden now people would start
sorting themselves out and choosing the United States or Sweden based on their beliefs.
And for those on the right, they see it through largely through a lens of meritocracy where
you work for what you get and you deserve whatever that outcome is.
Whereas people who are more on the left see it through a lens of fairness and differential
starting places.
So the gulf there gets mixed up with lots
of aspects of our identities and that makes it very difficult to discuss.
So let's look at a specific example where perhaps conservatives and liberals would think
very differently. The baseball player Mike Mike Trout, was recently awarded a contract worth nearly half a billion
dollars.
Let me ask you, Keith Payne.
Do you think the Los Angeles Angels are paying Mike Trout too much?
Well, if you look at teams, whether it's baseball or basketball, that have extreme levels
of inequality,
which are driven by paying superstars, astronomical salaries,
they don't outperform other teams that have more equality.
And that's counterintuitive if you think that
paying the superstars huge salaries means that
they're going to work harder and perform better.
You would expect better scores and more wins.
But in fact, in team sports like that,
higher levels of inequality and extreme pay
for the superstars is associated with poorer performance
and subsequent seasons.
And of course, this idea might be explained
what you said earlier, which is that even if you buy
the idea that maybe the superstar does perform very well, the resentment and unfairness that others experience might
and come at a cost to the team's performance.
That's right, the team coordination seems to be a critical factor here.
So extreme disparities like that, overall, tend to be destructive to teamwork and cooperation.
So in team sports, that interferes with overall performance.
Although in other kinds of sports,
like golf or NASCAR racing,
where it's just the individual performing their best,
their higher stakes, larger pay,
do seem to incentivize better performance
for the individual.
So let's think about what happened to Mike Trout,
just a little bit more closely.
Let's say that the angels read your book and they decided we want to do away with inequality
and let's take the extreme thought experiment example where they basically said we're
going to pay everyone on the team the same amount. We're going to pay everyone, let's
say there are 40 players on the team, everyone gets $4 million instead of some people getting
$500,000 and some people getting $30 million.
Do you think that would be a better outcome?
And what kind of problems do you think it would cause if the angels would do that?
Well, there are trade-offs here.
So probably your highest paid superstars would be very angry at that and maybe they would
leave the team.
So that would be a problem. But if you had a more equal
level of compensation, it would also increase teamwork and coordination. So that would benefit the
team. So in most cases, the argument here is not for absolute equality so that everybody makes
the same. It's that, you know, people should be compensated in proportion to their contributions.
people should be compensated in proportion to their contributions, but the scale
could oftentimes be much more compressed so that the superstar isn't making 30, 40, 50 times what other people are making and could still be compensated in proportion to how much
they're bringing to the team.
I want to switch gears just a little bit. The newspaper The Sacramento Bee published the salaries of every California state employee.
What happened when people discovered that they were being paid less or more
than other people who are working for the state of California?
The effects of learning what you made in comparison to what other people made depended on whether you were a higher earner or a lower earner to begin with.
So people who were below average when they learned that other people were making much more than them, they felt very dissatisfied.
when they learned that other people were making much more than them, they felt very dissatisfied. People who were higher up when they found out how well they were doing compared to other people,
ironically it didn't have the same effect. They didn't feel overjoyed at the fact that they were
doing so well. Their reaction was just sort of neutral. So they weren't any happier, but the people at the bottom were made more unhappy.
So when you look at the example of the Sacramento Bee and the publication of the salaries,
it raises a question in my head about whether transparency is a good thing or a bad thing.
Obviously, as a journalist, I believe that transparency is a good thing, but in this case,
if you make salaries transparent
and then people end up quitting their jobs and leaving,
is that a good outcome?
Well, I don't know whether transparency and pay itself
is necessarily a good outcome or a bad outcome,
but what I think transparency shows
is the underlying assumptions that we have.
So if you think that incentivizing good performance
with very high pay is a good thing,
then you would want your company
to have full transparency.
So everybody in the company could see
that the superstars were getting paid a lot
and that other people weren't getting paid so much
and the people who are the poor performers
are getting paid very poorly.
If your idea is that pay and equity increases performance, you should want total transparency.
And yet what we have is a system in which most companies, most organizations want to keep
their pay and equality secret.
And then social norms are not to talk about these things, which means that at some level,
we must kind of know that there's something else going on besides just incentivizing good performance. We know at some level that
there's this negative feeling surrounding talking about differences in pay and that that's likely
to have some bad outcomes as well. When you looked at the example of the aircraft studies that you told me about at the start
of our conversation, you mentioned that in aircraft that do not board passengers from
the front or aircrafts that don't have first-class cabin at all, you have fewer of these problems.
Now, of course, you're going to have rich people and slightly less rich people on those
airplanes as well, but the differences are not visible and those differences therefore
don't translate into some of the problems that you are describing. It raises the question again,
if you have inequalities in a society, an organization, a community, are we better off making them
visible or better off keeping them invisible? It's a good question.
There's no simple answer because on the one hand, when people are economically segregated
and you have the wealthy living behind gated communities and very separate from the way
ordinary people are living and the way poor people are living, it might be psychologically
easier on the middle class and the poor people not to
see that.
But on the other hand, it creates a greater feeling of distance between the haves and have
knots and less of a feeling of community.
And you know, that leads to increased polarization and lower levels of trust.
So there's no simple answer in terms of whether we want highly visible or highly transparent inequality
versus less visible and less transparent inequality because there's always a
trade-off at work between what feels good versus what has negative consequences
down the road.
Your psychologist's Keith, and you know that there are several small warehouses filled with studies that show that comparing ourselves to others is often a recipe for unhappiness.
It's clear that at a societal level, we need to sort of take heed of the lessons of inequality, the psychological
consequences of inequality.
Does it also translate to the individual level,
our individuals better off thinking about inequality
or better off not thinking about it?
I think there's not really any option
of not thinking about it because we're constantly
making social comparisons to what others have around us to define what's normal and what's enough.
But I think there are wiser and less wise ways to think about inequality and to make those
social comparisons.
So one of the things I recommend in the book is that we can be more strategic in making
upward versus downward social comparisons.
We talked earlier about how people tend to, by default,
make upward social comparisons to those who have more than them. But we can strategically make
downward social comparisons also, right? So instead of just thinking about what other people
have who have more than us, we can remind ourselves that, you know, there are a lot of people around
who have less than us.
And upward and downward social comparisons have opposite consequences.
Upward social comparisons, as I mentioned before, feel terrible, but they can be motivating.
Downward social comparisons feel great, and yet they can be demotivating.
So it depends on what your goal is.
Do you want to take a break from feeling stressed?
Well, then do some downward
social comparison. Do you want to feel motivated and energized to go out and do more, do some upward
social comparisons? So neither one is good in itself. It's just that we can be more mindful about the kinds of comparisons we're making on a daily basis.
Keith Bain is a psychologist at the University of North Carolina. He's the author of the
Broken Ladder, How Inequality Effects the Way We Live, Think and Die. Keith, thanks for joining me
today on Hidden Brain. Thanks so much.
This week's show was produced by Path Shal. It was edited by Tara Boyle and Raina Cohen.
Our team includes Thomas Liu, Laura Quarral and Jenny Schmitt.
includes Thomas Liu, Laura Quarell and Jenny Schmidt. Our unsung hero today is Keith Woods.
Keith is Vice President of Newsroom Training and Diversity at NPR.
He's helped establish NPR's training program, creating opportunities for journalists to
learn more about the craft of storytelling.
Keith is the epitome of the inclusive leader, always ready to share his
thoughts or to lend a hand. Thanks Keith.
If you liked this episode, please share it with a friend. I'm Shankar Vedantan and
this is NPR.
you