Huberman Lab - Dr. Laurie Santos: How to Achieve True Happiness Using Science-Based Protocols

Episode Date: December 23, 2024

In this episode, my guest is Dr. Laurie Santos, Ph.D., a professor of psychology and cognitive science at Yale University and a leading researcher on happiness and fulfillment. We discuss what truly i...ncreases happiness, examining factors such as money, social comparison, free time, alone time versus time spent with others, pets, and the surprising positive impact of negative visualizations. We also explore common myths and truths about introverts and extroverts, the science of motivation, and how to adjust your hedonic set point to experience significantly more joy in daily life. Throughout the episode, Dr. Santos shares science-supported strategies for enhancing emotional well-being and cultivating a deeper sense of meaning and happiness. Read the full show notes at hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1: https://drinkag1.com/huberman Eight Sleep: https://eightsleep.com/huberman ExpressVPN: https://expressvpn.com/huberman Function: https://functionhealth.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman David: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Timestamps 00:00:00 Dr. Laurie Santos 00:02:52 Sponsors: Eight Sleep & ExpressVPN 00:06:00 Happiness, Emotion & Cognition; Emotional Contagion 00:11:18 Extrinsic vs. Intrinsic Rewards 00:14:43 Money, Comparison & Happiness 00:21:39 Tool: Increase Social Connection; Real-Time Communication 00:32:16 Sponsor: AG1 00:33:47 Technology, Information, Social Interaction 00:39:22 Loneliness, Youth, Technology 00:42:16 Cravings, Sustainable Actions, Dopamine 00:47:01 Social Connection & Predictions; Introverts & Extroverts 00:57:22 Sponsors: Function & LMNT 01:00:41 Social Connection & Frequency; Tools: Fun; “Presence” & Technology 01:07:53 Technology & Negative Effects; Tool: Senses & Grounding; Podcasts 01:15:11 Negativity Bias, Gratitude, Tool: “Delight” Practice & Shifting Emotions 01:25:01 Sponsor: David  01:26:17 Importance of Negative Emotions; Judgements about Happiness 01:34:16 Happiness & Cultural Differences, Tool: Focus on Small Pleasures 01:41:00 Dogs, Monkeys & Brain, “Monkey Mind” 01:47:40 Monkeys, Perspective, Planning 01:53:58 Dogs, Cats, Dingos; Pets & Happiness 02:00:49 Time Famish; Tools: Time Affluence Breaks; Time Confetti & Free Time  02:07:46 Hedonic Adaptation; Tool: Spacing Happy Experiences 02:15:27 Contrast, Comparison & Happiness; Tool: Bronze Lining, Negative Visualization 02:24:08 Visualization, Bannister Effect; Tool: Imagine Obstacles 02:29:12 Culture; Arrival Fallacy, Tool: Journey Mindset 02:37:11 Mortality, Memento Mori, Tool: Fleeting Experiences & Contrast 02:44:33 Awe  02:48:15 Timescales; Community Engagement & Signature Strengths; Tool: Job Crafting 02:56:55 Strength Date, Leisure Time; Tool: Doing for Others, Feel Good Do Good 03:01:42 Tool: Asking for Help 03:05:32 Zero-Cost Support, YouTube, Spotify & Apple Follow & Reviews, Sponsors, YouTube Feedback, Social Media, Protocols Book, Neural Network Newsletter Disclaimer & Disclosures

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast, where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life. I'm Andrew Huberman, and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. My guest today is Dr. Laurie Santos. Dr. Laurie Santos is a professor of cognitive science
Starting point is 00:00:20 and psychology at Yale University. She is a world expert in happiness and in the science of emotions generally. Today we talk about true happiness, not in any kind of loose and aspirational way, but instead what the research really tells us about how to create lasting happiness for ourselves. We talk about relationships and happiness,
Starting point is 00:00:42 that is relationships of all kinds, between friends, between romantic partners, between family members, and of course, with ourselves. We talk about all of that in the context of what to do, what not to do, and how to frame your whole notion of what happiness is and how to attain it in the context of daily to-dos. For instance, most all of us by now have heard
Starting point is 00:01:02 about the power of gratitude and gratitude practices. In fact, I've done an entire episode about gratitude and the science of gratitude. But Dr. Laurie Santos today explains that by shifting our orientation toward gratitude, towards something more aligned with what delights us, we are able to better tap into the mechanisms that enable us to feel happier in a more pervasive way. We also discuss topics such as hedonic adaptation, that is how our pursuit of things and our whole experience of pleasure sets the stage
Starting point is 00:01:31 for what's going to feel like a meaningful pursuit and pleasureful in the days and weeks to follow. This is very important for everyone to hear, especially in this modern age of so-called dopamine hits, easy to achieve dopamine, highly processed foods, and the various things that you can find online. And speaking of online, we also discussed the role that smartphones
Starting point is 00:01:51 and social media play, not just in our happiness, but in our cognition. You'll be shocked, indeed I was shocked to learn that just having your phone in the room where you are trying to learn something, significantly diminishes your performance on things like mathematics and the learning of other topics. We get into all of that today, the interrelated parts,
Starting point is 00:02:09 and I promise that it's all made extremely clear and actionable thanks to Dr. Laurie Santos's incredible expertise. And she is an incredible teacher. In fact, the course that she has taught at Yale University entitled Psychology and the Good Life is the most popular course ever taught at Yale over entitled Psychology and the Good Life is the most popular course ever taught at Yale over the course of 300 years.
Starting point is 00:02:29 And that popularity will not come as a surprise as you now get to learn from Dr. Laurie Santos directly. This was a remarkable episode, I must say. I learned so much. And I'll just highlight one big takeaway that I've implemented in my own life and that you can frame in the back of your mind as you listen to today's episode is the difference between being happy with one's life
Starting point is 00:02:47 as opposed to in one's life and indeed how to achieve both. Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. It is however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information about science and science related tools
Starting point is 00:03:04 to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. Our first sponsor is 8 Sleep. 8 Sleep makes smart mattress covers with cooling, heating and sleep tracking capacity. Now I've spoken many times before on this podcast about the critical need for us to get adequate amounts
Starting point is 00:03:19 of quality sleep each night. That's truly the foundation of all mental health, physical health and performance. And one of the best ways to ensure that you get a great night's sleep is to control the temperature of your sleeping environment. And that's because in order to fall and stay deeply asleep, your body temperature actually has to drop by about one to three degrees. And in order to wake up feeling refreshed and energized, your body temperature actually has to increase about one to three degrees. Eight Sleep makes it incredibly easy to control the temperature of your sleeping environment by allowing you to control
Starting point is 00:03:47 the temperature of your mattress cover at the beginning, middle and end of the night. I've been sleeping on an Eight Sleep mattress cover for nearly four years now, and it has completely transformed and improved the quality of my sleep. Eight Sleep has now launched their newest generation of the pod cover, the Pod 4 Ultra. The Pod 4 Ultra has improved cooling and heating capacity,
Starting point is 00:04:05 higher fidelity sleep tracking technology, and even has snoring detection that will automatically lift your head a few degrees to improve your airflow and stop your snoring. If you'd like to try an 8Sleep mattress cover, go to 8sleep.com slash Huberman to save up to $350 off their Pod4 Ultra. 8Sleep currently ships in the USA, Canada, UK,
Starting point is 00:04:24 select countries in the EU and Australia. Again, that's 8Sleep.com slash Huberman. Today's episode is also brought to us by ExpressVPN. ExpressVPN is a virtual private network that keeps your data secure and private. It does that by routing your internet activity through their servers and encrypting it so that no one can see or sell your data.
Starting point is 00:04:44 Now I'm familiar with the effects of not securing my data well enough. Several years ago, I had one of my bank accounts hacked, and it was a terrible amount of work to have that reversed and for the account to be secured. When that happened, I talked to my friends in the tech community, and what they told me was that even though you think your internet connection may be secure, oftentimes it's not, especially if you're using Wi-Fi networks such as those on planes, in hotels, at coffee shops, and other public places. Surprisingly, even at home,
Starting point is 00:05:10 your data might not be as secure as you think. To make sure that what I described before would never happen to me again, I started using ExpressVPN. The great thing about ExpressVPN is that I don't even notice that it's running, since the connection it provides is so fast. I have it on my computer and on my phone
Starting point is 00:05:26 and I keep it on whenever I'm connected to the internet. With ExpressVPN, I know everything is secure. My web browsing, all my passwords, all my data, and of course, anything that's behind an account wall like a bank account. It can't be tracked and no one can access or steal your data, which is terrific. If you'd like to start protecting your internet activity
Starting point is 00:05:42 using ExpressVPN, you can go to expressvpn.com slash Huberman and you can get an extra three months free. Again, that's E-X-P-R-E-S-S-V-P-N.com slash Huberman to get an extra three months free. And now for my discussion with Dr. Laurie Santos. Dr. Laurie Santos, welcome. Thanks so much for having me on the show. I wanna talk about this thing that everyone seems to want,
Starting point is 00:06:07 but most everyone has trouble keeping themselves in a state of happiness, which raises the question of whether or not we should even be seeking to constantly be in a state of happiness. But just to sit back from that question for a moment, how should we think about the relationship between emotions and this thing that we call cognition? Because I think a lot of where we're going today
Starting point is 00:06:31 is to distinguish between feelings, thoughts, and behaviors. And as neuroscientists, psychologists, et cetera, we have to understand the difference between emotions and cognition and maybe where they overlap. So if you could educate us a bit on that, I think that will set the stage nicely for understanding happiness. Yeah. Well, I'm glad you started there actually because, you know, the very definition of happiness, I think, as social scientists tend to think about it includes both of these parts, right?
Starting point is 00:07:00 So I think social scientists tend to think about happiness as being happy in your life and being happy with your life. So being happy in your life is sort of the emotion side, right? I have a decent number of positive emotions, maybe slightly less negative emotions. Like you existing in your life feels good. That's kind of an emotional part, right? But then there's also kind of how you think your life is going. Do you have purpose?
Starting point is 00:07:20 Are you kind of happy with how things are going? It's how you think about your life, which is sort of a cognitive thing. And so even the earliest social scientists who started thinking about happiness, at the time they call it subjective well-being, because I think psychologists were like, ooh, happiness sounds too wooey, like we'll call it something else, but it means exactly the same thing. It means subjective well-being, right? When they started thinking about subjective well-being, they divided it into this sort
Starting point is 00:07:40 of affective emotional part, which is like how you feel in your life, but also this cognitive part, how you think your life is going. So that basic dichotomy has been there since the very beginning of folks studying happiness scientifically. I'm already struck by this distinction between how things are going in your life
Starting point is 00:07:59 versus with your life. One requires a kind of first person experiencing of life in your life. Do requires a kind of first person experiencing of life in your life. Do you wake up feeling good? Are you feeling good with your inside of your friendships and other relationships, family, romantic relationships, school, work?
Starting point is 00:08:16 The other involves a bit of a third personing of self. I'm looking at one's CV, either actual CV or reflected CV through the lens of other people and kind of getting a sense like, am I doing well? Am I not doing well? I think this is a really important distinction because it seems like ultimately the goal, if I may, is to be happy in your life, regardless of the third personing,
Starting point is 00:08:44 provided that you're not doing damage to somebody else's happiness in life. Yeah, well, I think ideally it'd be nice to do both, right? And I think there are moments when these things dissociate, right? So, you know, you interact with lots of interesting, rich people out here in California. I think a lot of them have,
Starting point is 00:08:59 it kind of in their life feels pretty good, right? They have lots of hedonic pleasures, they're drinking nice wine, hanging out. You'd be amazed. You'd be amazed at how much suffering they report. kind of in their life feels pretty good, right? They have lots of hedonic pleasures, they're drinking nice wine, hanging out with the beach. You'd be amazed at how much suffering they report. Oh, that's interesting. How much suffering they report. So this is the question, is this sort of cognitive part
Starting point is 00:09:13 the like third person part, or is it the reporting part? And I think when the psychologists are thinking about it, they really think about it as the reporting part, right? And this gets tricky, right? Because I see folks having their nice glass of wine on the beach, and I'm thinking like, that's coming with lots of positive emotion. Like I bet if I tested them and could have a direct look
Starting point is 00:09:31 at their sensory experience, it'd probably be pretty positive. It's only when they reflect on their life and they're asking, well, how's it going? That they say, oh, I don't know, my stocks went down. Or like- When I hear about lack of happiness, let me think of some of the kind of bullet point ones
Starting point is 00:09:47 that seem to come up repetitively. They are indeed not related to lack of resources. I don't hear that. What I've heard, and this is also true for where I spend part of my time and where I grew up, which is in Silicon Valley, which is also not everyone, but there are people there
Starting point is 00:10:02 who have accrued tremendous amount of wealth. The mean has shifted very high, and hence the cost of living. But it's often concerns about their kids, or their mother is ill, their child is struggling in a particular way. Very often that's what it is. They're concerned about the lack of wellbeing
Starting point is 00:10:27 in their kids related to mental health or physical health or other relatives' mental health, physical health, or they're upset about something politically. But we won't go there. Yeah, no, I think this is true, right? So much of our happiness is made up of the happiness of other people, right? Both kind of how they're doing and how we think they're doing cognitively, but literally just emotionally, right? You know, if you've ever been around a family member or a spouse who was incredibly pissed off, really sad,
Starting point is 00:10:57 it's incredibly hard not to catch those emotions yourself. And we as psychologists know how these processes work, right? These processes are emotional contagion, where you're literally catching the emotions of other people. And so oftentimes the things that you most worry about to be happy yourself is focusing on the happiness of the people around you, because that literally becomes your happiness at a very fundamental level.
Starting point is 00:11:18 Now I'm pausing just to think about this a little bit more. As we grow up, and I realize it varies by place and lots of circumstances, but as we grow up, we are taught to pay attention to how our life is going a bit from the outside. We gain evaluations starting really young. Little stars on our pictures or good job, or nowadays they say great effort in drawing. This whole thing, the growth mindset language.
Starting point is 00:11:49 But I don't know that in the United States, we are taught to think about being happy in our life. Right, as kids, I think all kids, all mammals, seem to gravitate towards joyful experiences for them playing is almost always an innate joyful experience. But then as the evaluations start coming in, we get better and better at assessing our performance and where we are relative to the sort of standard goals
Starting point is 00:12:20 of the third grade, the fifth grade, the 12th grade. But at the same time, I don't think anyone ever sat me down and said, how are you going to evaluate if you're feeling good in your life? Like that you're savoring your soccer game, that you're savoring your time with friends. That was never taught to me.
Starting point is 00:12:40 Yeah, and I think there's a real danger of these kind of extrinsic rewards, as you might call them, all this stuff outside the grades, you know, the performance measures and so on, literally stealing your intrinsic rewards. There's this funny phenomenon in psychology where if you have something that's intrinsically rewarding, so let's say exercise, right? Like I want to go out, you know, and like run like a bunch, right? I love running. I get this intrinsic reward from running. Now I get some sort of tool, whether it's my watch or something I'm scribbling down in a phone app, and I have to like log my running. Now it becomes a sort of extrinsic reward.
Starting point is 00:13:12 It's not just like the feeling of running, but it sort of takes on this extrinsic idea. And then what happens is sometimes we end up going for that reward anyway. The fiction writer David Sedaris has this wonderful article called The Fitbit Life where he talks about how he wanted to get fit. It's intrinsic reward of exercising more and he got the Fitbit and then it was all about the Fitbit and he would set the level higher and set the level
Starting point is 00:13:35 higher and he himself was miserable and no longer enjoying running to the point that like at some point he just would walk around you know shaking his arm just to get up to those final steps right. That's a really terrible case where your extrinsic reward winds up taking over. But so many of the cases you just talked about are ones in our real life where that comes up much more insidiously than with a Fitbit or something like that.
Starting point is 00:13:54 You talked about play in mammals, the easiest thing that little kid animals do all over the place. Little kid humans don't do that as much anymore, because even from really young ages, they're in toddler university where they're kind of learning things to get into the next grade and get the perfect grades so they can get
Starting point is 00:14:11 into institutions like ours, right? It all becomes about extrinsic rewards. And so I think you're really right. We're kind of extrinsic sizing all the rewards to the point that we're not getting to internal happiness. It was hard already to pay attention to that stuff because I think we'll probably talk about this, it's hard to be mindful about your emotions.
Starting point is 00:14:29 You really have to pay attention to what's going on. But I think it's gotten even harder because we have these metrics, they're all over the place in our culture, but they're just not the intrinsic thing. They're some extrinsic marker that could make the intrinsic thing even less fun. For people that grow up or live in areas where,
Starting point is 00:14:47 well, let's just say that have less disposable wealth, is there must be data on sort of relationship to intrinsic versus extrinsic forces on happiness? I mean, I can make up all sorts of stories in my head about how people starting out from very different circumstances would be more or less happy, but what do the data say? Yeah, so these effects of kind of resources on happiness
Starting point is 00:15:13 are really interesting and they're nuanced, right? So if you look at the lower end of the kind of income spectrum, you would obviously say that money affects happiness, right? If you can't put food on the table, if you can't put a roof over your head, definitely getting a little bit morning is going to affect your happiness in a positive way.
Starting point is 00:15:29 And the data sort of bear this out. There's a very famous study by the Nobel Prize-winning economist, Danny Kahneman, RIP. Back in 2010, he did this cool study where he looked at the correlation between income and happiness as reported in how much stress you have How much positive emotion you experience and so on and the low end of the income scales It just goes up and up and up right more money. Just almost linearly gives you more happiness But what Danny found and is the second part of this nuanced picture is that that slope kind of levels off and it levels off in
Starting point is 00:16:01 $2010 at around $75,000. What does that mean? That means if you get more than seventy five thousand dollars, you're not going to feel any less stress. You're not going to experience any more positive emotion. Even if I double or triple or quadruple your income on those metrics, you're not going to see any increase. Then those are pre-tax 2010. Yeah, they didn't get into like the real. You're like, oh, my God, well, I live in California.
Starting point is 00:16:25 Like you live in Iowa, maybe it's not so bad. But like, and those numbers will change. But the upshot is there's probably some number in like 2025, 2024 numbers that might be like, you know, maybe $100,000, $120,000, whatever it is. The point is that there's some number at which getting more is not gonna increase your happiness at the same slope.
Starting point is 00:16:45 Now, there's been nuanced fights about this, as there is a lot in kind of real research, about, well, is that really true? Does the slope really ever go up? And now the picture seems to be, well, the slope might go up a teeny tiny, like negligible bit, but it doesn't go up as much as, say, getting an extra 10 minutes of exercise in or another 20 minutes of sleep, or scribbling the things you're grateful for, all those things will impact your happiness much more than like quintupling your income.
Starting point is 00:17:11 And so do your resources affect happiness? Yeah, if you ain't got any resources, you definitely will feel happier if you can get them. But if you have a lot, getting more really isn't going to help. Sorry to interrupt, but lately I've been saying on the basis of those findings about this then 75K per year, probably now, like you said, 100 to 125K, let's just say something like that would be the equivalent amount that money indeed cannot buy happiness, but it can buffer stress.
Starting point is 00:17:46 Do you think that's true? You're making me rethink that statement. Maybe it doesn't buffer stress past a certain amount. Yeah, I mean, I think in the original Economin data, he found that it doesn't, right? I mean, how much stress you report on a daily basis was literally one of the measures they were using for happiness.
Starting point is 00:18:03 But I think you're right, the risk around it can buffer it, right? I think if you're at a certain set of means, you know that if a bad thing happens, you're going to be okay. So it can allow you to make riskier decisions. It can allow you to do things that you might not do if you're right at that boundary or losing some money, it might pop you back down. I think the problem is that one of the ways we evaluate our financial situation, but pretty much every situation,
Starting point is 00:18:27 I think this goes back to the neuroscience, is that we don't do it objectively, we do it relative. And when you think about your relative financial status, there's lots of other folks around to whom you're comparing yourself. I think one of the reasons that rich folks don't necessarily think they're less stressed when they have very high levels of wealth and so on, is because they're looking around and everyone's doing much better than them. And this is just a
Starting point is 00:18:49 fundamental feature of the way we evaluate stuff, right? Is that we don't evaluate in objective terms, we evaluate relative to these reference points. And honestly, as you get richer, you're kind of going up this sort of logarithmic scale where the reference points are getting even further away from you. And I think that that can have a huge hit on people's perception of their own happiness and their perception of their stress levels, right? Because they're working towards a goal that's probably not going to make them that much happier, but they haven't kind of abandoned this intuition that more money will make me happy. On my podcast, The Happiness Lab, I had this guy, Clay Cockrell, who is really fun. He's a wealth psychologist. So he's a mental health professional
Starting point is 00:19:26 that only works with the 0.0001%. And already we should say, well, if wealth made you happy, he should be out of a job. But no, he has lots of clients, lots of, I guess, very well-paying clients. He looked like he was doing well for himself. But he talks about how those folks haven't abandoned this notion that more money will make them happy.
Starting point is 00:19:43 They set some standard like, as soon as I become, as soon as I get 50 million, I'll be happy, or as soon as I become a billionaire. But then they get to that point, they're not feeling any more positive emotion, they're not feeling less stressed, and rather than saying, well, hang on, maybe that hypothesis was wrong, more money doesn't work, they say, ah, the hypothesis is all right,
Starting point is 00:19:59 more money will make me happy. I just get a, it wasn't 50 million, now it's 100 million or whatever it is. And so I think that that's a lot due to the fact that folks are comparing their wealth levels against others and our comparison system sucks because we constantly compare ourselves against others, but we never pick people that are doing worse than us.
Starting point is 00:20:17 We always pick people who are doing better than us. I know a fair number of very happy, wealthy people. I know a fair number of very happy, wealthy people. I know a fair number of very miserable, wealthy people. I know a fair number of happy, non-wealthy people and a fair number of miserable inside. They report feeling miserable, un-wealthy people. Well, it fits completely with what, you know, a lot of the happiness research suggests, right?
Starting point is 00:20:42 Which is that it's much less about our circumstances than we think when it comes to who's happy and happiness research suggests, right? Which is that it's much less about our circumstances than we think when it comes to who's happy and who's not, right? We often think, if I could get more money or if I could get more accolades at work or if I could get a new partner, if I could move somewhere, I'd be happier. But exactly what you're saying,
Starting point is 00:20:58 if you look at people with all those different life circumstances, both the good version and the bad version, you find some happy folks and some not so happy folks. Now what researchers are starting to think is that it actually doesn't involve our circumstances as much as we think. Again, I like with bracketed,
Starting point is 00:21:10 unless those circumstances are really dire, circumstances don't matter as much as we think. It tends to be the kind of stuff that's much more under our control than our circumstances, right? It tends to be how we behave, what thought patterns we use, the emotions we stake out, the social connection we experience.
Starting point is 00:21:26 Those things matter much more. And so I think your experience with the happy and not so happy rich folks and the happy and not so happy poor folks kind of bear out what we think, it's like, it's just not your circumstances that doesn't matter as much as you assume. Let's talk about this relationship between feelings,
Starting point is 00:21:42 thought patterns and behaviors in the context of happiness. I think anyone listening to this or watching this probably wants to be happy as much as possible. I mean, I suppose there are a few songwriters, poets, and I've got some friends in those domains of life and they do seem to derive a lot of insight and inspiration and have done amazing things through the kind of depths of unhappy human emotion.
Starting point is 00:22:12 We can get back to that later because I do think there's something about the contrast of moving from these more painful emotions to happiness that's very different than moving from a state of immense happiness to slightly less, but we can get back to that. But most people would like to be happy as much as possible. I certainly would, who wouldn't?
Starting point is 00:22:32 And one of course can ask, well, should I work on my feelings? Like think about my feelings, try and shift my feelings, let my feelings move through me in a cathartic way. Should I work on the thought patterns? Should I work on the behaviors? I'm a big believer from my own experience that behaviors are powerful in setting
Starting point is 00:22:52 the general trajectory of thought patterns and feelings, but I've also experienced going the other way too. So what does the research say about this? And what can we do? Because everyone wants to be happier. Yeah, well, we just talked about the thing you're not supposed to do. You don't have to change your circumstances.
Starting point is 00:23:08 And that's great, because quintupling your income is tricky, moving is tricky, switching your life around all over the place is hard, right? And the good news is the science shows you don't have to do that. That doesn't work as well as you think. But you can hack your behaviors and your thought patterns and your feelings to get some good results, right?
Starting point is 00:23:25 Let's let's take behaviors, right? One of the biggest behavioral changes you can make to feel happier is just to get a little bit more social connection Like psychologists do these fun studies where they look at people's like daily usage patterns so how much time are you spending sleeping or exercising or at work or whatever and The two things that predict whether or not you're happy or not so happy is how much time you spend with friends and family members and how much time you're just physically around other people. Like, the more of that you do, the happier you're going to be.
Starting point is 00:23:57 And you know, that's just a correlation, right? So your savvy listeners are thinking right now, like, well, is it that hanging around with other people causes you to be happier or do you tend to, like, hang out with other people more if you are happy? Like, which direction does the causal arrow go? And here we have these lovely studies by psychologists who do these kind of funny experiments where they offer people, like, a $10 Starbucks gift card to just talk to somebody. Usually talk to a stranger, like, that they don't know on the train. This is some lovely work by Nick Epley, and others have done this.
Starting point is 00:24:23 It makes you force people to get social. And what people predict, especially with strangers, is like, ooh, that's going to feel awkward and kind of weird. But what you find across the board, and this includes an introverts and extroverts, is that talking to somebody actually feels good. It increases your positive emotion.
Starting point is 00:24:39 It gives you a sense that your life is going better. You feel less lonely. It just has these positive outcomes that we don't expect. I love social connection. The problem I have with social connection is that if I drop in with somebody for, you know, 30 minutes or a couple of hours, when that's done, I usually have so much that I need to tend to
Starting point is 00:25:00 that I end up staying up later than I need to in order to complete that, diminishing my sleep. And then I feel like there's a underlying kind of like sinking ship sense to my physiology and then I have to recover my sleep. So, you know, everything's a trade-off. Yeah, yeah. What's interesting about the study you just mentioned
Starting point is 00:25:17 is that it's just a brief coffee, presumably. So maybe one doesn't need to spend quite as much time with people. But I think, you know, I think like even years ago, actually he's dead now, but there was a, I guess it's okay to say it even though he's dead. He was a somewhat eccentric professor at UC Berkeley. I took a class from him when I was a graduate student
Starting point is 00:25:40 there named Seth Roberts. He's known for some kind of bizarre theories about eating. And if people want to look this up, I mean,'s known for some kind of bizarre theories about eating. And if people wanna look this up, I mean, like really, really kind of different stuff. But I applaud his bravery and just being out there. But he was an eccentric guy. And he told us in this class when I was there that it was very important to see faces
Starting point is 00:26:00 at least once a day, real faces, not on a screen. And this was before social media. But, and that it was important at some point to leave your apartment and like see the barista and say hello and thank you and see people on the street. And now knowing what we know about these dedicated areas of the brain, like the fusiform face gyrus and Nancy Kanwisher's work
Starting point is 00:26:20 and about these brain areas, like we are hardwired for seeing faces and recognizing faces. Now that alone doesn't mean that seeing faces is a requirement for being happy on a consistent basis, but I think they were onto something. I think Seth was onto something, even though he had some also just like completely crazy ideas.
Starting point is 00:26:39 This idea doesn't seem crazy. This has been my experience, even though I spent a lot of time alone. If I go a few days without seeing a face, something happens inside that shifts my internal kind of set point for wellbeing. And then you see somebody and it's delightful, even if it's just a hello kind of thing.
Starting point is 00:26:58 Yeah, I mean, I think the reason why social connection matters so much is it's building off this basic neural circuitry, right? For seeing faces and so on. I think that gives us a real insight into the kinds of social connections that work best, right, which has been characterized in the field as sort of in real time social connection, right, which we're kind of moving away from. So what do I mean by in real time?
Starting point is 00:27:18 You know, you and I are sitting in a studio right now chatting and we're kind of chatting in real time. I can see your face. We're live. But we might have been able to do this like over some sort of video chat. Wouldn't be as good, you know, but, but it's pretty good. And the reason it seems to be pretty good is we're doing it in real time, right? Our auditory system, our visual system, all these systems that are used to as primates processing things with other folks around you, it works reasonably well.
Starting point is 00:27:41 What doesn't work so well is how we often communicate, which is like over Slack, over text, I text you, vroom, a few minutes later, vroom, it comes back. Like our primate brain's just like, that's just not the way communication is set to work. And so I think sometimes when I bring up social connection, people think like, oh, I got to see people in person and my friend's going to live far away. And I'm like at work all day. It's like, no, no, no, you can connect not necessarily live
Starting point is 00:28:05 and in person, but as much as possible, try to do it in real time. And I think that's in part, and if possible, try to do it with video, I think, for the reason that you were just talking about, is it's faces activate us. But we're primates that are also really good at language and paying attention to the voice.
Starting point is 00:28:20 I think it's one of the reasons that like, an old school phone conversation, no video chat with your friend can be some of the most emotional connective conversation sometimes better than in person because when we're a person we're pulling our phones and checking and paying attention to other stuff But we got to get back towards in real time. The other stuff just doesn't have the same I call a psychological oomph is there any evidence that Texting actually drives more of a desire for more social connection
Starting point is 00:28:48 and thus leaves us feeling less well than prior to a text exchange? I realize it's very hard to separate out the variables about what's the nature of the text exchange. How often do you see this person in real life, et cetera? But I could imagine that texting, the whoop, I don't do the sound effect as well as you do. I like that.
Starting point is 00:29:09 But that texting could be the equivalent of getting crumbs of nourishment, not full nourishment. I could also imagine that it's like putting nourishment just out of reach. And I'm asking this really at a neurological level, do we know is the reward circuitry that's triggered by in real life social connection triggered but to a lesser degree by text exchange
Starting point is 00:29:31 or by Zoom exchange? This would be an important study to do, I think. Yeah, there's not great evidence for it, but my intuition is that the way it works is almost like it's texting sort of the Nutri-Suite of social connection, right? I was feeling this motivation for social connection and I did it and I got something that was sort of social.
Starting point is 00:29:50 I got some information, but like psychologically I'm missing the like nutritious part of it, right? So it kind of fakes you out into thinking that it's social connection, but it kind of doesn't really work. And I worry that that's what we're all getting a lot of right now, right? It's just so much easier to participate in the nutrist version of social connection. Because as political scientists and sociologists
Starting point is 00:30:10 and others have pointed out, it's harder to meet with people in real life. We don't have these so-called third spaces where we can get together easily anymore. There's so many draws of just being on your screen, being alone inside. I think we're kind of missing out. And so a lot of us are kind of starving nutritionally
Starting point is 00:30:26 when it comes to social connection because we're going for the wrong stuff. So schedule some, if possible, in real lifetime with somebody. Or in real time, right? Call that friend that you haven't talked to and recognize, because this is clear from the psychological research,
Starting point is 00:30:42 that your brain is not telling you to do that. Probably even when you're listening right now, you're like, yeah, I guess that would be helpful for me. But you're not kind of having a craving to talk to your friend. And I think this is the problem with a lot of the behaviors that map onto happiness, is that if you think of the evolutionary pressures for those behaviors, natural selection never had to build in the goal of feeling social. Because we were just in these small bands, it was really easy.
Starting point is 00:31:07 Natural selection had to build in a craving for sweet, fatty food, because those were hard to find. Didn't have to build in the craving for a bunch of greens because they were everywhere. I think the same thing is true with social connection. We just don't have a strong motivation to seek people out because it was just there. And so I think our motivation and our reward systems don't cause us to kind of crave it. But in the modern day, where there's so many substitutes
Starting point is 00:31:30 and we're kind of more isolated, I think many of us are kind of experiencing the negative effects of loneliness. But then when we think, well, what can I do to get out of it? There's not this like, I'm starving for connection. We don't have this sort of motivational goal to go out and get it. And so what that can lead to is people making the prediction in their head of like, you
Starting point is 00:31:47 know, I just heard Laurie say that this is a good idea, but like, I don't know, probably not for me or maybe not as important. I think we just don't have systems that tell us to go out and get this stuff. So even if your brain is saying, that's not that important, try it, do your own personal experiment and get a little bit more in real time social connection and just take a moment to notice immediately after how it made you feel. And I bet it'll be like, you know, all the kind of fitness hacks
Starting point is 00:32:10 and nutrition hacks that you talk about on the show where you're like, oh my God, that made me feel so much better than I really expected it to. I'd like to take a quick break and acknowledge our sponsor, AG1. AG1 is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink that also includes prebiotics and adaptogens. AG1 is designed to cover all of your foundational nutritional needs and it tastes great.
Starting point is 00:32:30 Now I've been drinking AG1 since 2012 and I started doing that at a time when my budget for supplements was really limited. In fact, I only had enough money back then to purchase one supplement and I'm so glad that I made that supplement AG1. The reason for that is even though I strive to eat most of my foods from whole foods and minimally processed foods,
Starting point is 00:32:49 it's very difficult for me to get enough fruits, vegetables, vitamins and minerals, micronutrients and adaptogens from food alone. And I need to do that in order to ensure that I have enough energy throughout the day, I sleep well at night and keep my immune system strong. But when I take AG1 daily, I find that all aspects of my health, my physical health, my mental health, and my performance, both cognitive and physical, are better. I know that because I've had lapses when I didn't take AG1 and I certainly felt the difference. I also notice, and this makes perfect sense given the relationship between the gut microbiome
Starting point is 00:33:20 and the brain, that when I regularly take AG1, which for me means a serving in the morning or mid-morning, and again, later in the afternoon or evening, that I have more mental clarity and more mental energy. If you'd like to try AG1, you can go to drinkag1.com slash Huberman to claim a special offer. Right now they're giving away five free travel packs
Starting point is 00:33:38 and a year supply of vitamin D3K2. Again, that's drinkag1.com slash Huberman to claim that special offer. If seeing faces, and I don't have evidence for this, but if Seth Roberts was right, and what we're talking about here is clearly based on existing data, if seeing faces somehow triggers the reward system in a healthy way
Starting point is 00:34:02 that reinforces the social connection thing, like fills the vessel that we're connected because we no longer live in small, small village and tribe type formats. Most of us don't anyway. That if we plop down onto the couch and kind of like assume the classic C shaped position of somebody who's about to go on their phone
Starting point is 00:34:21 and you can scroll and see faces. You talked about that as a bit of like an artificial sweetener giving the illusion of some sort of nourishment. And then, you know, you see some stuff, you respond to stuff, you can see someone kind of dunk on somebody, maybe hear a joke, maybe make a joke, and then go into your DMs and like read a few, check a view. And then you basically got no real social connection.
Starting point is 00:34:43 Correct. You didn't have to move to do it. And in a lot of ways, this has parallels to the ease of highly processed foods or something like that. And I think we're starting to understand this a bit through Jonathan Haidt's work and other people's work, including your own,
Starting point is 00:35:01 but I don't know that it's anything but really dangerous and bad. I don't want to sound alarmist, but I am really concerned that, certainly for the younger generation, but that if we don't have an intrinsic drive to go do something. We stop doing it.
Starting point is 00:35:20 We stop doing it. And then the brain is pretty plastic throughout the entire life, especially for these low grade, like many times repeated behaviors. I mean, we can slowly, you know, it's like there's drift. And then we wonder why we don't feel so good.
Starting point is 00:35:39 Yeah, I mean, you know how the dopamine system works, right? Like it has these mechanisms to crave stuff that's quick, quick hits, right? Our instant, you know, when we go on Reddit or go on Instagram and scroll through a feed, we're getting these kind of quick hits. Another thing that is rewarding is new information. You know, your Stanford College of Miozaki has done these lovely neuroscience studies that just finding out some interesting social information feels rewarding.
Starting point is 00:36:02 And kind of for the first time, we've been able to separate the reward value that comes from interacting with live human people and faces and social rewarding information that comes at us quickly at this dopamine hit that we crave a lot, but we don't have the craving mechanisms for the in real life connection. And yeah, I think that's causing a lot of problems. And it means we're kind of building more tools to do just
Starting point is 00:36:24 that. I had the musician David Byrne on my podcast. Talking heads. Talking heads, David Byrne, who cares a lot about these issues. He wrote this really cool article called Eliminating the Human, where he made the claim that pretty much every technological invention of the last 20 years has been, you know, dealing with actual people is kind of frictiony. So let's just get rid of them 20 years has been, you know, dealing with actual people is kind of frictiony. So let's just get rid of them, right? We'll, you know, have Uber or Lyft or a car company where I don't have to talk to the driver. I just plug it into the phone. We don't have to have a conversation. We go away, right? We have music and streaming
Starting point is 00:36:58 mechanisms. I don't know what you're, Andrew, you're like my age. So you probably remember that you used to have to go into a record store to flip through CDs or tapes even if you're really old school to figure out music. And often when you do that, you'd run into humans or talk to the cashier guy or somebody would see you flicking through like, oh, you like talking heads, I like talking heads. Now we just go to an algorithm, right? From food delivery apps to kind of education, right? I have an online course where students don't have to sit
Starting point is 00:37:25 in a real classroom with other students, they could watch it directly. So many of our technological innovations are assuming that what we wanna get rid of is the friction part. That's what we're kind of motivated to get rid of. But ultimately we're getting rid of like the human and these interactions and our primate brains are left, you know, with the like little nutrisweet dribbles
Starting point is 00:37:41 of connection when what we really need is something in real life and in real time. It's interesting because I think just, but 10, 15 years ago, our knowledge of most all humans was based on in real life experience, except for, I guess, famous humans, and then it was not in real life.
Starting point is 00:37:59 Now, most people's knowledge about most humans is based on not in real life interactions. Which means that most people's knowledge about most humans is based on not in real life interactions. Which means that most people's knowledge about humans generally is kind of accruing through non in real life electronic experiences. And so that has to change our entire scheme of like what human experience is. I'm not trying to, you know,
Starting point is 00:38:22 like ratchet up to something too abstract here, but I think it's a powerful notion that what Byrne is saying that we're dehumanizing ourselves through getting essentially fragments of in real life experience. And video is so captivating as somebody who was a vision scientist for a long time. I mean, if a picture is worth a thousand words, a video is worth 10 billion pictures.
Starting point is 00:38:50 It's just the number of videos that you can access in an Instagram feed or even on an X feed is just astonishing. And then of course the high emotional salient stuff is gonna be the stuff that you hover on. And then the algorithm knows your dwell time as it's called and then your basic feed and discovery is set by that.
Starting point is 00:39:11 And I don't think there's anything really inherently diabolical about it, I don't take that stance. It's just they figured out some good neuroscience based on behavioral forging. Yeah, I mean the diabolical part is having a real consequence for our happiness. It's certainly having a real consequence forging. Yeah. I mean, the diabolical part is, it's having a real consequence for our happiness. It's certainly having a real consequence for loneliness. You look at rates of loneliness in young people who've grown up with these technologies,
Starting point is 00:39:32 and you see things like, you know, young people today report being lonely at rates of like 70, 75%, right? More people are lonely, extremely lonely, than not right now. How do we rate loneliness? I'm not dismissing what they're saying, but since they grew up that way, this sounds very
Starting point is 00:39:48 cross-generational judgment, but how do they know they're lonely? Your point is well taken, right? If anything, they grow up lonely. So if they're self-reporting being lonely now, it might be even worse. Then it might be kind of getting worse over time. Yeah. So it's all self-report data, right?
Starting point is 00:40:01 So people on a scale of 1 to 10, how lonely are you feeling? But the fact that 75% of people are saying, yeah, I feel extremely lonely, that's sad. I mean, our primary ancestors, if they could look at us, would be like, what? These wonderful social brains. They were probably like, oh, I want to go hide behind that rock for a little bit,
Starting point is 00:40:18 get a little bit of space. I'll never forget years ago when I, there was this time when I worked with ferrets, I don't miss it. And they would have these huge litters, and there were these, in these pens, the mom could climb up and get up on top there, and so she'd have these huge litters,
Starting point is 00:40:35 and she'd kick the litter off at some point, she'd go up there and sleep, and you'd go in there to take out the moms, and they did not, that was the only time when they didn't want to be bothered, right? Because they loved to be held and things like that, but they did not want to be bothered because they just needed some peace
Starting point is 00:40:53 because they had like 16 ferret kits, you know? So I think that nowadays, right, if there's a lot of loneliness and people that are growing up in these electronic formats report feeling lonely and I believe them, then what it speaks to is a yearning. And to me, a yearning is a neurological drive, the same way that a room that's too warm,
Starting point is 00:41:14 you wanna get to a cooler space. If it's too cold, you wanna get to heat. So that loneliness speaks to an underlying yearning for something that they're not getting. I'm just stating the obvious, but it says that we're, or they are doing something that's inherently against the grain of their healthy neurology.
Starting point is 00:41:32 The problem is I think what loneliness is a recognition of is you know you kind of don't like this state, but I'm not sure that loneliness is causing people to seek out more social connection. Or if it is, you're seeking out the thing that is the easiest, fastest social connection you can get. This is just like food. Which we've talked about is the nutritious food.
Starting point is 00:41:50 You're not craving vegetables because they were presumably in abundance at one time in our evolutionary history, as opposed to meat or sweets or things like that, fruit and meat. And I think this is a problem with social connection, but I think it's a problem generally with the kinds of things that make us happier because like we just don't have mechanisms to seek those things out
Starting point is 00:42:10 They just kind of don't code in our reward system in the same way as you know The Nutra Sweetie stuff of the world. So what is the the term if there is one or could you come up with one? I don't want to put you on the spot for a fundamental Could you come up with one? I don't want to put you on the spot. For a fundamental desire that's healthy for us that we are not driven to pursue a resolution to. Like for everything else, you know, there's like the hypothalamic circuits for the desire to mate, to seek warmth when it's cold,
Starting point is 00:42:40 cold when it's, you know, when it's too warm. You know, we know what hunger is, right? But there must be something about the, I don't want to get too technical here for those that are tracking this or not tracking this. What I'm trying to say is, for so many of the reward punishment pathways in the human brain, you're trying to avoid the feelings of pain and move towards a feeling of either neutrality or pleasure but here you're talking about being in a sort of place of low-level pain being able to meet that pain with a
Starting point is 00:43:11 Truly low-level pleasure that then it doesn't mask the pain But it fills the vessel just enough that then you drive yourself into a place of More pain, but I think this is the kind of thing that happens when you have easy outs for all these cravings, right? I mean, take processed food, right? You probably have a craving for certain nutritional requirements, right? You wanna get vitamins or healthy stuff, but that stuff's easy, it's frictionless, right?
Starting point is 00:43:35 You know, I run to McDonald's and that's much faster than cooking up a really healthy vegetable-filled meal. I think the same thing happens with social connection, right, like you're a lonely person at your house sitting on the couch, you have this negative bodily state of you feel lonely. Maybe it kind of manifests as a craving, but what's the fastest thing for you to do? I'm going to scroll through my friends' Instagram feeds.
Starting point is 00:43:54 Or I'm going to get a kind of little mini hit of social connection that's not as nutritious. Honestly, I mean, not to diss our respective fields, but I actually think this is one reason that people love podcasts so much, right? It's a frictionless way to feel like you're part of this interesting conversation, but ultimately it doesn't work as well as picking up the phone and calling a friend, connecting with someone in real life. I think we have too many outlets for things that kind of feel socially, but don't give us social nutrition.
Starting point is 00:44:21 And it's true. I mean, we should be honest, like really connecting with actual people in real life takes more friction than pulling out your phone and scrolling through your Instagram feed. It's just the Instagram feed doesn't work as well ultimately when it comes to what's really going to end up being rewarding. And I think this is true for just like a lot of the way the reward system works. The things that we have craving for, that we seek out, that like we have really strong mechanisms to go after. Sometimes those things don't work to get us towards real likeability. You know, drugs of addiction are a real obvious answer to this, right? You know, if you have a kind of heroin problem, you're going to really seek out that drug, but ultimately it's not bringing you towards something. I mean, it will maybe feel good in the moment, but it's, you know, no, you're not neutral, sweetie, but it's not getting you towards something that evolutionarily would be really awesome for your survival and reproductive success.
Starting point is 00:45:07 Well, I try my best not to speak in tweets, which I guess they now call ex posts, but I've been saying a lot and I'll say it again now that I think everyone should be aware any dopamine that is not preceded by effort in order to achieve it. In other words, any fast high inflection of dopamine that does not require effort to achieve it is gonna put you in a trough
Starting point is 00:45:33 and on a metaphorical lever pressing cycle that will drive your trough deeper and deeper over time. And that peak will just never go as high as it did or could again, unless you take a period of abstinence from that behavior or substance and then introduce effort prior to a adaptive behavior to get the dopamine. The other thing is I've thought,
Starting point is 00:46:00 I like to think of addiction as a progressive narrowing of the things that bring you pleasure. And I don't speak to enlightenment, but happiness or enlightenment seems like a progressive broadening of the things that bring you pleasure. And I'm glad we're talking about reward circuitry because we know how to reset that reward circuitry
Starting point is 00:46:17 and it doesn't require these dopamine fasts, although that's one approach and that makes sense why people do it. But I think this notion of having to spend effort to engage in what we know as a hardwired source of reward, not just dopamine, but other neurochemicals as well, of course, in the form of social connection. So this higher friction thing of having to call somebody
Starting point is 00:46:37 or drive someplace or deal with traffic, deal with traffic on the way home. Well worth it if it was a good social interaction, but maybe it was a meh social interaction, in which case you're like, that was a lot of driving today, I have now all this other stuff to do. Other times a great social interaction can set you
Starting point is 00:46:55 in an amazing emotional plane for days, if not weeks. So I think what you're bringing up is really important. How do we introduce these behaviors, not asking you to put it into a standardized protocol too much, but since we started with this issue of behaviors being a path to more happiness and social connection being the, in real life social connection or by phone in real time,
Starting point is 00:47:19 as you said, being one of the main paths to behavioral happiness, behaviorally derived happiness, excuse me, then what are the data on sort of the frequency of this? Does it vary for introverts versus extroverts? This question is getting very long, but maybe we could define introverts and extroverts. And then if you would, if you could give us some sense of how often should people seek out an in real life interaction? Yeah, probably way more than you think you should.
Starting point is 00:47:46 We have good data on what people predict, which is that people predict social interaction is just not going to be that fun. It's not going to be worth it. This seems to be a spot where our predictions about how good something is going to be don't necessarily match how good it ultimately will going to be. I put it in the context of the reverse of something like processed food, where I think for a lot of people, you predict this is going to be amazing. And you taste it, and you're like, now I feel kind of gross.
Starting point is 00:48:09 Processed food. Processed food, right. That's a case where your prediction is like, oh, this is going to be awesome. But your actual likability is like, I feel kind of yucky. Where social connection, I think we predict, be all right, but maybe not that good. But when we get it, we feel really great.
Starting point is 00:48:24 The University of Chicago psychologist Nick Epley has this term, he uses under-sociality, where he thinks we just kind of don't get the right reward benefit of social connection, writ large, right? He talks about examples of, you know, expressing gratitude to people, giving somebody a compliment, even things like asking for help, right? All these domains where we can kind of connect with another person, we sort of was like, expressing gratitude to people, giving somebody a compliment, even things like asking for help, right? All these domains where we can kind of connect with another person, we sort of was like, yeah, it may be net good if I was rating it on some scale, but it winds up being way better than we predict in all these contexts.
Starting point is 00:48:54 He does these studies where he has people predict how good something will be, you know, giving a gift to somebody, he's in Chicago, right? So he's like, here's a hot chocolate, how good will it feel to like, you know, give that guy over there a stranger the hot chocolate? And people say, you know, I don't know, three out of 10. But then they do it, and then they feel, oh, it's like more like a six out of 10. It was much more rewarding for me, the giver, than I thought.
Starting point is 00:49:12 Same thing with compliments, expressing gratitude, calling a friend you haven't talked to in a long time, reaching out to somebody that you care about but you haven't connected with. All these spots are ones where our predictions are off. It's not the valence that's off. We know it'll be good, but we just don't realize how good. And his argument is that if we don't realize how good,
Starting point is 00:49:30 then we never seek it out. So it's kind of the opposite of what you might think of as the processed food problem, where our prediction is like, oh my god, that cupcake's going to be so good. We have all these mechanisms that are like, go get it, go get it. But then we actually get it.
Starting point is 00:49:41 We're like, that wasn't as good as we thought. I think that the problem is that we have all these things that work like the processed food that interfere with social connection Going on the reddit feed, you know plopping down and watching Netflix just kind of being by yourself, right? There's all these alternative behaviors that we're predicting are gonna feel nice, but then we get there. They feel kind of yucky. They just Yeah, this is a problem in the happiness space where, I know you talk a lot about the reward system, but the happiness space is one where the cravings we have, the rewards we seek out, the predictions we're making about what feels good, we're often
Starting point is 00:50:13 just really wrong with them. You know, my podcast, we talk a lot about like our mind lies to us when it comes to happiness. You know, we go for more money, we go for accolades, you know, we go for the quick dopamine hits without any work, but really it's more like social connection. It's all these things that we kind of don't expect are going to feel good. And so I actually don't know what that means evolutionarily.
Starting point is 00:50:31 My theory is like you didn't need to build in craving mechanisms because the things that really matter for our happiness we just kind of got for free in the evolutionary environment, but it means it's hard to go after them. You mentioned introverts and extroverts, and just to get back to your longer question, this is something that's been studied in them. So introverts versus extroverts is typically thought of as a personality distinction, often thought of as sort of something that's built in, although there's lots of evidence that over time you can sort of change these things around, you could become a little bit more extroverted if you're introverted. But introverts tend to value deeper close conversations, one-on-one kinds of things, and a lot of alone time. They get a lot of benefit from alone time.
Starting point is 00:51:08 Whereas extroverts tend to be more energized by being around other people, especially bigger crowds of people. And so introverts tend to be a little bit more deliberate, a little bit more thoughtful, a little bit more, kind of want to have my own personal chill time. Whereas extroverts tend to like people. And so you might think that everything I've just said
Starting point is 00:51:26 applies to extroverts, but not to introverts. Folks have gone out and tested this. And what they find is there is a big difference between introverts and extroverts, but it's in that prediction error. Extroverts predict, ah, social connection, we are right, not that great. Introverts predict, it's going to be terrible.
Starting point is 00:51:41 It's going to be awkward. I'm going to hate it. But when you actually force people, as in these studies where you say, hey, $10 Starbucks gift card, you gotta talk to somebody, when you force the introverts to be social, what they wind up doing is self-reporting, you know, a level of happiness that's like better than they expected. So the problem seems to be that introverts have a prediction error. I'm gonna say this, you're gonna, I promise you, because I've said this on my podcast, tons of hate mail, lots of the comments will be like,
Starting point is 00:52:05 not me, not this introvert. Or maybe they don't quite understand, so I wanna make sure that it's crystal clear for people. Introverts anticipate a less than great or even eh interaction, maybe even a negative interaction. It's usually negative, usually negative. They anticipate a negative interaction. So it's like saying, we're gonna go to a restaurant
Starting point is 00:52:24 and the food here, it sucks. They go in, they have a decent to maybe a great interaction. So introverts are positioned to derive more pleasure from social interactions. Then extroverts who enter social situations thinking it's going to be great, their anticipation is high. And therefore, they require a much bigger dopamine inflection
Starting point is 00:52:48 in order to come away from that interaction saying it was great. Although the one update to the framework that you just prevented that I'd add is that you said, well, if you go to the restaurant, you predict it's going to be not that good. And you go, and you're like, oh, it was all right. I think the problem with introverts is they so predict that social connection's going to be awkward that they don't engage in it.
Starting point is 00:53:07 And now this becomes a learning cycle, right? You predicted it was going to be crappy. You never got any evidence. Oh, maybe I was wrong. And so you keep doing that over time. And so I think that this can lead to cycles of loneliness in introverts. And there are these lovely accounts of introverts
Starting point is 00:53:21 who try to become a little bit more extroverted. I had this lovely woman, Jessica Pan, on the show who has this book called Sorry I'm Late, I Didn't Want to Come, colon. I love that. Colon. I'm actually pretty social, but I'm late to everything because I'm an academic. Noon means noon 10, which means starting at 12 15, which means at 1 15 when the lecture was supposed to end at 1, you're still going half the room is full.
Starting point is 00:53:44 But this is not just like, she had a reason to say- Any academic knows what I'm talking about. Yeah, I know. I'm with you. 115 when the lecture was supposed to end at one, you're still going half the room is full. But this is not just like, she had a reason to say. Any academic knows what I'm talking about. Yeah, I know, I'm with you, I'm with you. But dude, sorry I'm late, I didn't want to come. Colin, an introvert's guide to extroverting. So she did this year, where as a super hardcore introvert, she talked to people, joined an improv comedy group,
Starting point is 00:53:59 like went to these social networking kind of businessy, nasty social connection events, just did all this stuff. And what she found was two things. One is that she actually did enjoy it a lot more than she thought. went to these social networking kind of businessy, nasty social connection events, just did all this stuff. And what she found was two things. One is that she actually did enjoy it a lot more than she thought. At the end of the year, she was much happier than she expected, but she also watched her habits changing too. And this is a thing I think that we also get wrong about introverts and extroverts, is
Starting point is 00:54:17 we assume, I'm born that way, you know, never going to change. And it is true that there are predispositions towards this stuff. But the data suggests that if you can maybe like update your reward value of this, you as an introvert, try a little social connection. Don't go to like the hugest party ever, jump into improv comedy. Just try call a friend that you haven't talked to in a while, right? Notice how that felt like, oh, I was, I was a prediction error, right? I actually felt better than I expected. Then you might update your prediction and get, and so you can kind of update your introversion in part by trying things out and noticing the
Starting point is 00:54:47 reward value you get. I think the thing that is different for introverts is like you definitely need your alone time, right? So you want to balance any social connection you get with a little bit of time by yourself but the research really shows that if you're predicting right now like I just don't like the social connection, you might actually like it more than your prediction is suggesting. I don't want to social connection. You might actually like it more than your prediction is suggesting. I don't want to micro dissect social interactions to the point of becoming artificial,
Starting point is 00:55:09 but I'm fairly introverted. I love New York City and I love London. I love busy cities. So I don't mind being surrounded by people, but one byproduct of being surrounded by people in a big city is you're not interacting with everybody. You're seeing lots of faces. So is it the case that introverts are really uncomfortable
Starting point is 00:55:29 in big social interactions? Or to me, the most mentally demanding social interaction would be one where I go to a party where I know there's gonna be like 20 people, everyone's gonna have to go around the room and introduce themselves, goodness. Clearly I don't have a problem with public speaking, but that to me just like spikes my cortisol immediately.
Starting point is 00:55:50 And then there's sort of an expectation of like real connection. The expectation of real connection oftentimes undermines real connection, sometimes it serves it. But is it the case that introverts want to avoid people or they want to avoid the requirement to really engage in a deep way. And I like engaging in a deep way one to one or maybe with two or three people, you know,
Starting point is 00:56:12 maybe a few more. But I don't know that it's the number of people that becomes overwhelming or daunting or the punishing feature. It's more, you know, the sort of requirement to like be pulled out of oneself. Yeah, I think it might be pulled out of oneself. Yeah, I think it might be all of the above. I mean, I think what we know about introverts is that they often self-report being better
Starting point is 00:56:31 in these sort of one-on-one kind of things. So as an introvert, you're going to have a coffee date with your friend. That often doesn't cause as much social anxiety as the dinner party with a bunch of people, right? And so that's the claim. It's not like, well, jump into the dinner party with a bunch of people, or join an impromptu group,
Starting point is 00:56:44 or talk to everyone on the street. It's like,, well, jump into the dinner party with a bunch of people or join an improv comedy group or talk to everyone on the street. It's like just a one-on-one little mini conversation can be great. Um, and, but, or not as a great, but much better than you expect. And we'll kind of have this happiness benefit that kind of sustains you over time. Um, Nick Epley, who does all this work talks about your happiness. The best metaphor for happiness is that it's kind of like a leaky tire, like it sort of goes out a little bit. And each one of these little conversations, whether it's chatting with the barista, calling a friend,
Starting point is 00:57:11 giving someone a compliment, whatever, kind of fills up the tire and then it kind of goes down. So you can sort of use these little mini micro doses of social connection to boost your happiness tire. I'd like to take a quick break and thank one of our sponsors, Function. I recently became a Function member after searching for the most comprehensive approach
Starting point is 00:57:30 to lab testing. While I've long been a fan of blood testing, I really wanted to find a more in-depth program for analyzing blood, urine, and saliva to get a full picture of my heart health, my hormone status, my immune system regulation, my metabolic function, my vitamin and mineral status and other critical areas of my overall health and vitality.
Starting point is 00:57:49 Function not only provides testing of over a hundred biomarkers key to physical and mental health, but it also analyzes these results and provides insights from top doctors on your results. For example, in one of my first tests with function, I learned that I had two high levels of mercury in my blood. This was totally surprising to me. I had no idea prior to taking the test. Function not only helped me detect this,
Starting point is 00:58:11 but offered medical doctor informed insights on how to best reduce those mercury levels, which included limiting my tuna consumption, because I had been eating a lot of tuna, while also making an effort to eat more leafy greens and supplementing with NAC, N-Acetylcysteine, both of which can support glutathione production and detoxification and worked to reduce my mercury levels.
Starting point is 00:58:31 Comprehensive lab testing like this is so important for health. And while I've been doing it for years, I've always found it to be overly complicated and expensive. I've been so impressed by function, both at the level of ease of use, that is getting the tests done, as well as how comprehensive and how actionable
Starting point is 00:58:47 the tests are, that I recently joined their advisory board and I'm thrilled that they're sponsoring the podcast. If you'd like to try Function, go to functionhealth.com slash Huberman. Function currently has a wait list of over 250,000 people, but they're offering early access to Huberman Lab listeners. Again, that's functionhealth.com slash Huberman to get early access to function. Today's episode is also brought to us by Element. Element is an electrolyte drink that has everything
Starting point is 00:59:14 you need, but nothing you don't. That means the electrolytes, sodium, magnesium, and potassium, all in the correct ratios, but no sugar. Proper hydration is critical for optimal brain and body function. Even a slight degree of dehyd critical for optimal brain and body function. Even a slight degree of dehydration can diminish cognitive and physical performance. It's also important that you get adequate electrolytes. The electrolytes, sodium, magnesium, and potassium are vital for the functioning of all the cells
Starting point is 00:59:36 in your body, especially your neurons or your nerve cells. Drinking element dissolved in water makes it extremely easy to ensure that you're getting adequate hydration and adequate electrolytes. To make sure that I'm getting proper amounts of hydration and electrolytes, I dissolve one packet of element in about 16 to 32 ounces of water when I wake up in the morning, and I drink that basically first thing in the morning. I also drink element dissolved in water during any kind of physical exercise that I'm doing, especially on hot days when I'm sweating a lot and therefore losing a lot of water and electrolytes. They have a bunch of different great tasting flavors of element.
Starting point is 01:00:07 They have watermelon, citrus, et cetera. Frankly, I love them all. And now that we're in the winter months in the Northern hemisphere, element has their chocolate medley flavors back in stock. I really like the chocolate flavors, especially the chocolate mint when it's heated up. So you put it in hot water and that's a great way to replenish electrolytes and hydrate,
Starting point is 01:00:24 especially when it's cold and dry outside when hydration is especially critical. If you'd like to try Element, you can go to drinkelement.com slash Huberman to claim a free Element sample pack with the purchase of any Element drink mix. Again, that's drinkelement.com slash Huberman to claim a free sample pack. So you're talking about engaging in social connection in real time and perhaps even in real life. Yes, in real life as well. With some effort to engage in it, which might just be built into modern living now,
Starting point is 01:00:57 as one of the primary drivers for behavioral approaches to improve what we're calling happiness. So could we say, and I know we don't want to set up strict protocols around this, make the effort to schedule in real time over the phone or Zoom or in real life interaction with somebody maybe once a week? Yeah. Minimum?
Starting point is 01:01:21 I think more than you're doing now, if you're not feeling so happy, add some in. And again, as you mentioned before, these are all kind of trade-offs, right? You don't want to add so much in that now you're not sleeping or exercising or all that other stuff. But like one more interaction than you're getting now
Starting point is 01:01:35 and check how it feels over time. Given how busy people are and given that we've established that some effort that's required to engage socially is going to be beneficial toward the reward and all of this. And we're not trying to hack the dopamine system here, folks. We're just trying to figure out what is going to be rewarding given that everyone has constraints on their time and everyone seems to have a device in their pocket
Starting point is 01:01:57 that allows them to get the illusion of nourishment that leads to either same levels or less happiness overall, what's going to be most effective? And it seems to me, I was thinking about this during one of your answers, I was paying attention, but I was thinking about this, that my memory of prior social interactions as really great is a useful tool.
Starting point is 01:02:22 So for instance, one of my best memories of time with my girlfriend was driving back from her grandmother's house with the dog in the car and we had no phone reception, so we couldn't be interrupted by our devices. She actually had some work to do, so she was doing some work on her computer at one point. She may have taken a nap at one point
Starting point is 01:02:42 and the dog kept jumping back and forth between our laps And that to me was like one of the best days ever. Mm-hmm. Just ever it was just an awesome day That memory occurred to me now and I think could serve me well and thinking okay, so like going on a road trip with somebody but it was that it was the lack of Kind of structure around it. It was just imposed on us. We had a drive to complete. There was a dog in the car. There was some work to do.
Starting point is 01:03:08 There was no phone reception. And we've had many great interactions, but that would be the one that I'd highlight as like, that was an awesome interaction for whatever reason. Yeah. And so can one use memories of great social interaction as kind of a, as a compass for how to construct these social plans?
Starting point is 01:03:29 Yeah. Because I think it can be a little bit mystifying to people like, oh, how do I get this thing called happiness by meeting up with a friend? We enjoy hiking or something like that, but maybe that's not accessible. And I don't want people to underthink or overthink it.
Starting point is 01:03:43 But to me, it seems like, seems like okay like road trips everyday things We needed to go up there. It was some things to tend to so like tending to life things Yeah, life requirements together. Yeah, I mean if you want to ask yourself a question that can highlight good memories I recommend the one that the journalist Katherine Price uses a lot She does a lot of studies on fun ask yourself the question What was three times that you just had the most fun? The last three times you would describe as, oh my God, that was the most fun, right?
Starting point is 01:04:09 And this is a helpful question because usually the answer, my guess is at least two out of three, probably all three, will have someone else in it. Like they'll be, you'll have like another person involved or a dog, sometimes some other agent of being, right? It wasn't you by yourself, it probably didn't involve a screen, right? And so that's kind of like...
Starting point is 01:04:27 Definitely not. And that actually gets back to your road trip, you know, talking about, I think the social part was really important, but it seems like that road trip also tapped into a sort of thought pattern that we know is really good for happiness, which is presence, right? Just being kind of mindful. You're paying attention to the dog flopping on you. You're seeing the scenery, right? Just being kind of mindful. You're paying attention to the dog flopping on you. You're seeing the scenery, right? You're not in a rush to do something, so your mind could kind of be on that drive and how it felt. And we know so much about how kind of these moments of mindfulness really paying attention to your sensory experience,
Starting point is 01:04:58 how much that matters for happiness. And one of the biggest hacks you can use to get more presence is to do exactly what you accidentally did driving through these parts of the world where you don't get phone receptions, which is to get rid of our phones. You know, our phones are just like the biggest attention stealers ever. And it makes a lot of sense because what grabs our attention? Things that are really interesting and provide a little gloss of quick dopamine hits, right? Or just kind of scream at us with information and announcements and so on.
Starting point is 01:05:27 This is what our phones do really well. And our brains aren't stupid. Our brains know that on the other side of our phone is like such rewarding content. And it becomes really distracting. My colleague Liz Dunn has this kind of analogy she uses that is like, imagine, instead of this kind of conversation
Starting point is 01:05:45 we're having right here, maybe I'll do with my husband at a dinner party where I'm sitting my husband at dinner and we're chatting and I have my phone out. And my husband's a philosopher, he's a very smart guy. We have great conversations. But I know on the other end of that phone is like really interesting stuff.
Starting point is 01:05:59 And Liz said, imagine the comparison is instead of having your phone there, I had this big wheelbarrow next to me and my husband at our dinner table. And in that wheelbarrow was photo albums of every photo I've taken since 2016, physical printouts of my emails and news articles and stuff that I could get, you know, like videotapes of cat videos and porn and everything just like piled up really high in this wheelbarrow. If we were trying to have a conversation that wheelbarrow was there, I'd be like, oh, I just want to take a quick look at the photo or do something.
Starting point is 01:06:26 It'd be so distracting. It'd be so interesting. Your brain's not stupid. Your brain knows, even though your phone is much tinier than that wheelbarrow, that all that interesting, dopamine-rich, exciting stuff is on it. And it makes it hard to pay attention to my husband. Again, an interesting philosopher.
Starting point is 01:06:41 We have great conversations. And so there's lots of evidence showing that even the act of having your phone out is subtly stealing your attention from other people, from the tasks that you're doing. One of the biggest pieces of advice I give my college students is to study without your phone near you, because Princeton studies have looked at this, where you have somebody do say a math test or a studying test with your phone there versus a phone in the other room, and you see like double digit increases in performance just to have your phone away. And you might ask like, well, why would that be? It's like, well,
Starting point is 01:07:11 part of your frontal lobe is like, no, no, no, don't look at the phone. Don't look at the phone. Don't look at that big wheelbarrow of delicious, interesting stuff. Stay on task. And that's this kind of constant moment of multitasking where we're kind of yanking our brain back onto task. And so a big hack, if you want to be be more present is to find ways to do activities without your phone. I guess if we go back to that fun question, if I said those three times you're having the most fun, you weren't in the middle of it pulling out your phone to look at your Instagram feed.
Starting point is 01:07:37 You were just there. JS And what you just described was a dramatic performance enhancement on mathematics by not having this opportunity for distraction in the room, which is incredible. Yeah. I mean, when you dive deep into the effects of having your phone around you, they're striking, especially in getting back to social connection,
Starting point is 01:07:59 especially social connection. Liz Dunn has this paper where she puts two people in a room, just kind of a waiting room together, and you either have your phone or you don't. You're not allowed to look at it, it's just present. And she finds that there's 30% less smiling at the other people in the waiting room when your phones are present. 30% less. I mean, I actually think of this when I think of the loneliness crisis.
Starting point is 01:08:20 I walk, I was a head of college on campus, which as a faculty member at Yale meant that I lived on campus with students. And you'd walk through the courtyard, and everybody's walking through the courtyard, but they're not looking at you, they're looking down at their phone, right? There's these like subtle interactions that we're missing because our phone is stealing us. That's the social case,
Starting point is 01:08:38 but I think there's a real performance case too, right? If you wanna pay attention and learn something, if part of your brain is inhibiting that urge to look at all the interesting stuff on your phone, which we don't notice, then that's going to be affecting your performance. It has good benefits too. There's this lovely finding that people are buying less gum and less candy in checkout aisles now. Like the national worldwide sale of gum has gone down and it's gone down on the same slope as the iPhones have gone up.
Starting point is 01:09:07 So as number of iPhones in pockets goes up, sales of gum and checkout lines has gone down and you can see why that is. They're not looking around as much. You're not looking like, ooh, that, you know, like double mint looks really good. You're staring at your phone and looking at your Instagram. Soon the ads are going to pop up on,
Starting point is 01:09:22 they'll know you're in the aisle because they can know you have proximity to a lot of devices. I have a friend who's a very accomplished songwriter and musician and someone does his Instagram and other social media for him. He's not on there. And we met for dinner the other day
Starting point is 01:09:38 with a couple other people and I got there and I started telling him about something I had seen online and he said, I won't use it. I usually will do his voice, but I won't do his voice because I don't want to give it away. People might know who he is. But he said, I don't want to talk about what's on Instagram. In fact, I don't want to talk about what's on the internet.
Starting point is 01:10:02 Let's just have dinner. And at first I was like, dude, I was like, and I thought, great, he's exactly right. It's not just having the phone there, it's not just being on the device. It's also that you're talking about things that you saw in the world, some of which are very interesting and important at times.
Starting point is 01:10:25 But what he was saying was, I don't want to talk about things that you experienced about somebody else's experience. That wasn't really an experience that you had today. That's an experience of someone else's experience that you had today. It wasn't about a news article or something. So we're playing the telephone bucket brigade game
Starting point is 01:10:44 of social connection many, many degrees away from the actual interactions that we were kind of hardwired to experience ourselves. And those are the ones that really influence our happiness in the world, right? You know, one of the great ways to increase your presence in addition to kind of getting rid of your phone is to just go back to your senses, right?
Starting point is 01:11:04 What are you looking at right now? What do you see right now? I'm in this room, there's like really nice kind of cool black lighting, and I'm sitting there, I'm hearing your voice, there's like a subtle hum in the room that I hope the podcast is not picking up, that I hear in the audio, right, it's a little cool. That grounding, I can watch my breath
Starting point is 01:11:20 completely change around. It's like a quick way to just kind of be embodied. And I think so often we're just not doing that as much, in our discourse, but definitely like even when we're by ourselves, we just wind up distracting ourselves from the very sensory experience that like literally is the experience that we have of the world,
Starting point is 01:11:40 just not noticing it as much. Now, I will say that a memory of a really terrific time I had alone was around this time of year, actually around the holidays. Typically I would be in my office organizing papers, maybe dealing with some end of year stuff for academics. My life's a lot different now with the podcast, even though I still teach at Stanford,
Starting point is 01:12:02 but the end of year is the time when you kind of get your office organized, every academic knows this. And over the holidays, I tended to have a lot of time in my office alone. It was a great time to come in, like parking was everywhere. You go in and I used to listen to Ted Talks
Starting point is 01:12:17 or I would listen to podcasts. And these days I'm trying to do more physical things, not just exercise, but working on some like lighting stuff in my house. And I like to listen to podcasts or books, sometimes music, but podcasts or books while I do that. And I do feel that when we're alone,
Starting point is 01:12:35 sometimes it's nice to have other voices in the room that are not just the voices in our head. And it could be music, podcasts, books, movies, et cetera, that people seem to find that soothing. I certainly do. And that doesn't feel like it's diminishing from my experience of being present. In fact, it allows me to just really tend
Starting point is 01:12:53 to what I'm doing mechanically. And I have some plans to do some more like craft drawing type projects in the new year. And I look forward to being able to hear those conversations but not have to participate in them. Yeah, yeah, yeah. So would you consider that sort of healthy or am I diminishing my experience
Starting point is 01:13:10 and the depth of my crafts? Yeah, well, I think there's some nuance there, right? You're talking about your craft in a very embodied way, even as you're talking to me, you're describing how your hands are moving in these motor ways as you're doing it. You're talking about kind of what it felt like. It felt like your senses were activated for the physical stuff you were doing.
Starting point is 01:13:31 But you also mentioned that your mind was wandering and maybe you were ruminating and stuff like that. So it sounds like what you did was have a really nice emotion regulation strategy of like you could kind of fill your head with something so that you can work on the physical stuff. But it didn't impede your experience of the physical stuff. The way you described it shows that you were there,
Starting point is 01:13:48 you were present when you were doing it. I think the problem is when it impedes our presence doing it. And I think it kind of depends on the activity we're doing, right? You know, take driving. Probably some of you who are listening right now are sitting in your car as you're driving,
Starting point is 01:14:02 doing this other interesting motoric activity. And that's one where it's like, you're not missing out on that much on the drive by listening to us. It's probably a positive experience that you're having, learning something and so on. But you wouldn't want to listen to podcasts in some physical situations. If you're ballroom dancing, for example,
Starting point is 01:14:22 you wouldn't want to necessarily be listening to a podcast then. If you're really experiencing art and engaging with art in an art gallery, you wouldn't want to also be listening to a podcast at the same time. And so I think the thing to think is, are you listening to this in a way that you're missing something in the real world, that your presence of it would matter, make you feel really good? Or are you kind of just like, you know, killing some other free time and maybe using this
Starting point is 01:14:48 as a nice emotion regulation strategy to stop what would otherwise be a really ruminative drive, now you get to listen to me and Andrew and that's probably better. But there's nuance there. I think our tendency is to move away from the rumination, is to run away from it. And I think if you find yourself kind of avoiding
Starting point is 01:15:05 your thought patterns altogether, that probably might be the pendulum swinging a little too far in the other direction. Noted. Noted. So we've got in real time and or in real life, well in real life is always in real time, but in real life and or in real time social interaction
Starting point is 01:15:21 and if it requires some effort to plan or get to, organize all the better. You stand to gain more from those interactions. So that's really key. Presence, obviously, try and get the phone out of the room, at least off and put away, but ideally out of the- Out of sight. Out of sight, out of sight.
Starting point is 01:15:36 Out of sight, love that. And shared experience, presumably, actually maybe doing something, but it could even just be talking. I guess it depends on what people enjoy doing, right? So these are powerful levers for shifting one level of happiness up using behavior. What about leading with thought patterns or feelings?
Starting point is 01:15:59 Seems like it's a more challenging, but certainly tractable entry point. Yeah, and I think it's important here to remember what are our natural evolutionary patterns towards thought patterns, because some of them aren't necessarily built for our happiness. Take what's a kind of common evolutionary thought pattern,
Starting point is 01:16:18 which is a negativity bias, right? We're just built to notice all the scary stuff, all the bad stuff, all the potentially risky stuff. Our brains instantly go there. And that makes fabulous evolutionary sense. Like if there's a possibility that there's a tiger that's gonna jump out or some sort of risky thing, you want your brain to lock onto it. Not as evolutionary beneficial
Starting point is 01:16:36 to notice all the blessings in life, just all the good things. It doesn't really give you that much of a survival benefit to notice, hey, there's the absence of a tiger. We don't really know. There's no tigers around, right? In fact, it probably helps drive more motivation to go pursue resources.
Starting point is 01:16:53 I mean, you could imagine an adaptive feature to lacking satisfaction. I mean, you could gain more resources and at a time where resources were presumably shared more in these small village formats. I don't know, do monkey troops share resources? This gets a little common. It depends on the monkey.
Starting point is 01:17:08 It depends on the monkey. Just like humans. Depends on the human. Depends on the monkey. But I think you're making a really critical point, right? Which is like, if we're noticing the negative, if we're noticing the bad stuff, we tend to fix it. But also if we're craving, if we're wanting,
Starting point is 01:17:23 if we're kind of constantly in search of something, we get off our butts and go do stuff, right? I mean, Steve Jobs and his parting words were stay hungry, stay foolish. Maybe it was stay foolish, stay hungry, but stay hungry was definitely in there. And I realize he's not going to represent the epitome of what to strive for for everybody,
Starting point is 01:17:38 but he certainly held up as somebody who changed the world through the development of certain technologies. So we revere these people that are hungry for more. Yeah. Right. And it makes great evolutionary sense. Doesn't make as good happiness sense, right? What's one of the best ways to be happy?
Starting point is 01:17:57 To just appreciate what you have, to notice and appreciate the blessings out there. But we've got to push against this natural negativity bias to do this. So how do we do that? Well, it turns out that this is a spot where harnessing attention and the way we were just talking about can be really helpful, just taking time to notice the blessings, notice kind of all the good stuff. It's often talked about in terms of a gratitude practice, although gratitude sounds kind of cheesy. I don't know. My friend, Catherine Price, who I mentioned earlier, she has this practice that she calls a delight practice. We just noticed delights in the world.
Starting point is 01:18:27 I love the word delight. You know, I walked in your studio, you had a picture of your bulldog, and I was like, that's a delight, that's so cute. Thank you for delighting in him. I delight in him too, even though he's dead several years now. Delight is a wonderful word.
Starting point is 01:18:40 Yeah, and we can train our brain to notice them, right? You can literally have a practice where, you know, put in your notes app on your phone, like list of delights, or even better, pick a friend like I have with Katherine where you can just like text them, delight, you know, at the end of this, you know, text like, saw a really cute dog, delight, or heard this really funny song, delight. Then you get the social connection and the gratitude. But what that does is, if you have this practice where you got to write down the delights,
Starting point is 01:19:02 your brain starts to automatically be on the lookout for them. It becomes rewarding because you get to write this thing down. Now all of a sudden it can be a practice that you're sort of shifting your negativity bias to notice more of the good things that are out there. And there's so much evidence suggesting that people who naturally notice the blessings in the world are happier. If you do one of these kind of gratitude or delight practices, you wind up happier. Sonia Lubomirski has this lovely study
Starting point is 01:19:25 where you scribble down three to five things you're grateful for, three to five delights, in as little as two weeks, you significantly improve your overall satisfaction with life, right? So super free. I love that. So much so that I end because I accidentally interrupted. The comments always tell me I interrupt too much.
Starting point is 01:19:39 It's out of interest, it's out of interest, I promise. If I could interrupt myself, I would. And I probably do from time to time. Could you repeat what the, it's three to five things I promise. If I could interrupt myself, I would. And I probably do from time to time. Could you repeat what the, it's three to five things. Yeah, three to five things you're grateful for. I'm not sure if the number really matters, but it's committing to kind of noticing the good things in life and really trying to take a moment
Starting point is 01:19:56 to notice how they felt, right? So if I look at, I do delight practices sometimes or gratitude practices and it's things like, my husband, you know, these big things in life. But then sometimes it's like my morning coffee or like probably, you know, seeing your cute dog. Like it's funny to see the picture or folks that don't know Andrew's studio,
Starting point is 01:20:10 it's a picture of his dog on a microphone. It's just very funny. It's a giant microphone. A giant high quality photograph. He was actually standing on the table that I do my solo podcast from at the microphone. And his tag just happened to rotate a few degrees toward the camera just at that moment. So you could see his name Costello, you know.
Starting point is 01:20:30 And I invite listeners to pause right now and notice what's happening to their face. As you hear Andrew say that, probably you're just smiling, right? You didn't even see this really cute photo, but you're also smiling. That's the power of delights, right? Not just noticing them yourself,
Starting point is 01:20:43 but potentially sharing them too. And so this is another thought pattern practice that we can engage in, which is like, just train your brain to find these things. And what you'll find is that, you know, there's a limited ratio of the stuff we can focus our attention on. If we start shifting towards the delights
Starting point is 01:20:59 from the hassles and the yucky stuff in life, now we're just kind of filling our brain with stuff that gives us a little more positive emotion. What I love about this conversation about gratitude is that I must say I do like the word delight more than gratitude. Gratitude sounds cheesy. It sounds a little hippie dippy, I gotta say. Well, I'm from Northern California, so I'm cool with hippie dippy, even though I'm not a hippie. Punk rocker, not a hippie. You're Berkeley roots. Yeah, I'm from the other end of the big peninsula. I love the East Bay, but anyway, this is getting, but the point is it's not that the word feels soft.
Starting point is 01:21:30 I need to think about this a little bit more. It's that maybe it's just that delight is such a powerful unselfish word. Like it's not taking anything from anybody. It's not requiring a shift away from one sort of intrinsic self. I feel like gratitude requires this like, okay, I'm gonna now be grateful.
Starting point is 01:21:52 It's like kind of like pulling, if you're not already in a state of gratitude, I feel like there's more effort involved. And we've been saying effort that precedes reward is good. But with delight, it feels like it's just very much in concert with almost like who one is. Yeah. You know, and like I delight in Costello.
Starting point is 01:22:11 I don't expect everyone to delight in Costello. People who did, I delighted in their delight. So it was just, you know, amplifying all the delight. But the thing that really strikes me about delight is that every example you gave, it's very rapid time scale. Like you, you know, like I will say, I normally drink yerba mate during these things which I delight in,
Starting point is 01:22:33 but today I decided I haven't had coffee in a while. Took a little break from it for no particular reason. And I had a single shot of espresso and I was thinking to myself, this is really good. So this is a fast time to go. Maybe it was the fact that I haven't had it in a little while. And it's just really fast.
Starting point is 01:22:49 No one suffers. It's all game. And so it runs a little bit countercurrent to what we were talking about before, which is the requirement for effort to precede the reward. Delight feels like a very smooth road to a reward that's all net positive. And as you said, these delights are available
Starting point is 01:23:11 throughout the day and it doesn't, it requires just noticing something inside and outside. Whereas I feel like with gratitude, I love gratitude practices, the data are incredible. It is anything but squishy. It is like a real power tool for shifting one's state of mind. That's clear from the literature. But the gratitude thing, I feel like,
Starting point is 01:23:30 requires an almost like a formalization, like, okay, I'm gonna be grateful now. Whereas delight, you're just kind of on the lookout for things that spark you and make you reflexively smile. And a few things are better than that. Yeah, and I think it's really sensory, in the way we were talking about before, right? It gets you back into being present.
Starting point is 01:23:47 Most of these delights are something you taste, or you experience, or you see that's funny. There's a really lovely book by the author, Ross Gay, called The Book of Delights. And he used a delight practice where every day he not only had to find a delight, but write a short essay about it, because he's an author. And it's just hilarious.
Starting point is 01:24:04 It's like one of my favorite books. And you just kind of go... And it's really strange things. It's like, when does he notice the flowers? He notices lilacs, and he has this whole idea of... One delay is purple flowers. Why are there so many purple flowers? There's purple flowers everywhere. He also has a delight in music. He really likes the 80s band El DeBarge, you know, from the beat of the room. I'm vaguely familiar with it. So it's like, and he talks about his love of DeBarge. And you can kind of have this connection with other people's delights. And it's silly. They're just silly things. But the fact that we've noticed them,
Starting point is 01:24:32 I mean, again, as the listeners probably experiencing right now, if you pay attention, a little bit of positive emotion, right? If you're driving around your car, feeling a little stressed out in traffic, you can kind of take a breath. And so that's the power of the practice. You're shifting your emotions because you're noticing these good things. You're noticing the good things, which is great. You're sort of training your attention to get there. And you're sort of forming this habit
Starting point is 01:24:54 to shift that negativity bias that's sort of built in, but isn't really making you as happy as you could be. I'd like to take a quick break and thank one of our sponsors, David. David makes a protein bar unlike any other. I'd like to take a quick break and thank one of our sponsors, David. David makes a protein bar unlike any other. It has 28 grams of protein, only 150 calories and zero grams of sugar. That's right, 28 grams of protein and 75% of its calories come from protein.
Starting point is 01:25:17 These bars from David also taste amazing. My favorite flavor is chocolate chip cookie dough. But then again, I also like the chocolate fudge flavored one. And I also like the cake flavored one. Basically, I like all the flavors. They're incredibly delicious. For me personally, I strive to eat mostly whole foods. However, when I'm in a rush or I'm away from home
Starting point is 01:25:35 or I'm just looking for a quick afternoon snack, I often find that I'm looking for a high quality protein source. With David, I'm able to get 28 grams of protein with the calories of a snack, which makes it very easy to hit my protein goals of one gram of protein per pound of body weight each day. And it allows me to do that
Starting point is 01:25:51 without taking in excess calories. I typically eat a David Bar in the early afternoon or even mid afternoon if I want to bridge that gap between lunch and dinner. I like that it's a little bit sweet so it tastes like a tasty snack, but it's also given me that 28 grams of very high quality protein with just 150 calories.
Starting point is 01:26:08 If you would like to try David, you can go to davidprotein.com slash Huberman. Again, the link is davidprotein.com slash Huberman. I've long been interested in shifting one's emotions and when that feels good, when it is good and when it doesn't feel good. I asked our friend, Ethan Cross, about this too. I'm not gonna compare your answers as a template
Starting point is 01:26:36 for who's right, who's wrong. I think there are a lot of differing opinions on this. But I know from the time we are young kids, but we don't like to be shifted. Like we don't like people to impose an emotional requirement on us. In fact, my niece, when she was little, I was telling her this, she's 18 now, she was not amused, which delighted me that she was not amused,
Starting point is 01:26:58 but when she was little, she was a pretty healthily stubborn kid. And you'd ask her to do anything. Like, hey, let's go downstairs for a walk. And she loved going outside for walks. And she'd say, no, push me. And then she would get her stuff and then you'd go for a walk. But I loved her like, no, push me.
Starting point is 01:27:16 I love this. It was also Costello was like, don't push me. You couldn't. So there was this immediate vocalization from the time she could speak really. I was like, no, I'm gonna decide how I feel. Such a healthy thing too, such a healthy thing. You're not gonna shift me.
Starting point is 01:27:32 I was like, we're going out for a walk, it's gonna be fun. And she's like, no, push me. And then she'd go for the walk. Most of the times it was a fun walk. But I think that we don't like to be shifted. And in some ways we don't really like to shift ourselves. Like when we're in a given emotion, when people are feeling upset,
Starting point is 01:27:51 they don't want to be told they should feel happy. Yeah. And yet no one really wants to be upset. Although there's this, do you know this result? I don't want to spin off into it, a long discussion about this, but Robert Heath, a very controversial neurosurgeon from the like 70s and 80s did these experiments of stimulating
Starting point is 01:28:09 in different parts of the brain, allowing people to self stimulate different parts of their brain. And there were only three subjects because it's a in vivo human neuro stimulation experiment. All three subjects by far their favorite area to stimulate was this midline central nucleus, midline thalamic nucleus rather.
Starting point is 01:28:28 All three of them reported that the sensation that they would lever press the most for was frustration and mild anger. Humans like that shit, excuse my language. Why? Look, the horror movie industry would not exist if we didn't like fear, right? The honestly like Twitter X, whatever we're calling it now, would not exist if we didn't
Starting point is 01:28:49 like outrage, right? These are kind of complicated negative emotions that have some positive benefit to us. And I think that this is something that people get wrong when they hear my line of research. You know, I tell people like, oh, I teach this class about happiness at Yale, and people will say like, oh, you just want everybody to be happy. You sort of embrace this toxic positivity. And I'm like, no, no, no, no, no. Toxic positivity. Yeah. It's this idea. I mean, you kind of see it in our culture right now. It's the sort of good vibes only, right? It's this idea that anything that feels, you know, mildly frustrating or like hard to do,
Starting point is 01:29:23 it's like, oh, no, no, just don't do that. It's like good vibes only, right? And there's this idea that if you're experiencing negative emotions, if you feel sad or you feel a little lonely or you feel a little upset at politics, whatever it is, that something's wrong or you got to take a pill or you got to do something to fix it, right? I think that's a really dangerous idea, right? Because it's getting rid of this signal that we've been built to experience evolutionarily that's really important, right? If you're experiencing outrage that's telling you something super crucial, if you're experiencing kind of frustration, overwhelm is a big one. If you're kind of feeling, oh, I'm so overwhelmed at work and I'm burned out, that's a really useful signal about behavioral changes you should make. In class, I often tell my students that negative emotions are like that dashboard on your car.
Starting point is 01:30:09 You go in your car and sometimes you're driving and the tire light comes on or the engine light comes on. And that's a pain in the ass, honestly, because you're like, well, I've got to deal with it. So it's not fun when these lights come on, but it's super useful information that if you actively ignore it for months and months, it's going to cause a much bigger problem later on. And I think this is how all of our negative emotions work. If you're feeling that negative emotion of loneliness, it means you need more social connection. If you're feeling overwhelmed, it means you're probably going to take something off your
Starting point is 01:30:36 plate before you burn out or get sick. If you're feeling sad, that's probably because of some stimulus that matters that is like you're not there anymore, if you're feeling grief and so on. I think too often we just like want to get rid of those. We don't like them, so we want to suppress those emotions. But suppressing our emotions is giving up useful evolutionary information that probably means we can take action
Starting point is 01:30:58 to fix and feel better. Americans might be surprised to hear this, but I learned this from my father who's from South America. He's from Argentina, went to British schools when he was young. And he told me when I was probably 10 or 12, I can't remember exactly how old,
Starting point is 01:31:13 he said, you know, in the British formal school system, if you act too happy, people accuse you of being stupid. You know, to be gleeful or happy. And I said, now I would say, well, they're perfectly fine being happy when they're drinking. I will say that the after work alcohol culture in London. The 5, 14 PM crowd is it? I don't know if it's still the case, but they drink a lot.
Starting point is 01:31:39 And then they get very outwardly happy. But there's this idea, and this was true when I came into academia, that if somebody wasn't super serious that they might be stupid. And I think in the United States now we tend to celebrate more expressions of glee, but that's usually in the context of like celebrity and wealth, like these people getting on their private planes
Starting point is 01:32:05 or something, but I think there's still some elements to this that we internalize, that if you're happy that you're not worrying about something, if you're not worrying about something, then you're ignoring the woes of the world, maybe even the threats that are all around you. And so in some ways we are conditioned to always wanna be happy, that does seem to be one message,
Starting point is 01:32:24 but then we also get the conflicting message that to be happy is to be ignorant of what's really happening, if not to you then to other people, and therefore you're not fulfilling your role in society. So who are you to be happy all the time? There's a lot of judgment written into this thing around happiness, I'm realizing.
Starting point is 01:32:39 Yeah, totally. And I think you're bringing up something that I actually worry about a lot, right? Which is, is that hypothesis correct? Is it the case that if you're bringing up something that I actually worry about a lot, right, which is, is that hypothesis correct? Is it the case that if you're feeling happy, you just ignore the woes and all the terrible stuff in the world? Because then I'm creating like, you know, a whole generation of Yale students who are going to not fix the bad problems of life.
Starting point is 01:32:55 And so it turns out there's a researcher at Georgetown, Constantine Kuchleff, who's tested this. He actually asked the question, you know, is it the case that people who are experiencing more positive emotion, more satisfaction with life, do they ignore the problems of the world and not act? Or are they the ones kind of going out and doing stuff? And so he did this in a couple different contexts. He looked for social justice causes. I'll tell the climate version.
Starting point is 01:33:15 So he looked at how many people are taking climate action. So do you go to a protest? Do you put solar panels on? Are you donating money to climate causes? And he finds that the people who are really climate anxious, they tend to have less positive emotions. You're really worried about climate change. You tend to be more on the depressed, anxious side. But if you're doing stuff about it, then you tend to have more positive emotion. I think he assumes that Causal Arrow goes in the other way, that if you're happier, if you're experiencing
Starting point is 01:33:40 lots of delights and positive emotion, you kind of have the bandwidth to do stuff, right? You can go to that protest where if you're super depressed, you're just going to like lie in bed with your duvet. You don't have the bandwidth to do this stuff. And so this whole kind of like Pollyanna-ish hypothesis about happiness makes complete intuitive sense. But if you look at the data, it's actually the opposite, which is a good thing because I think it gives us a mandate not to stay depressed about everything in the world,
Starting point is 01:34:05 pissed off about what's happening. Yes, those negative emotions are good to notice and experience and act on, but like we can take care of ourselves and it's okay. It doesn't mean we're going to stop doing good stuff in the world. I have a family member. She's wonderful.
Starting point is 01:34:19 She saves animals constantly. And she knows that she has an excessive number of animals, but she's from the East Coast from New Jersey. And the other day she told me she goes, you know, I like your podcast, but you know, sometimes you have these guests on that are clearly from the West Coast and you guys get into this real like West Coast, California squishy stuff. And I just can't listen to those. And I said, I won't say her name, but for sake of privacy, but she said, but you know, I really like it when, even if the topic is about something kind of squishy, if the person's from the East coast,
Starting point is 01:34:50 then like, you know, like I believe what they're saying. And I said, and she goes, yeah, you know, out there, you're into this and that. And I said, well, out there in New Jersey, you know, language is kind of a weapon. She was, it's absolutely a weapon, you know? So I do think there are these even local cultural things like people from the Midwest, to me,
Starting point is 01:35:05 I don't want to stereotype, but every time I go to the Midwest, I must say that there's an etiquette. People are just so polite and kind. So the level of sort of mean level of decency is much higher than it is, say, in California. In California, there's some other things that are wonderful that are lacking elsewhere,
Starting point is 01:35:23 and on the East Coast and so forth. But yeah, I think one can overgeneralize, but I think that the reason I raise this is that maybe we all need to pay a little bit of attention to the messages that we internalized in our family and our culture growing up and ask ourselves whether or not our degree of happiness or lack thereof is by some programming, literally social programming
Starting point is 01:35:48 that we've internalized. Because I grew up in a home where cynical humor was rewarded. And I've learned over the years in part through discussions with Jimmy Ozaki and others, like I'm working on it. Not all my humor is cynical, but I don't like cynicism. It bums me out. It doesn't feel good.
Starting point is 01:36:07 And I realize it doesn't feel good. I love delight, but I don't like cynicism. That's just me. And for the cynics out there, like, cool, you do you. But I think we have to pay attention to kind of like where our set point is with this stuff. Because some people are like sitting real in the no push me and they want to be unhappy
Starting point is 01:36:25 Yeah, right. We're heading up on the holidays here. So like Scrooge Right. Yeah, and other people they're not feeling good. They want to be happy and then other people really are just like no worries I mean it down in Australia. It's all no worries. And what do they say in Costa Rica? I was oh poor Evita Yeah, which means like like the kind of good life, you know, chill life. Everyone's just telling each other how great life should be all day long. Yeah. Yeah, I mean, I think you're getting at a really important issue, right, which is, like, do we have a happiness set point?
Starting point is 01:36:54 And kind of, if we do, where does it come from, right? In your example, you know, kind of growing up cynically and having these kind of cynical messages, it could be that that was some sort of, youigenetic thing, or you're around all these people that are cynical and you learn how to do it. But it could be more of the genetic side. Maybe there's some pre-programmed sense of your negativity bias or something. We don't have great answers to those things.
Starting point is 01:37:16 But it's definitely true that our place really shapes a lot of our tendencies that matter for happiness. We know this from some of the local place things that you said place things that you said, I think, you know, my in-laws are from the Midwest and like, yeah, totally. They're just like great, decent, kind, happy people if they're listening right now. Like, I don't know, I'm trying to train to be like you. They're wonderful. But we also know from, like, even more macro level, right? So for now, for decades, the World Happiness Report in collaboration with the Gallup survey has been surveying happiness
Starting point is 01:37:45 of people across the world, right? And they come up with these like really consistent country level differences in happiness. The US for a very wealthy country is like not very happy. We're pretty low on the scale. And in fact, in the most recent World Happiness Report, we dipped like below like the top 10, like we've kind of had this had this major dive for the first time. Where are the happiest? Scandinavians. So it's usually Denmark, Norway. My stepmom's Danish.
Starting point is 01:38:10 I love going to Denmark. Yeah, Danish, exactly. Yeah, they're very happy people. So they're going to be happy. And so we can ask the question, what's the difference? Maybe it's the great Scandinavian genes, probably not. It's actually a lot of their cultural practices, which build on the sorts of things we're talking about.
Starting point is 01:38:24 Take social connection, right? There's a lot less work hours so people can go home and hang out with their family. There's a huge culture of clubs, for example, in Denmark where people go off and do sporting things. A lot of fitness, right? And the structure is to kind of get that fitness, right? Like nobody expects you to be at work.
Starting point is 01:38:40 So you can go ski or workout or hang out. I will say they're very effective when they work. They're very proficient. I mean, I'm struck by the average level of operational and intellectual intelligence of somebody. Like the person, your waiter in Denmark is an awesome waiter. Yeah. And oftentimes has very interesting things to say.
Starting point is 01:39:00 Like the level of proficiency and the level of focus when they are working is immensely high. So they're not just like kicking back all day. No. And I think in part, it's a different attitude towards work, that there's a time for work, but you don't let your work kind of like leak into other things. There's this woman, Helen Russell, who wrote a book about the happiness in Denmark, or the Danish past the happiness, I think is the name of the book.
Starting point is 01:39:23 And she had this quote of like, she was talking to people in Denmark, and there's often a thing that will happen where your manager has to talk to you at work and give you feedback. And it's in part because you're not leaving work on time. You're there over time, and they wanna have a conversation with you, like, what's your problem? Why can't you finish your work in the allotted hours? Which again, to American ears is like, what?
Starting point is 01:39:41 Your manager would never say that. I love it, I love it. But that's the social thing. But country level happiness is also affected by some of the thought patterns we talked about. The Scandinavians, even though it's cold and dark and nothing like it's here in California with you right now, they take joy in these tiny moments.
Starting point is 01:39:59 This idea of hygge, right? H-Y-G-G-E, hygge, where you notice the warmth of your coffee or have these candles or things. It's a society that's really focused on presence in a really rich way. Love that. Like I said, my father's Latin, he's from Argentina and he married a Danish woman.
Starting point is 01:40:15 And I would say much of their life is about cherishing and delighting in these small things. Yeah. The everyday things. I think that's, dare I say, I think that's one of their major points of convergence. I know, I'm sure there are others, but that's a major point of convergence.
Starting point is 01:40:34 I think, growing up in the United States, I sort of internalized this idea that, you're supposed to figure out who you are and go do big things. That was the message that I internalized. Part of that was the high school I went to, a super competitive high school and kind of people I tended to surround myself by.
Starting point is 01:40:54 But I think for some of us, the effort is in trying to learn to appreciate the little things. Having a dog, and we have to talk about dogs, because you've actually studied dogs extensively. Dogs and non-human primates in a natural setting and the other old world primates, humans. You know, people talk about how dogs are just present.
Starting point is 01:41:18 They're not thinking about the past. They're not thinking about the future. I'd like to challenge that just for a second. This isn't the cynic in me. This is the scientist in me, and I'm genuinely curious. How do we know that dogs aren't thinking a little bit about the past or the walk they're gonna take later
Starting point is 01:41:32 that afternoon? Do we know? I mean, they have a prefrontal cortex that can anticipate things. They have a memory system, they have a hippocampus and a cortex that can remember things. So how do we know that our dog isn't sitting there, yes, trying to glean as much sunshine as they can remember things. So how do we know that our dog isn't sitting there, yes, trying to glean as much sunshine as they can
Starting point is 01:41:46 on their belly through the window, but maybe they're thinking like, gosh, like, when are they gonna finish doing whatever it is they're doing so we can go outside and play ball? Yeah, it's such a hard question. It's such a hard question. And I mean, I think it's one that every dog owner
Starting point is 01:42:00 has really wondered about, right? I mean, I've thought about this question actually more in the monkeys, right? Who, you know, we can fight about dog neurobiology and they've got some of the stuff, but like they're kind of distinct, the tiny, you know, walnut brains rather than like primates.
Starting point is 01:42:13 Yeah, dogs have, I will just say that, well, first of all, dogs have, as far as I know, one of the most dramatic ranges in body size within a given species of animal. So Chihuahua Great Dane, I think it's the dosing of IGF-1 that regulates body size in dogs. It's got a beautiful cover of Science Magazine
Starting point is 01:42:29 that we can put a link to with a Chihuahua and a Great Dane. And it's just like, whoa, same species. They have relatively small brains relative to their body weight size, regardless. And if you look at what the brain's doing, a lot of it is sensory. I mean, a lot of it's olfactory, right? It's not the ruminative thinking about stuff
Starting point is 01:42:47 that we kind of have like expanded a lot in the primate brain. You know, we- Not a lot of prefrontal cortex. Not a lot of prefrontal cortex. Stuff right before, behind your forehead, folks, is the part that allows you to say shh to your impulses, to quiet your impulse, suppress them,
Starting point is 01:43:02 and also context-dependent learning and planning. So what to do, what to say, what, to quiet your impulse, suppress them, and also context dependent learning and planning. So what to do, what to say, what not to do, what not to say in a given environment. There's a lot in your brain about that, that is controlled by so-called executive function, the sort of conductor of the whole thing. And you're saying dogs have a pretty limited real estate there.
Starting point is 01:43:22 They're limited. And if you think about what that real estate does, it can kind of do that shush, it can take you out of the moment, but they're kind of related parts of, kind of cortex close by that's doing a lot of the work of thinking about past episodes, thinking about what other people are thinking, thinking about counterfactuals.
Starting point is 01:43:40 This is what humans are doing. This is what humans are doing, like the human big version of this, right? And this is the kind of stuff that gets us into trouble when it comes to presents. I think the dog's walking around, it's like, you know, I don't know what it's sniffing, like, hydrant, hydrant, hydrant,
Starting point is 01:43:53 don't, you know, like dog, dog, person, person. And I think it's there because it doesn't have as much circuitry to be like, well, this hydrant's not exactly as good as the other hydrant I smelled before. Like, what would, you know, Bob, the other dog, be thinking of this hydrant right now? I think-
Starting point is 01:44:06 Or they're still laughing at so-and-so from the dog park incident two weeks ago, right? Exactly. So much of human negative interaction is humans exchanging good and bad information about other humans. Oh, totally. It's like kind of the basis of,
Starting point is 01:44:22 not all, but a lot of social media. Yeah, and a lot of our rumination is us thinking about the other information that people have about us, right? You know, kind of rummaging. When in fact we have no idea what people are thinking. Exactly, exactly. Yeah. And so, you know, I often think about this.
Starting point is 01:44:34 In Buddhist circles, there's this discussion of the monkey mind, by which they mean the part of your mind that when you're trying to be present and focus on the moment, especially in practices like meditation, kind of runs off somewhere. That's your monkey mind running off and you just kind of yank it back by the tail or something. And I've always thought that was a real kind of unnecessary diss to monkeys, because my sense is at least the rhesus monkeys, which is a species I worked with, they seem a lot more like dogs than humans. I work with this group of monkeys in a field site called Cayo Santiago.
Starting point is 01:45:04 It's this island off the coast of Puerto Rico, and it's home to a thousand free-ranging rhesus monkeys. So we can do our studies and just kind of walk around with these monkeys who are kind of living freely. And you see them, and they just, you know, I'll sometimes sit near a monkey who's like sitting there looking out into the ocean and just sitting there. And I'm like, I bet what's going on in his head is not that human Buddhist version of the monkey mind where he's like, what about this ocean? When do I have to go
Starting point is 01:45:26 home? I have to cook something. Oh, what did my husband say to me? It's not that. I think the monkey's version is just like ocean. Ocean. It's just there. Or even better, like Costello, I used to look at him and think, what's going on in that brain of his? And then I realized it's probably, and this is a neurophysio, I wouldn't consider myself a neurophysiologist, but I've done some, certainly a fair number of recordings from the live brain. And I'm guessing most of what was in there
Starting point is 01:45:51 is what we call hash, not the drug, but it's background white noise. Shhh, shhh, shhh. That's the sound you hear on the audio monitor when there's no clean signal to noise. I'm guessing it's just hash, like shhh, shhh, shhh. I wonder if the term monkey mind, well, I'll just come clean.
Starting point is 01:46:09 I always thought that monkey mind was this image of a little monkey swinging from tree to tree and that it's the adjective sort of superimposed on the human brain. So excuse me, it's the verb of the moving monkey transformed into an adjective judgment about the human brain. But it's so sad if you're a monkey. Like, I think if monkeys had frontal cortex and talked to us,
Starting point is 01:46:32 we'd be like, don't blame us for your life. Exactly. It's the human brain part of the brain that should be around. Well, it's like bird brain. Yeah, exactly. You know, somebody who really appreciates raptors and diving birds. Think about the computations diving birds have to do.
Starting point is 01:46:43 They have to adjust for the refractory index of the water. So where they see the fish is not where the fish is. And, you know, so when people say bird brain, I'm like- Oh yeah, don't get me started on corvid cognition. These guys are the smartest, yeah. Smartest guys I've ever- So monkeys, but you're making me realize, I always thought rhesus macaques,
Starting point is 01:47:02 which are old world primates like us, or we're like them, that they had a fair amount of prefrontal real estate in their brain such that they could think and strategize and plan. I mean, if one just watches one episode of that Netflix special, which I love, which is Chimp Empire, there's all this stuff about who's in power
Starting point is 01:47:25 and then they're gonna team up and then they're gonna wait a few days until that one's injured and then they're not gonna groom the other one and boom, there's an overt, I mean, like very complicated. It's chess, not checkers for old world primates. Yeah, I think there could be a big distinction between what chimpanzees are doing.
Starting point is 01:47:43 I mean, they're our closest, you know, tied with Bonobos, our closest living relative, you know, that was what, like 30 million, you know, like it wasn't so long, right? Whereas rhesus monkeys are pretty far off, right? I think there could be a lot of things that happened in between. And we know that not necessarily from the neurobiology, because it's hard to ask kind of functional neurobiology questions with animals. You can't kind of put them in fMRI where they're doing behavior as easily as you can with a human. But we know it from cognition studies that look at things
Starting point is 01:48:12 like how good are, say, rhesus monkeys at perspective taking, at kind of taking on someone else's beliefs, knowing, oh, somebody's thinking something. Theory of mind. Theory of mind, yeah. Thinking something different than I'm thinking. And they're not so hot at it. They really use their own perspective Theory of mind. Theory of mind, yeah. Thinking something different than I'm thinking. And they're not so hot at it.
Starting point is 01:48:26 They really use their own perspective to make judgments pretty well. Same thing when we look at cases of like counterfactual thinking. Do you have regret over an outcome that you didn't get, right? Something that rhesus monkeys find kind of hard, right? So it seems like they're very good at sophisticatedly planning in the present moment, right? You and I are talking here right now. If you're watching the video, you can see I have a cup, like I might be planning to pick up the cup.
Starting point is 01:48:50 But the cup's here, everything. I'm not simulating, what if this was a lovely martini? That's the kind of thing that probably a human can do really well, but a monkey can't. So they can kind of plan and take next step actions when there's around the world that they experience, but they can't simulate worlds that are totally different. And that includes the kind of complicated stuff going on in somebody else's head. So they're good at short-term strategy.
Starting point is 01:49:14 This fits with what a friend of mine who studies behavior in macaques told me, which is that you can set up a really nice, complicated, on paper, perfect experiment where the monkey is going to inform you about some important feature of how the brain works in terms of behavioral economics or something. But then what you realize is, or what the monkey realizes, even if it doesn't consciously realize, is that no matter what, they're gonna get reward 50%
Starting point is 01:49:42 of the time on average, and they'll just hit the lever or give an answer as fast as they can so they get that day's ration of reward and just that's the end of the day. And so they're not cheating, they're just like why would I work any harder than this to actually do the experiment you want me to do? And so what many primate behavioral researchers
Starting point is 01:50:02 end up becoming are monkey trainers. Oh yeah. I mean the bane of every animal researcher's existence, this is whether you test dogs, monkeys, rodents, whatever, is what are called side biases. What's a side bias? It's you're giving an animal a choice between A and B. One, A's on the left, B's on the right. And rather than think through these complicated things you want them to think through, they're just like whatever, A, left, I'll go left, left, left, left, left, left. And you're like, no, I know you get rewarded 50% of the time, but I had this really creative question
Starting point is 01:50:29 I wanted you to pay attention and they just don't care. They don't care, well, they're- Or they're getting rewarded enough at 50% that you have to do something like, what researchers in the field, I mean, now we're getting really in the trenches called breaking a side bias. We're like, no, I'm gonna get more reward at B.
Starting point is 01:50:44 If you're only going to left, then I'm going to give more reward at right. We move them and stuff, but it does often seem to be the case that the monkeys are training us more than we're training the monkeys. I delight in that a little bit. I confess just a little bit, just a tiny bit. I mean, I'll share one story that kind of gets it, you know, this sort of perspective taking and how good they are, that they are good when it's kind of what's in the here
Starting point is 01:51:08 and now, right? We were doing these studies on the island which involved showing monkeys some food and we had these these eggplants in a box that we were making the monkeys look at for various reasons. We couldn't find a monkey to test on this island. You have to kind of hike around until you find a monkey who's kind of chilled out and whatever. We hiked around the whole island, it was
Starting point is 01:51:26 taking forever. We get back to our starting location and there's an eggplant that's sitting there and we're like, where'd that eggplant come from? And we're like, and it's got like bite marks out of it. We're like, how did that? And it was like, wait a minute, somebody must have stolen my eggplant. And we're like, well how did that happen? Like we were like, we were paying attention to the monkeys the whole time. We realized like, no, no, no, they must have stolen it when we were, like we probably put it down for a second and like they took it. Like we didn't drop it. It was like, and so it was like, we realized like, oh, not only are they
Starting point is 01:51:54 good at stealing, but they can tell like if we're looking at it. But looking is something that, you know, all animals pay attention to gaze. Like, you know, this is the kind of thing that even, you know, insects pay attention to, right? That's why they have these kind of markings that look like eyes so that birds won't eat them and stuff. Gaze following is really robust, but that's different than that monkey thinking, I bet that person's not looking. He's probably like, no eyes, I can grab it, right? But if you look kind of more sophisticatedly, they're not good at it. But this was another case of realizing like, oh, the monkeys are actually
Starting point is 01:52:22 a lot smarter than, than we give credit for. And maybe us too. Yeah. So what we're talking about is they are good at figuring out the basic rules, maybe even up to the level of what you call in computer programming and gates. If this and that are happening, then I go right. If that and that are not happening, then I don't do anything. And if that and a third option are happening, well, then I go right. If that and that are not happening, then I don't do anything. And if that and a third option are happening, well then I go left. Yeah.
Starting point is 01:52:51 And if all three are happening, it doesn't matter. Yeah. Like they can probably figure out, you know, two or three levels of and gates. But what they can't do is simulate all these various situations. I mean, the amazing thing about being a human is I could, you know, I could imagine any scenario.
Starting point is 01:53:05 Imagine like 700 podcast listeners jumped on this table right now. Imagine if the table was orange. Imagine if you were, I don't know, another podcaster. You're Malcolm Gladwell or whatever. Like, I'd get more hair. True. I mean, I can simulate all these infinite different things
Starting point is 01:53:20 to try to program that so hard. But it comes to us as humans in seconds. It's so fast. And it's the kind of thing that we use all the time. Honestly, it's the basis of a lot of our happiness. Look at the fiction that we engage in, right? We're constantly paying deep and close attention to fictional worlds. I've cared more about some fictional worlds than I've cared about my own family members. Sorry, family members, but it's true, right? Like you're reading a novel and you're like, I'm crying, I'm bawling, I'm cheering for these people that I know are completely made up
Starting point is 01:53:50 because our brains just kind of dive into these sort of fake worlds, these alternative worlds so easily and so quickly, so powerfully. I definitely want to continue along the dimension of how we construct our happiness. But I just want to make sure that I ask again about dogs. Yeah. And, you know, let's just do this
Starting point is 01:54:13 because, you know, I know it's so politically dangerous, but let's talk about the dog versus cat thing. Mm, yeah. You know, my sister loves cats. I don't dislike cats, but that doesn't mean I like them. I once rented a place where there was a big cat, I think it was one of these Maine Coon cats. His name was Baloo.
Starting point is 01:54:29 They're basically dogs. He's basically a dog. And Costello had certain cat-like qualities, like he just wanted to lounge all day. He could really move like a dog, but most of the time he was kind of like a cat at home and it was frustrating for me. But what do you think it is?
Starting point is 01:54:43 You have these feline people and you have dog people. And I'm definitely in the dog camp. But one of the reasons I love dogs is I assume they're in the present, but mostly it's the unconditional love. But what do you think it is, this dog-cat thing? Cats are presumably in the present as well. They don't make long-term plans, and if they do, they don't actualize those plans. So why is it that some people feel that cats are like
Starting point is 01:55:09 nasty animals that are plotting against them, and some people delight in cats? Yeah, and they don't have great data on it, but my sense is it gets back to the unconditional love idea. The cat, like, if you're the kind of person who craves the unconditional love, you wind up being more of a dog person. Yes.
Starting point is 01:55:23 But if you like the, what was it that your, was it your niece? Yeah, my niece, yeah. What was it that your niece said this like? No push me. She used to hold her finger up. Yeah. Like you could say we're going.
Starting point is 01:55:33 Cats are a lot more no push me. Unless we said we were going for ice cream, it was no push me. And I remember, and I still delight in it, thinking like, awesome. She had just, I mean mean like she had such a strong spirit from the time she was little. You might like cats because cats are a lot like no push me and I think this is... Oh no, I don't want a cat.
Starting point is 01:55:52 Well I think the like removal of the sort of no push me might be one of the last stages of evolution and domestication of dogs and I know this because in my dog work we also did some very fun work with dingoes which are the Australian wild dogs. We don't fully know their history. Our sense is like those dogs kind of like got pretty close to humans, pretty tolerant of humans, but didn't go like all the way to Costello in terms of the bond. And one of the amazing things, we interacted with this group at a sanctuary for dingoes out in Australia, one of the only sets of genetically pure dingoes in the world with
Starting point is 01:56:23 this wonderful privilege to work with. But we kind of had to like do our keep at these field stations. So we kind of went in and, you know, helped clean up with the dingoes and so on. And every morning you'd go out and you'd give the dingoes their food, which are these big kind of chickens. And they would just, I mean, they just kind of raw chickens, not live chickens, but they'd just like chomp it in one gulp, just like bones and all. And you're like, Whoa.
Starting point is 01:56:49 And then right after that, they'd want to be kind of, oh, come on, nudge me, be nice. It was like a cat in its best move. But then at a certain point, they were just like, no, like, I'll stop. They just had like such their own will in this really amazing way. And it just felt incredibly cat-like. I'm like, you look like a dog, but your behavior is so much more cat-like. So, so I don't know, no great studies on this. It'd be great to kind of figure it out. But my sense is distinction between dog people and cat people might be the unconditional love, no push me ratio of what people like.
Starting point is 01:57:15 So interesting. And I like to think did not take us too far off course in our discussion about happiness because we clearly delight in this. And hopefully people do too, is that we think about the different brain architectures and the different capabilities that different brain architectures have across species.
Starting point is 01:57:31 And the fact that we live in such close proximity as some of these species is wild. When I was growing up, not everyone had a dog unless they had the space for it. Now I feel like dogs are everywhere. Yeah, I mean, it's like such an enormous billion dollar industry to have dogs. And it raises a question that kind of gets us back to some of the happiness work, like,
Starting point is 01:57:47 which is, is that a good idea? Right? All these people are investing their time, their energy, their spaces in dogs. You know, you could ask the question, do they make us happier? And I know they do. Yeah, I'm not going to break everybody's heart. We've killed the cat people. No, no, the cat people.
Starting point is 01:58:02 No, pet dogs in particular, but pets in general, wind up making us happier. Pet owners are statistically happier. And I think it's for a couple of reasons based on the stuff we just talked about. Take the behavioral pattern that matters, social connection. For sure, dogs provide that social connection themselves. We just talked about they tap into your caregiving system and so on. But as you talked about in your interactions with like, you know, bulldog pleading, you know, like bulldog, you know,
Starting point is 01:58:27 what was the phrase you used? When you see a bulldog and you say bulldog something. Oh, when I see somebody with a bulldog, I just say, excuse me, there's a bulldog tax. Oh yeah, bulldog tax, sorry. And then I pet their bulldog. Yeah, good, let me go back. And if you're gonna meet a bulldog,
Starting point is 01:58:39 just understand they're shaped like a beer keg, so they can't scratch themselves on their hindquarters. So if you give them a scratch there, they're like, thank you, because it's got to be just awful. It's like having that scratch in the middle of your back and you can't reach it. So you do the bulldog text and you have this nice social connection with the bulldog, but my guess is, and because you're using verbal language, you also get connected with the person.
Starting point is 01:59:01 You probably say, oh my gosh, what's his name? Oh, when did you get him? Blah, blah, blah. That's chatting with the priest at the probably say, oh my gosh, what's his name? Oh, when did you get him? Blah, blah, blah. That's chatting with the breeze at the coffee shop, right? That's doing the Nick Eppley experiments we just talked about before. Pets wind up bringing us social connection. And one of the pieces of advice if you're feeling lonely
Starting point is 01:59:15 is get an animal not just so that the animal will give you some comfort, but particularly with a dog, you get to walk that animal and then people talk to you. It's much easier to connect with people when you have dogs. So social connection is huge. Second thing is, you know, particularly again for dogs, what are people doing? They're getting out and walking, right? So we're getting some people who never had a lot of physical exercise before are at least getting the kind of walks they need to do in for the dog. And even if they won't choose to do it for themselves, they often choose
Starting point is 01:59:42 to do it for their dog. So you get, So you get exercise in, which is good for physical health, and we haven't talked about, but is enormously good for happiness. Meta-analyses showing a half hour of cardio exercise a day is as good as an anti-depression medication for reducing symptoms of depression. So just that walk with your dog is great. But beyond that, I think they help our thought patterns.
Starting point is 02:00:01 And this is true for dogs, I think, and cats. Where, as you were saying, we're wondering what they're doing. Sometimes if they're sitting there and we're just petting them, what we're doing is we're sitting there and we're petting them. So they give us these wonderful sensory experiences. And I think they cause us to be a little bit more present, especially when we're kind of interacting with them. You know, we're interacting with our dogs, unless you're taking the Instagram
Starting point is 02:00:19 pictures of the dog, but usually when you're playing with the dog or whatever, you're just there, you're not on your phone. You're just kind of mindfully experiencing life with your dog, kind of like when you talked about your road trip. Part of what probably brought you into the present moment, especially if your girlfriend was working, was that the dog was interacting with you.
Starting point is 02:00:35 So the dogs help us not because they're inherently kind of happiness-inducing. They help us tick these boxes of better behaviors for happiness, better thought patterns for happiness, and they're kind of a delight. So they kind of give us some positive emotion too. I love all of that. I just want to double click
Starting point is 02:00:54 if it were on this idea that they can be a bridge for social connection. That's really powerful. A friend of mine who used to smoke cigarettes who doesn't any longer. In fact, I remember when I was a postdoc at Stanford, that like the mostly the foreign postdocs, but they used to gather outside
Starting point is 02:01:10 and have cigarette cigarettes breaks all the time. Now you're not allowed to smoke on the medical school campus. And I think probably on the main campus too, most places you're not allowed to smoke outdoors. Right, because of secondhand smoke, in any case. But he said to me, you know, it used to be that before people either knew or fully internalized how bad smoking was, is that asking for a cigarette
Starting point is 02:01:34 or sharing a cigarette side by side with somebody was a way that people engaged in casual interaction, not just outside of bars, not just to meet potential mates, et cetera, but it was just, it was a bridge. You could walk up to somebody and say, you know, like, we could call it like bombing a smoke. Like you asked for a cigarette or if someone was smoking,
Starting point is 02:01:50 you could go stand by them and you also smoke. And so it was this terribly health diminishing habit, but it served as a lot of social lubricant. Yeah, it also gives you another behavior that we know is really important for happiness, which is time. There's a lot of social science research on this phenomenon that's called time affluence, which is a sort of subjective sense that you feel wealthy in time. You kind of just
Starting point is 02:02:11 have a break, right? You get a, like a smoking break is one of these, right? You get a break. And often people, you know, back in the day when smoking was allowed, one of the ways that you got your breaks often in not so great workplaces was like, you could ask for a smoke break. My mom talks about this. She was a teacher educator for a super long time where you don't get a lot of breaks, but you know, back in the 70s if you're a smoker, they let you go outside for 10 minutes and that was a sort of break, right? So I think this other unhealthy habit kind of gave us the opportunity to take breaks, which we know are great for happiness and so great for happiness that if you don't
Starting point is 02:02:41 have any of this so-called time affluence, a sense that you have some free time. If you experience what researchers call time famine, where you feel like almost starving for time, it's a huge hit on your well-being. If you self-report in these surveys being time famished, so I don't have time to meet up with my friends, I never have time for the stuff I want to do, that's as big a hit on your well-being as if you self-report being unemployed. You know, listeners, if you lost, you have a job and you lost it tomorrow, you'd probably think that that is a big hit on your happiness. Just not having any time for the little breaks in life is as bad. And this gets back to our earlier discussion about money and happiness,
Starting point is 02:03:15 which is researcher Ashley Willans at Harvard Business School has kind of pushed the idea that what's going on with these low-income folks who have a real hit on happiness, right, not having a high income hurts your happiness. her theory is a lot of that actually has to do with time. Because if you have a really low income, you don't have a car to get to work, so you're taking the bus and it's taking you forever, you're working multiple jobs, right? A lot of the reason that money affects happiness and not having money affects happiness is
Starting point is 02:03:40 that it co-varies with not having time. And the real hit on our happiness is just the time part more so than the money part. So we need to be in pursuit of things, we need to work, but we also need some free time. We can't have too much free time or too much work, basically. And the sweet spot is often hard to maintain or even know what that sweet spot is. Yeah, I think that this term kind of time affluence and what researchers mean, Maya, is helpful here, right? It's the subjective sense that you have some free time. It's not I objectively go into your calendar
Starting point is 02:04:10 and you show me how many open blocks there are. It's your sense that you have a break. And this provides an interesting hack that we can use to get more of it, right? Which is that we can kind of just frame things as having more time. Because sometimes when you get a break that you don't expect, it can feel like a lot.
Starting point is 02:04:27 I teach this class about happiness on Yale's campus, and I talk about time affluence. It's one of the topics in my class. And I always felt that was really ironic, because our young people today, especially at elite college institutions, are so time-famished. They're running from thing to thing, and have a million extracurriculars, and so on.
Starting point is 02:04:43 So I felt willed, like, I'm going to lecture them for an hour on time affluence and tell them all these studies. And so what I did was in the syllabus, I said there's a lecture on time affluence and they come to class and I have my teaching assistants that are handing out little flyers that say today's lecture is about time affluence and to teach you what that is, I'm going to give you some. No class today.
Starting point is 02:05:03 So you didn't know, you walked to class. Now you got a free hour and a half. And it just happened to be one of these unusually warm, California-esque days in New Haven where it's sunny out. So kids got a bubble tea with their friend, or some of them went on a hike near the local state park. And one of the students I remember burst into tears when she got this form. And she said, this is the first free hour and a half
Starting point is 02:05:26 I've had for like the last three months. Wow, they're that stressed. They're that stressed. But what I find interesting about this is like, I didn't give them a month off vacation, right? I gave them an hour back unexpectedly, and it felt like it was huge. And Andrew, I don't know your schedule,
Starting point is 02:05:41 but sometimes my schedule can be so overwhelmed and so packed that there's a half hour meeting that gets canceled. And I'm just like, ugh. And like, the relief, I feel like I could learn a new language. You just feel like it's a half hour, right? And I think this is a hack we can use for ourselves, right? Listeners right now, go on your calendar a few months from now and just take months, months away, pick an hour period and just write in like, Hman lab time affluence and just don't put anything in that.
Starting point is 02:06:07 And my guess is when you get to that hour that you've scheduled months later, you'll just be like, oh my gosh, this feels great. We can kind of gift ourselves these little windows of time. Another hack we can do is to make good use of the free time we do have. And this is kind of a puzzle of something that I found unexpected when I saw the data on this, which is that it turns out we actually have more free time now than we did like 10, 15 years ago, if you add it up. Not just kind of post-COVID, but in general, we've
Starting point is 02:06:32 been getting more free time. However, the free time we have is cut up differently. It's in smaller chunks. It's like five minutes when that Zoom meeting ends a little earlier, 10 minutes if your kid falls asleep early, or whatever it is. And we don't think it's that much, so we just kind of blow it. But if you add it up, it winds up more than people in past decades have had, and probably
Starting point is 02:06:52 like good time that we could use for stuff. And so these little chunks of time are what the journalist Bridget Schultz calls time confetti, which I think is such a great image of it. It's this little, you know, five minutes here and there. But you can do a lot with those minutes if you add them up. We just have to use them a little bit more intentionally, right? And that could be, you know, for some of the stuff you talk
Starting point is 02:07:11 about in this podcast a lot, like use that, you do the seven minute New York Times workout, you know, do the kinds of things we're talking about, you know, that's the time you text your friend and have a delay or- Get some sunlight. Get some sunlight, walk outside, right? Walk in sunlight, huge or just face the sun. Problem is what do we do when we get the time confetti? Or I mean, what do I do when I'm in a bad moment? Get some sunlight. Get some sunlight. Walk outside, right? Yeah. Walk in sunlight, huge or just face the sun.
Starting point is 02:07:25 The problem is what do we do when we get the thyme confetti? Or, I mean, what do I do when I'm in a bad moment? Blot our phone, check our email, scroll through. It's like, again, this sort of nutrous sweet dopamine hit that's not being effective. So if you feel really overwhelmed and you objectively don't have a lot of time, remember that the thyme confetti that you already have, it's already sitting there, can be really valuable if you use it well. Super important because I think the filling of those spaces with what I love the analogy
Starting point is 02:07:54 to NutraSweet or artificial sweeteners is it's going to taste like it's providing some sort of nourishment, but it's probably just creating a more sense of craving and want at some level. Would really like to talk about reward circuitry just thematically listeners of this podcast. And even if they've never heard one of these podcasts before, probably familiar with the word dopamine.
Starting point is 02:08:16 We've talked about it a bit. And as we were talking about earlier, everything about the dopamine reward circuitry, which of course includes other chemicals too, is based on prior experience relative to current experience relative to anticipated outcome. What's sometimes referred to as reward prediction error. Think something great's gonna happen,
Starting point is 02:08:36 something great happens, great. Think something great's gonna happen, something less than great happens, sucks way more than you would anticipate. Think that something not so great happens sucks way more than you would anticipate. Think that something not so great is gonna happen, something so so great happens, huge reward. Novelty surprise brings the positive novelty and surprise brings the biggest rewards.
Starting point is 02:08:57 And this is what I would like to kind of paint as the backdrop, think about it as a conceptual mural behind us, is I asked the question, maybe, just maybe, we're not supposed to be happy all the time or maybe even all that often. And when we're feeling not so great or even lousy,
Starting point is 02:09:21 provided it's not dangerous levels of depression. Maybe we should frame that as the backdrop for the greater happiness that will come when we start to emerge from that lousy state. Now, some people would say, well, now you're just kind of using neurobiology to twist around what would otherwise be a lousy experience and tell me that it's good for me.
Starting point is 02:09:41 No, what I'm trying to say is, people want to be happy. I think we'd all love to be happy all the time, but we're not wired to be happy all the time. And maybe the feelings of happiness can't exist unless they have contrast with these neutral or negative emotion states that we call, I don't know, feeling lousy, feeling anxious, et cetera.
Starting point is 02:10:02 And just, I realize I can pose long questions, but I just want to provide a little bit more context for the moment, which is that every circuit in the brain, our ability to see light, literally, depends on the contrast with the so-called off circuitry, which is the circuitry in our visual system that perceives darkness. We need contrast to be able to see light.
Starting point is 02:10:25 Everything's push-pull hunger satiety cold heat perception Go no-go. It's all push-pull circuitry in there. Why wouldn't happiness have a push-pull relationship with unhappiness or at least? Neutral affect. Yeah. Well, I think it does I mean you're giving a neurobiological explanation for what psychologists in this field of positive psychology have referred to as what's called hedonic adaptation, which is a fancy way of saying we get used to stuff.
Starting point is 02:10:56 You know, you like grab the delicious ice cream cone, or I know we're Cuban, we do a delicious salad, really healthy, but it's a tasty, healthy tasty salad, right, start eating it. First bite is like, this is awesome, salad, really healthy, but it's a tasty, healthy, tasty salad, right? Start eating it. First bite is like, this is awesome. I'm so into it, it's great. Bite number two, a little bit less awesome, a little bit less awesome.
Starting point is 02:11:11 By the 10th bite, it's not because you're full or you're like, you know, feeling disgusted. It's just like that sensory experience, you've gotten used to it, right? It's just no longer as interesting. Walk into a bakery. Yeah, exactly. Mm, it smells amazing.
Starting point is 02:11:23 Spend five minutes in the bakery, 10 minutes in the bakery, you attenuate, you habituate. Yeah, which is great. I mean, you wouldn't maybe want to be firing your neurons, you'd get all exhausted and stuff. But it's in one way terrible for happiness, in another way very good for happiness, but in a major way terrible for happiness, which is the following. Every good thing in life, if it sticks around, becomes kind of boring over time. You're just kind of used to it. I use the example sometimes of, you know, the last sticks around, becomes kind of boring over time. You're just kind of used to it. I use the example sometimes of the last time, the first time your partner said, I love you, or if you had a kid, the first time your kid said, mommy or daddy, that feels amazing,
Starting point is 02:11:57 right? But like, you know, last week my husband said, I love you. He's like, whatever, I'm just used to it, right? You know, last week when your kid was like, I love you, mom. Like, you know, mommy, like, you don't care. Right? The most amazing thing in life, if it gets repeated, just becomes boring. And that sucks because, you know, you like the most amazing things in life to kind of keep being awesome.
Starting point is 02:12:17 It's pretty sad that we don't have it. Right? This has a flip side though, which is very good for happiness, hedonic adaptation, which is the most terrible thing in life can happen. And over time you get used to that too. So your partner breaks up with you. You find out you have a chronic disease, right? Just something like really bad happens. Day one when you find out that piece of information, it is awful. But day two, yeah, still awful, but that's just your life. And then over time, it kind of gets better. There's a very famous study in the field of happiness science that tried to look at this
Starting point is 02:12:50 with people who experienced a really great event in theory, winning the lottery, and people who experienced really bad events, real events in life, becoming paraplegic. So you used to be able to walk and now you've lost the use of your legs. You survey happiness in people who haven't had these experiences and you ask, predict how bad it would be to have this. People say, you know, day one of winning the lottery would be really great. And you know, a year from now, a year from that point, winning the lottery would still be just as great.
Starting point is 02:13:16 It'd be awesome. Same thing with paraplegic. You know, your moment you become paraplegic, that day is a really crappy Thursday, but a year from then is still just as crappy. And what you find is people, you know, on the day you become paraplegic, that day is a really crappy Thursday, but a year from then is still just as crappy. And what you find is people, you know, on the day you become paraplegic or the day you win your lottery, like that's a big shift in your contrast, right? You know, the day you win the lottery is an awesome Thursday. Day you become paraplegic is terrible. But a year from then it turns out your happiness is no different from baseline
Starting point is 02:13:40 from the day before that event happened, right? Statistically. Um, and that is shocking. Like, I know these results. I can quote the paper. But if you told me today, Laura, you walk out of the studio, you get hit by a car, you're paraplegic, how would you feel in 2026?
Starting point is 02:13:54 I'd be like, my life is still really crummy. But statistically, that's just not going to happen. What does that mean? That's kind of good news about hedonic adaptation for happiness. That means the worst thing possible could happen to you, and you have all these processes that are just going to get used to it over time,
Starting point is 02:14:11 and it's going to be OK. And I think this is an important aspect of our psychology that we forget. I think sometimes we have opportunities to do things in life that are a little risky, something we might try out that we might screw up or fail at or that we'll be bad at at first. And we don't do it because we're scared. We're making we're making a prediction like, oh, well, if I failed
Starting point is 02:14:28 or if I screwed that up, you know, I'd just be unhappy. But actually, all these mechanisms that we have of hedonic adaptation means those things aren't going to affect you for as long as you think. So I think the contrast hypothesis about happiness is real. Good things don't stay good things over time, but the bad things don't either. But we still want the good things to stay good over time. And so that raises a question of how we can do that.
Starting point is 02:14:52 And Liz Dunn, whose work I've mentioned before, she likes to use this phrase that scarcity engineers happiness, right? One thing we can do is space out the good things in life. So if I was having that really delicious, healthy salad with the avocado and whatever, if I was having that really delicious, healthy salad with the avocado and whatever, if I had that every day, it would stop being good.
Starting point is 02:15:09 But if I had it very, very infrequently, it would still be good every time I come back to it. And so sometimes, oddly, the way we make ourselves happier is to kind of remove positive experiences, especially extreme positive experiences, and kind of space them out so we can kind of come back to them over time. I definitely agree with that.
Starting point is 02:15:29 I also, and forgive me folks, but I think I understand why dogs are so awesome. They don't attenuate to reward. You tell them they're going to get this little piece of amazing whatever beef jerky or something, and they're like, yes. And second trial, yes beef jerky or something. And they're like, yes. And then second trial, yes. Third trial, yes.
Starting point is 02:15:47 I mean, presumably at some point they reach satiety or fatigue, but there's something about their reward pathways that they don't seem to attenuate much. And if there's feedback to us on that, it's like, okay, okay, you know, it's great that they'll keep delighting in the simple little things. It seems like almost as much as the first time. Yeah.
Starting point is 02:16:10 We are not like that. It's interesting, which by knowledge people haven't studied hedonic adaptation in dogs, but it's a really good question, but we are not like that for most things. And this sucks, right? I mean, it's also the case that in addition to kind of getting used to stuff over time, it's also
Starting point is 02:16:26 showing a different feature, which is a more particular contrast feature you're talking about. So over time, we kind of habituate. That's one sort of neural mechanism. But another is the one that you mentioned, which is about the contrast, right? And that's what you see kind of. You see both of them, say, in the light perception, right?
Starting point is 02:16:41 If I show you the same light over time, you're going to habituate. That's hedonic adaptation. For folks listening, it literally disappears. If I set up the right experiment, Russ and Karen Deavalloy at Berkeley years ago did these beautiful experiments. You look at like a grating of light projected onto a wall
Starting point is 02:16:58 and if you can stabilize the eyes so that they're not moving around, it literally will disappear. And the same thing with an odor, same thing with touch, right? Like I wasn't thinking about my contact with the chair. Same thing with happiness, my deliciousness of salad. Habituation, attenuation, these are technical terms
Starting point is 02:17:13 when you really get down into it. And the push-pull antagonism between light and dark, the smell, yes, no, on, off, push, all of it, go, no, go. Every single aspect of the nervous system functions this way. The flexor extensor in the musculoskeletal system. But that gets to maybe what I would think of as different. So heat ionic adipation is the same stimulus over time, like almost like habituation.
Starting point is 02:17:35 There's a different thing that happens when you get what you might call a contrast. And there's all kinds of visual illusions that sort of function on this. If you've ever seen the one where it's like, you know, is it the same color over here, over here? We throw this on your show page to show people. And it's like, oh, it looks different.
Starting point is 02:17:50 It's like, no, no, no, that's because of the kind of contrast between the two things. You see something that's really bright over here, it makes something else look a little darker, right? That's a different negative effect on our happiness a lot of the time. This is the comparison effect, right? This is like, my $50 million seems kind of crappy because I hang out with people who, you know, have $100 million. Objectively, I have a tremendous amount of money, but I feel bad because I'm kind of comparing against something else. And so oftentimes,
Starting point is 02:18:17 when we're evaluating different rewards, we're kind of comparing them against what other people had or what we've had in the past. And that means that being in an objectively good situation might feel really crappy if you just have somebody else that has a slightly better objectively good situation. My favorite example of this actually comes from the sports world. So researchers asked this interesting question, like how happy are you when you win an Olympic medal?
Starting point is 02:18:41 You're on the stand, you won an Olympic medal. And also who's happiest? So gold medalists is up there best in the world, you might assume, they're the happiest, right? You're on the stand, you won an Olympic medal. And also, who's happiest? So gold medalists is up there, best in the world, you might assume, they're the happiest, right? And they are, they're smiling. The researchers analyze this by looking at facial expressions and kind of code the muscles and so on. But turns out they're not the happiest, right?
Starting point is 02:18:57 Who's the happiest? Well, let's look at the silver medalist. Are they happiest? No. In fact, actually, if you code their facial muscles, they're showing expressions like contempt, deep sadness. This is the same expression you'd make like if your parent died or like a real terrible grief moment.
Starting point is 02:19:11 This is the, I don't want to adhere to this, but this is the quote unquote second place's first loser kind of mindset. Because the idea is like, you know, who's your major comparison point if you're in silver, you know, 0.2 seconds or something, you would have gotten gold. And you're not feeling objectively like you're the second best on the planet. You'd be all but one of billions of people on the planet. No, you just feel terrible. So that's silver medalist.
Starting point is 02:19:33 What's going on with the bronze medalist, right? There's another person on the stand. What's their comparison point? It's not gold. They were multiple people, multiple seconds away. Their salient comparison is like, by the grace of God, like, I'm up here at all. I almost like, you know, two seconds the other direction, I would have never gotten up here. And when you analyze the bronze medalist facial expressions, they're
Starting point is 02:19:52 sometimes even happier than the gold medalist, definitely happier than the silver who's objectively better, but sometimes even happier than the gold medalist because they're like, relative to my comparison point, I'm doing amazing. And the gold medalist is expected to get gold the next year, or else it's pure reward prediction error, especially if they internalize the expectations of the audience, the spectators, excuse me, because if they come back the next year
Starting point is 02:20:17 and they're second or third on the podium or not on the podium, it's seen as falling from a higher place. Exactly. This is a point that I make with my Ivy League students who've been perfect in their grades and perfect at everything to get into a place like Yale, which is like, turns out that's a terrible recipe for happiness. The only way forward is stay there, down,
Starting point is 02:20:35 or create a new opportunity. Stay there, you don't notice, right? Because you're habituated to it, just like the pattern. Down feels really bad. That's a terrible comparison. I often play my students that DJ Khaled song, all I do is win, all I do is win, win, win. And I was like, all you do is win, win, win,
Starting point is 02:20:49 would be a terrible way to experience success in life because you just stop noticing it over time if you won. And that's messed up because it means when you get, when you finally hit the success that you were striving for, if you just stay at that level, just stops being good, which sucks. And so that raises a different question, which is like, what is a hack that we can do
Starting point is 02:21:11 to get away from that? One is to not look for the silver lining, but to look for the bronze lining, ba-dum-bum, which is, you know, you kind of think of reference points that are lower than you are from. I love a good conceptual pun, especially when it's framed in an experiment, so thank you for that.
Starting point is 02:21:25 Yeah, it was like, science experiment is good. Yes, yes. So look for the bronze lining, which means find a reference point that's not as good. And for most of the things you're comparing, whether that's your looks, your fitness level, your finances, you can look and find somebody that's doing worse than you. Another great hack for this, and this is more of one
Starting point is 02:21:43 that's a kind of a hack for hedonic adaptation, getting used to stuff, actually comes from the ancient traditions. I know you talk a lot about, you know, smart folks back in the day who came up with this stuff, right? This is one from the Stoic tradition, a practice called negative visualization. So Stoics like Marcus Aurelius thought, when you wake up in the morning, you should have the following thought pattern. You should think, today, I will lose my success. I will be exiled. I'll lose my partner. I will lose my success. I will be exiled. I'll lose my partner. I will lose my health.
Starting point is 02:22:07 I won't be able to walk. It doesn't say ruminate on that for forever, but just like a little and then stop and say, huh, I'm not exiled. I still have my success. I still have my partner and so on. This is a technique called negative visualization, where you just imagine you don't have to live it in real life.
Starting point is 02:22:22 You just imagine you lose something. If you've ever lost something you're hedonically adapted to, you know how quickly you recognize the value of it. This happens to me with my phone all the time, I'm a chronic phone loser, and I'm like, you know, and I'm like, oh my God, my phone is gone, I left it in the airport, all my contacts are there, da da da, and then I'm like, oh, it's in the car.
Starting point is 02:22:40 You have this, I love my phone, like it's so valuable to me. There's that line in Pulp Fiction where he says like, my phone. Like it's so valuable to me. There's that line in pulp fiction where he says like, what is it? It's like it's finding, at some point I think it was Travolta says something, someone will know it, where finding it almost made losing it worth it. Exactly. Like because you appreciate it in a way
Starting point is 02:22:59 that you didn't before because it was taken away from you. And that sucks to really lose your phone. Sometimes in my case, you really lost the phone, right? But negative visualization, you don't have to do that. You just use your imagination, right? And so if you're listening right now and you have a kid, let's do this negative visualization. The last time you saw your kid was the last time you ever saw them. Never gonna see them again.
Starting point is 02:23:19 Nope, didn't happen. You don't have to remitted about it. But my guess is the next time you hug your kid, just that two seconds of thinking about what things would be like without it can break through hedonic adaptation. So one of my favorite hacks for hedonic adaptation, you can use scarcity, really space things out.
Starting point is 02:23:36 But for the things you can't space out, you can't have a kid and get rid of a kid for two weeks and come back to your kid, right? You can use your imagination. It doesn't take much to start to realize what you have and appreciate it more, so. I love this one mostly because I think most people, including myself really,
Starting point is 02:23:54 we want to avoid thinking negative stuff, especially on purpose. But what you're telling us is that it provides a wonderful contrast point to kind of trampoline off into the reality that is our current reality, which is far better than these horrible scenarios. And I think this gets to another domain in which I see kind of toxic positivity playing out a lot,
Starting point is 02:24:13 which is kind of in this sort of domain of like, how do we do this stuff better, right? Like, how do we kind of get good things in life? And there's a lot of talk in some circles about this idea of like manifesting, right? I'll just think about, you know, I'm feeling lonely. I'll just think about what it's like to have friends or I'm not fit right now. I'll just imagine, you know, my fit future and fantasize about what it's like to run marathons and things like that. Turns out
Starting point is 02:24:36 this can have like a, this can be a case where you're using imagination in a bad way because what happens when you really deeply imagine, say, the rewards of like being super fit, you start to get like your brain's like firing the reward cylinders for like what it feels like to be super fit. And there's evidence from Gabrielle Oettingen's lab at NYU that like you actually get less motivated to do stuff. She does this in the context of fitness. She has people who want to run a 5k or want to lose some weight. For example, they talk about like, you know, imagine how great it would be to do this.
Starting point is 02:25:05 And they're less motivated to put on their running shoes in practice because they've already imagined the fantasy future turns out instead of manifesting a better technique, if you have some habit that you want to engage in is to imagine the obstacles, the bad stuff that's coming up, right? So, Oh, I want to get out and run this 5k. Well, what's the obstacle to that? I'm going to be in bed and alarm's going to go off. What's going to happen?
Starting point is 02:25:26 Like, oh, I'm going to be too warm. Like, maybe I put my running clothes on. Or, oh, like, I'm not going to want to, it's going to be cold out. Like, oh, I should get a nice fuzzy hat to be able to do this. This, Otingen's work shows that if you actually imagine the negative things, again, not ruminating about
Starting point is 02:25:41 them freaking out, but imagine particularly the obstacles for a habit you want to engage in. You kind of naturally come up with solutions to those obstacles, which makes it easier. So sometimes thinking about the bad stuff can be helpful. We just have to regulate when we do it. I have a friend who's a cardiologist from UCSF, and he says, you know, the danger of telling people
Starting point is 02:26:00 that you're going to write a book or that you're going to start a podcast or that you're going to start a company is that if you have very supportive friends and if you tend to be a pretty high agency person, you'll get a lot of praise and a lot of reward and there's a lower probability that you'll actually do the thing
Starting point is 02:26:14 because you've derived some of the reward. Whereas if people tell you, you know, yeah, that seems kind of unlikely given that this and that, you know, it doesn't feel so good. And obviously we want to encourage each other. This is the complicated thing. It's a, you know, it's a, it's a, it's a very narrow beam to walk on. You want to encourage people, but you don't want to give them so much reward that then
Starting point is 02:26:34 it undercuts their, their motivation. And you certainly don't want to discourage them to the point where they give up on themselves prior to even trying. That's right. But at least in the United States, probably in other countries too, goodness, do we love a story about somebody who was told like they couldn't do it and they did it.
Starting point is 02:26:53 You know, I think about the enormous popularity of David Goggins, who had a truly difficult childhood and internalized all these messages of how terrible he was and then used those voices, other people's and his own in his head to push himself to do tremendously difficult things. And then to continue to do tremendously difficult things and to self-publish one of the most popular
Starting point is 02:27:16 self-published books of all time. And then to go off and become a medic. And now he's effectively doing the training of somebody going to medical school for his new training, like he just refuses to stop. And it's, according to him, sat in that very chair and said it's fueled by an internal voice of you can't do it.
Starting point is 02:27:35 And then he fights back against that voice, which is oh so different than manifesting this image of success. Exactly. And there's, again, this is a case where there's nuance. You have to believe it's possible, right? Those negative voices can't tell you it's impossible because something else we know about motivation
Starting point is 02:27:50 is that believing something is possible, which requires lots of effort, but it is possible is quite helpful for you. The best example of this comes from another sort of sporting case. I don't know if you know the case of Roger Bannister, who's the first guy to run the four minute mile. Run under.
Starting point is 02:28:07 Break the four minute. Break the four minute mile. And before he did that, people thought it was like physiologically impossible. Like the human body cannot do this. And he was like, no, the human body can do this. And everyone was like, Roger, you're crazy, whatever. But then he, you know, trained and trained
Starting point is 02:28:21 and probably had to overcome his obstacles. He ran it. And then within like two months, somebody else broke the four minute mile. Like it had not been broken in all of human history, but as soon as people had evidence of like, oh, people can do that, like now everybody does it. Now I don't know.
Starting point is 02:28:33 I mean, as you can see, not a fit person, but like lots of people run four minute miles. It's not like the hugest thing. High schoolers run it. Yeah. Which is crazy. I mean. But the point is that they're falling,
Starting point is 02:28:41 they're probably helped out by this thing called the banister effect. Like they know it's possible, right? So if you train, if you run into obstacles, if you don't get that time, you're not like, well, I guess it's physiologically, I just can't do it. You can kind of do it. And so there's this idea with the Banister effect, you kind of have to be optimistic enough to think that it's doable.
Starting point is 02:29:00 But when you think that it's doable, it's really helpful to ask the question, OK, what are the things that are going to come in the way of my doing it? Maybe imagine them really kind of vividly so you get a sense. It can super help it. One of the things that contrasts a country like Denmark, for instance, compared to the United States, and I know this from discussions with my stepmother, is that in this country we have this notion, because we have a lot of examples of people that went from absolutely nothing
Starting point is 02:29:28 to these tremendously quote unquote high places, financially, reputationally, et cetera, performance in whatever domain, sometimes overnight. This last year, I would say two events stick out in my mind as like, whoa, like, wow. The first was seeing the SpaceX rocket get captured by the quote unquote chopsticks. That was just a rocket landing of all things.
Starting point is 02:29:54 As it was so cool. Everybody, regardless of what else was going on with, you know, people's opinions of SpaceX or Elon or whatever, we're just like, whoa, that was just an awesome feat of engineering. Just undeniably awesome feat of engineering. So it sets a bigger upper ceiling on what we thought was possible.
Starting point is 02:30:15 We're seeing something that we hadn't seen before, at least not like that, not at that scale and resolution. So that changes what one conceptualize about what's possible in different aspects of life. And I think that's important. It really lifts the ceiling. The other was very different example was the overnight, the hock to a girl, right?
Starting point is 02:30:37 Who nobody knew of, right? Made a comment in a passing video on the street, one of these like whatever spontaneous interview things and then now has a quite successful podcast was ranked one of the highest new podcasts of the year. Has a, as far as I understand, has a staff and a thriving business now. And like, you know, took, you know,
Starting point is 02:30:57 this is a very American thing, right? To go from an unknown to one or two quick comments to all of a sudden being a famous and presumably famous and somewhat wealthy person as well. And, you know, wish her nothing but luck in evolving that show. Good luck, Huck, good luck. Yeah, good luck, yeah, sure.
Starting point is 02:31:17 Like I love to see people win, you know, like, okay. It was an unusual trajectory, but not so unusual for the United States in some sense, because we also have the people who climb the staircase or the people that climb the staircase fell, then came back. We love and we cherish these stories in this country. And I think it frames the young mind in an interesting way.
Starting point is 02:31:41 It sets this anything as possible. Oftentimes it takes clearly a ton of hard work. Oftentimes at the expense of other aspects of one's mental or physical health or life, life enrichment, family, et cetera. But it's a very American thing for people to be like anything's possible. What do you think that does to our level of happiness
Starting point is 02:32:04 if we're somebody that is looking for happiness, wants a good life, wants resources, but doesn't like, maybe they feel a little guilty that they're not as quote unquote ambitious as everybody else. But then you contrast that with a country like Denmark where people are very happy. They're certainly ambitious Danes,
Starting point is 02:32:25 but they're actually, I was told that the word ambition is a little bit of a pejorative. Little bit, because you're not supposed to, you know, it's a very, you're not supposed to get that far ahead of anybody without acknowledging that you're still part of the pack. Forgive me Danes, but they're nice people, so they'll likely go easy on me.
Starting point is 02:32:40 And I have Danish relatives. So, like, how do we take what's out and around us, address who we are and reconcile those things so that we're good with what we've got and know that we are good with what we've got? Yeah, yeah. I mean, I think this is a spot where culture plays a big role.
Starting point is 02:32:57 I think you're exactly right about Denmark. In fact, the Danes have this idea of Jante's law. I'm probably pronouncing this poorly, but JanteN-T-E's law, which is like, you aren't really supposed to be better than anybody else. Or like kind of showing off or like pushing yourself or thinking you're better, even kind of maybe striving specifically, not to be, you can strive to be better,
Starting point is 02:33:15 but to strive to be better than other people is kind of, it's like a no-no. It's sort of culturally frowned upon in this way that I think is completely the opposite in the US right now where like that's seen as an awesome thing. I think the problem is that these rags to riches stories, you've given cases of ones where there's a moment in SpaceX, they do this wonderful thing, you're like, yeah, they got there.
Starting point is 02:33:37 And Huk Tua is still doing her thing, but there was this moment of like, oh my gosh, you achieved this success. There's this idea that we think that there's an end destination for something. You know, I'm going to get $50 million, or I'm going to get married, or I'm going to get that promotion at work, right? For my students, I'm going to get into like a really elite college or something like that. We don't put our emphasis on the journey part. We put our emphasis on the destination part.
Starting point is 02:34:02 And we assume that the destination is going to come with a lot of happiness. This is a bias that researchers have called the arrival fallacy. I'll be happy when, you know, it's almost like the happily ever after. I'll be happily ever after if I get that promotion or happily ever after when I meet that person. And what we know from hedonic adaptation is that thing that's awesome in the moment when you arrive there quickly becomes the other thing. You mentioned briefly the gold medalists who have this moment where it's like,
Starting point is 02:34:26 they won the gold medal and that's awesome, but now everything else is downhill or I just gotta do it again, right? We arrive at the best possible place we could have fantasized and instantly it's like, I just have to start chasing the next carrot. So sometimes when we find ourselves, I think as Americans, you know, chasing after the thing,
Starting point is 02:34:43 I think it's important to remember that first of all, that chase is gonna involve lots of ups and downs. It's not gonna be a linear path. It's probably not gonna be overnight. Even the ones you mentioned, maybe except with the exception of rock to a girl is really extreme case, required some kind of work in ups and downs and these kinds of things, right?
Starting point is 02:34:59 We don't see those, but more the happiness that we're gonna get, it's better off if we're going not for the end result, that arrival and falling prey to the arrival fallacy. It's better if we can see some happiness in the journey. This has often been called this idea of sort of finding a journey mindset, which is sort of what can you take from the process of getting there, right? So you want to run your 5K, but like what can you do to try to enjoy the process of those runs that go along the way and noticing the kind of ups and downs and sort of paying attention to the journey.
Starting point is 02:35:30 It's one way to kind of break out of falling prey to this arrival fallacy. Requires a serious frame shift. Totally, and I think one that is not culturally accepted in the US. And I think this causes a lot of, it causes a happiness hit, not just because like sometimes we don't get there.
Starting point is 02:35:46 Sometimes there's reason, you know, if you set your height super high, you know, you want to be Roger Bannister or whatever, like not all of us are going to get there, whether that's a four-minute mile or success at work or $50 million or whatever it is. So sometimes if you set your sights too high, you just don't get there. And so that's a happiness hit. But a bigger happiness, and sometimes when you do get there, it's a happiness hit because you get there and there's a happiness for a moment, but then, you know, that hit doesn't keep coming. I think we also just lose out on something when we're not in that journey mindset because there's a lot of cool stuff
Starting point is 02:36:15 along the way if we can kind of pay attention. But yeah, I think it's a big cultural shift from the way Americans usually think, but if it's one that if we can achieve that, we'll start feeling a lot better. And it means even the failures in life are kind of good because you are enjoying yourself along the way. For my podcast, I did an episode about these sort of Olympic medals where I talked about the bronze lining effect and things. And I had Michelle Kwan, who, Olympic medalist, we all remember her, but mostly just one silver.
Starting point is 02:36:44 And I talked to her about what you know, what that felt like. And she said, it didn't matter to me. The things I loved about being in the Olympics wasn't the medal stand. It was when she first, she talked about putting her skates on and seeing the rings in the ice and recognizing as soon as I tie these laces, I'm going to get to skate over those and I fantasize. That's the journey mindset, right? You're not looking at the thing at the end.
Starting point is 02:37:03 You're paying enough attention to the stuff along the way, even some of the stuff that's the journey mindset, right? You're not looking at the thing at the end, you're paying enough attention to the stuff along the way, even some of the stuff that's a pain in the butt that you kind of get some joy on the ride. I certainly have learned to relish in the failures as well as the successes. And I think some of that also just comes with age. I've always wanted to say that. Yeah, it's true though, it kind of comes with age.
Starting point is 02:37:24 You're old enough now, Andrew, you can jump into it. Yeah, you accrue enough experiences, good and bad and neutral. And you kind of go like, the other day I was in like in this kind of weird state of mind, I was like, well, yeah, I've been here before. Like this shifts, like no worries. Like this shifts and then sure enough, it shifted.
Starting point is 02:37:41 You know, like I think the first time we find ourselves in a place or we find ourselves back in a place and we forget we've been there before for whatever reason, or we try and pretend we haven't been there before. It's like, and then you go through enough of those cycles like, okay, this is part of a larger trajectory. This is an amazing thing about the brain I never understood, I still don't,
Starting point is 02:38:00 which is that when we're feeling happy, we don't tend to think, gosh, this feeling is going to go away. Sometimes a little bit of that, but when we're feeling happy, we don't tend to think, gosh, this feeling is gonna go away. Sometimes a little bit of that, but when we're feeling lousy, it does seem to do something to our sense of time, our time perception that makes it seem, especially in the real lows and the real trenches,
Starting point is 02:38:16 that it's gonna go on forever. We can't imagine feeling differently. The this too shall pass is very hard to internalize when we're in those states. Totally, but it can make you feel a lot. If you can get that distance from your current state, and this is a kind of, you know, you had Ethan Cross on the show, he talks a lot, a lot.
Starting point is 02:38:31 If you can kind of get that distance of, well, how's this gonna feel in five years? How's this gonna feel in 10 years? You can sometimes feel a lot better. Interestingly, even when the happy stuff, if we can get some sense that like, this isn't going to last forever, that can sometimes boost the happiness.
Starting point is 02:38:47 Because we're kind of almost doing a negative visualization in the forward direction. So a scarce experience, if you're having it, it's useful to remember this is limited, this is temporary. I should enjoy this now while it's happening. The most extreme version of this, of course, is with our own lives, right? Contemplating our mortality. There's this idea of memento mori, which is a common phrase. Actually, my ring has memento mori on it, which is morbid, right? I'm going to die.
Starting point is 02:39:15 I'm not going to be here. But when you recognize that, you know, the old school folks thought, and I think it's true, like you realize, like, I can't take any of this stuff for granted. I have to pay attention now. This is not, you know, the kind of thing that's going to last forever. And so I think moments like that for positive experiences can feel like that. You know, if you're tasting a delicious glass of Pinot Noir sitting yesterday, and you know, while I'm here, you know, took a walk on Santa Monica Beach and was like, you know, my brain was like, oh, I have to, Andrew, I was like, no, no, I'm gonna like fly. I was like, no, no, no. I'm going to fly back to cold East Coast tomorrow.
Starting point is 02:39:47 I need to pay attention, right? So thinking that this is finite can actually help you. There's a very funny study on this with college students where they did this sort of funny framing technique where they brought senior college students into lab kind of halfway through the spring semester and told them, you either have this many hours left of your time, which maybe is a big number, it makes it like thousands of hours,
Starting point is 02:40:10 or you have only this many days left before you graduate, just a reminder. What they found was the one that got the days manipulation, where it felt kind of short, they wound up doing more things, like kind of getting in those things that they thought, oh, I'll get around to it eventually, and wound up kind of feeling happier. So recognizing that things are short sometimes
Starting point is 02:40:28 has a benefit, maybe both for negative emotions, like this too shall pass, but also the positive stuff, like this too shall pass, so I gotta enjoy it while it swats around. It's so interesting because it's kind of counterintuitive that realizing that something positive is also fleeting allows us to savor it more. Because from an uninformed perspective,
Starting point is 02:40:50 one could imagine, okay, so you're at a great meal with people you love and it's been, let's even say it's been a rough month before and you're like really in it and someone says, well, you know, like this too is gonna pass. And you're like, that sounds like kind of a downer, right? But then if it allows you to savor it more, that's key. So yeah, there does seem to be this inverse relationship
Starting point is 02:41:10 between sad states and happy states where when we are in sad states, we feel like it will go on forever and we'll do almost anything to get out of those unless they've completely collapsed us. In our happy states, we don't want to be reminded that it will pass. And this is why I think in part,
Starting point is 02:41:28 not the only reason why people will take mood altering drugs. I'm talking about this in the recreational sense, like to sort of forget everything else and forget that whatever they're experiencing is going to wear off. Yeah, yeah. And I think, you know, it's not nice to think that these good states are going to pass,
Starting point is 02:41:46 but I think it is helpful because it forces us to pay attention to them. I'm having this a little bit now where, you know, we're coming up on the holidays, that I'm you and I are having this conversation and, you know, I'm getting ready to do the holidays with the in-laws, you know, which there's lots of positives. So it's like, oh, God, I don't want to. But because my in-laws, my mom is getting up there, I'm kind of like, oh, god, I don't want to. But because my in-laws, my mom is getting up there, I'm kind of like, oh, recognizing that there's not infinite holidays left with these people
Starting point is 02:42:10 that I care about, that it's kind of more finite and maybe more finite than it's been, it's causing me to be more excited about it than I would have been. And so that's a morbid thought. My mental mori was meant to be a really bittersweet emotion, that we are finite, right? But it can kind of give you this appreciation.
Starting point is 02:42:28 It can cause you to savor in a special way. So sometimes the morbid is good, a little bit morbid. It's the contrast again. Yeah, yeah. It's the contrast. And maybe it's why people watch horror movies. I'm not into horror movies, but so that maybe you feel safer.
Starting point is 02:42:40 I don't know, that stuff always made me feel terrified if I was, you know, watched some of that late at night. High amygdala reaction, me too, me too. I hate horror movies, but it's worth noting that like, you know, a lot of people like them, you know, huge industry. And even if you don't like horror movies, you might like, you know, maybe a spicy food
Starting point is 02:42:56 that feels not even good. It feels awful in the moment or super hot bath or, you know, cold, like a really cold plunge, right? I like getting out of the cold plunge for exactly the reason we're talking about. Yeah, or even, you know, honestly for me, like I'm not a super cold plunge, right? I like getting out of the cold plunge for exactly the reason we're talking about. Yeah, or even, honestly for me, I'm not a super fan of exercise, but a really, really hard workout that feels miserable,
Starting point is 02:43:12 when you finally stop, it feels, I do a lot of yoga and my favorite thing is at the end when they're like, and now you can do Shavasana. The Shavasana is always good if you've worked the worst. You're just really like, ha. It's helpful to kind of have these moments, to like have this contrast. And so building the contrast in where you kind of
Starting point is 02:43:29 give yourself some negative emotion, you know, whether it's a kind of imagined negative emotion like negative visualization or a fictional one, a lot of our favorite fictional experiences are pretty terrible. Like a novel's really boring if the protagonist's like, there's nothing bad happens, it's just gonna go so long and things are just mildly positive.
Starting point is 02:43:46 No, we want them to go through some terrible stuff, even when we really associate with them and sort of see them as ourselves. And so, yeah, these like fictional worlds where we can play with negative emotions a little bit are super interesting psychologically, because like, why would we do that? But, you know, as you're saying, even when you get these like neural stimulation, we kind of want some of the negative stuff. So there's an interesting paper about what's the right ratio of positive to negative emotions. And it's not 100% positives for hedonic adaptation
Starting point is 02:44:15 and so on. But I think really the recipe for a rich life is varied for these contrast reasons we've been talking about. So what's the ideal ratio? They don't know. They didn't figure it out. It was like, boom, it's exactly. 60-40, positive, negative. You're obviously anticipating a number. And I think it's also worth remembering
Starting point is 02:44:34 that we're talking as though there are negative emotions and positive emotions. A lot of the most interesting emotions are more complex than that. You talked about this SpaceX kind of chopsticks moment. My guess is the emotion you're experiencing there is one that researchers like Dacher, Keltner and colleagues would call awe, right? This sense of, oh my gosh, that is amazing. There's something bigger than me that like is able to do this thing. And one of the reasons awe is such an interesting emotion is it's usually
Starting point is 02:45:01 destabilizing, right? They're like things that are better than I ever expected. You know, humankind is so masterful, space is so big, nature is so vast, right? It kind of feels a little destabilizing when you experience awe, but we also see it as positive. And so I think kind of if you're feeling a little bored in your emotional life, trying to find moments where you can get these emotions that are not so obviously positive or negative,
Starting point is 02:45:25 but are a little bit of both can be really inspiring. It's one of the reasons, you know, we talk a lot about sort of psychedelics and these sort of altered experiences. Those experiences tend to be thought of as being really consistent with moments of awe, but they, again, are not universally positive, but they kind of expand you and take you a little further.
Starting point is 02:45:43 I can attest that they're not universally positive. Sometimes they're terrifying, even in their clinical application. The thing I appreciated about the rocket landing was that indeed I feel all looking up at the stars at night or just thinking about how we're having this conversation in a room and well, and then expanding out to like we're a little object floating in the universe.
Starting point is 02:46:04 And that can be a bit overwhelming. What's I think incredible is that through the harnessing of engineering and physics, SpaceX was able to create something that was so well controlled at a scale that I'm normally accustomed to thinking about things. Sure, I've seen planes and we landed on the moon, et cetera. Some people will debate that, but we were on the moon. I wasn't, but somebody was.
Starting point is 02:46:27 To see control and harnessing of physics and engineering at a scale that is certainly not at the scale of the entire galaxies, but it's starting to approach outer space and back again, clearly. And in such a, I think it was the slowing of that enormously large object and the capture that felt so gratifying. I also think, and this can explain a fair amount of human technological evolution,
Starting point is 02:46:52 is that the human brain either delights in or at least marvels in creating action at a distance. I mean, think about what went into creating that amount of action in an object with that much mass at a distance. Right? And then you can layer through all the things where we're looking at our phones, on our screens. I mean, all that technology is relatively recent.
Starting point is 02:47:14 And to think that us, human beings, as opposed to macaque monkeys could do that. Like we are the primate species that is so far ahead in terms of technological development compared to every other species on the planet. The only other species of life that might be besting us and we don't know is I've heard this theory, it's rather entertaining,
Starting point is 02:47:37 which is that all these trillions of microorganisms that live in our gut microbiome, what if we're just vehicles for them to get around and pass to one another? And they're just, they have a sort of a consciousness that is all about just propagating and that we think that we're doing all this stuff for some evolution, but it's just to keep the microbiota.
Starting point is 02:47:54 I don't really believe that by the way. Finally we could get to space where we could really evolve the microbiota. And they just want more microbiota. So, you know, that we're being hijacked. I chuckle at that theory. I don't actually think that's what it is. Yeah, there's, we talked about too few studies of dog
Starting point is 02:48:08 and primate cognition, way too few studies of microbiota cognition, unfortunately. This is probably the right time to say that we are a storytelling species. This is what we're doing right now. We're creating story around these things that we can't quite explain. And during the course of today's conversation,
Starting point is 02:48:28 I realized that this thing that we call happiness has at least three levels or layers that we filter it through when we ask ourselves, are we happy? How do I be happier? This element of contrast with negative experiences seems to be a repeating theme. Momentum ori being being negative sort of dark cloud
Starting point is 02:48:48 from which we're supposed to see the light and act in the light. This exists in religious narratives, philosophical narratives and scientific reality. I could imagine three layers. The first is sensory experience. The reason to take a cold shower folks, in addition to the fact that it'll save you
Starting point is 02:49:07 on your heating bill, is that the warm shower that follows, in fact, that's how I do it, feels so good. 10 times better than it would if you had just gotten into the warm shower, I promise. Same thing about getting out of the cold plunge. You know, there's a lot of debate about these things, but this is just pure sensory experience and contrast of the sort that we're talking about today.
Starting point is 02:49:26 Hunger and then eating a delicious piece of food, or eating a not so delicious piece of food, but you're hungry and so it's that much more delicious. Okay, million examples we could spiral towards. So there's sensory experience, there's raw sensory perception and experience from which the contrast creates this thing that we like feel better, AKA happiness, sort of.
Starting point is 02:49:46 Then there's story, like, God, last year was a tough year. This year was better. There's also, and I've seen this before, like we were killing it for two years and then this year was kind of a meh year. This is not the case, by the way, but I'm very fortunate that the podcast has continued to grow and expand.
Starting point is 02:50:02 But for some people, they're not as happy with their whatever salary this year, because even though it's spectacular by somebody else's standards, by their standard, it's down from previous years. So there's the story that we create that it's not sensory experience, it's perception based on dopamine
Starting point is 02:50:19 and it's perception based on reward and punishment, et cetera. And then there's this third layer, which is meaning. Like you said, yeah, you know, spending time with in-laws, like, okay, every moment of it might not be as awesome as you might like, but there's meaning in spending time with people that are extended family, especially when elders and younger are in the same room. There's something really, it layers on story
Starting point is 02:50:43 to create this sort of other level that we call meaning. And so what I'm realizing is that these are three timescales. So we have the immediate timescale of happiness, we have the kind of intermediate one where we introduce a story, and then we have meaning, which is kind of like this whole picture. So it seems to me that we need to approach happiness from all three levels, that it's not enough to just be like a dog,
Starting point is 02:51:08 which are in the sensory experience, presumably, of happiness. If they tell stories, they don't tell them to us. And if they have meaning, I don't know. But they seem to like nail the first level. And they probably don't have the capacity to do the other two. So it's not like they're not doing it and kind of missing out. They kind of have brains that don't let them notice they're missing out.
Starting point is 02:51:29 But we unfortunately have brains that would feel like we were really missing out. If we just had the sensory experiences, you know, without the good stories, I think you're pointing to this idea that sort of being happy in your life and being happy with your life. The with your life part has the kind of medium time scale stories, but also they're really big ones. Right. You know, is my life, the with your life part has the kind of medium time scale stories, but also they're really big ones, right? You know, is my life, am I doing anything really meaningful with my life? Am I finding purpose and so on? The funny thing though is to get to that big time scale, to find
Starting point is 02:51:55 a sense of purpose and stuff like that. Sometimes it pays to do stuff at the local level at the medium and shorter term time scale. And one of the things researchers have found is that if you're engaging in activities at the short term time scales that kind of fit with your values or what these positive psychologists have often called your strengths, that can be a way to sort of achieve purpose. So what are strengths? So researchers do this thing where they want to look at like all the valuable things people can do out in the world, right? And so what are the things that you value?
Starting point is 02:52:23 And folks like Chris Peterson and colleagues have come up with this list of what they call different character strengths. And there are things that like, you know, you can actually, if you Google online character strengths, you'll get the big list. Often people talk about there being 24. But they're just universally good things like being brave, you know, citizenship, humor, like, you know, social intelligence, love of learning, right? You know, kind of empathy, fairness, right?
Starting point is 02:52:44 These kind of sets of values that we have. People differ in how much they value one or the other. So I could ask you, Andrew, what's better, like bravery or humor? Probably both pretty high for you, I would imagine. But like, about prudence versus love of learning, I have a guess. Yeah, I mean, if I had to pick between bravery and humor,
Starting point is 02:53:03 I think bravery is probably more important to me. I see, I'm more humor, yeah. I mean, I love humor, but if I had to pick between bravery and humor, I think bravery is probably more important to me. I see, I'm more humor, yeah. So I mean, I love humor, but if I had to pick, it's sort of like, you know, steak and coffee, I'm going steak. Yeah, okay. Well, the point is, there are individual differences in this, and there are formal tests you can do online
Starting point is 02:53:17 if you Google the VIA Character Strengths Test. You'll see these 24 and you can do one of these very systematic, you know, kind of tests to do it. But really just trying to think about like, what are the values that you value? You'll see these 24 and you can do one of these very systematic, you know, kind of tests to do it. But really just trying to think about like, what are the values that you value? And the ones that come to mind as being particularly about you, the ones that you resonate with are what somebody like Chris Peterson would call your signature strengths. They're the ones that when you execute them, you kind of feel like things are meaningful
Starting point is 02:53:39 and purposeful and so on. And so the idea is that one recipe for a purposeful life at the local level is trying to engage in behaviors that allow you to use more of these values or strengths. And one of my favorite pieces of research that looks at both the power of this and how, even though if it seems like that those are hard things to bring in, like you should bring them in more, is some work by this woman, Amy Resninski, who's a professor at the University of Pennsylvania. And she does these studies on what she calls job crafting,
Starting point is 02:54:10 which is a practice where you take your normal job description, whatever your job is, and figure out ways that you can infuse your signature strengths into them. So as a podcaster, if your signature sank with bravery, you could bring in guests that made me feel a little bit intimidating to you, probably like me, I imagine. Or like you could take on topics that are a little bit harder, that kind of push you
Starting point is 02:54:30 a little bit. If your signature strength was humor, you'd add more company or make more jokes. If it was love of learning, you'd pick topics that you didn't know anything about, but you kind of dive in. You take whatever your normal job description is and you find a way to build in your strengths. And the reason I love Amy's work so much is that she studies signature strengths, not in academics like us who have very flexible jobs or podcasters. She studies signature strengths in hospital janitorial staff workers who are, you know,
Starting point is 02:54:57 these are people who are cleaning the linen in a hospital room or mopping the floors and stuff. Not a job where you think there's lots of flexibility or you could build in things like you know humor and love of learning and the stuff. But she finds interestingly that like around a quarter to a third of these janitorial staff workers say that their job is a calling, they love it, they get a lot of purpose from it, and they're the ones that are naturally building in their signature strengths. And she tells in her story in her work she tells these lovely stories. It's the story of a janitorial staff worker who worked in a chemotherapy ward.
Starting point is 02:55:28 And if you've been unlucky enough to have cancer and had to have chemotherapy or know someone who did, you know that people tend to get really sick because the medicine makes people really nauseous. So a big part of this guy's job was like cleaning up vomit, basically. But he said, my job isn't to clean up vomit. My strengths are humor and social intelligence. And what I do is I make a joke. This is somebody's having
Starting point is 02:55:48 a really crappy day and I'm going to do something that's going to make them laugh. And if I do that, then I won. It's not my paycheck. And I guess he had a standard joke, which was like, oh my God, let's play a big pile of vomit over time. Like for me, and like, you know, you're laughing listeners, probably laughing. He's like, that's my job. I talked to another worker who worked in a coma ward. So this individual couldn't talk to patients because they're in comas, but her strength was creativity. And so every day she like moved the artwork and the plants around, you know, just kind of created some changes. And she thought maybe that would pop people out of their coma. I don't know if
Starting point is 02:56:19 that's medically plausible, probably not, but it doesn't matter to her. She felt like she was executing her creativity. And so the moral of this job crafting work is no matter what your job is, there's probably some room to build in some more purpose. If you take some time to think about like, what are the strengths? What are the things that get you going? If you need a tip, you can kind of Google these things. But then how could I infuse that into my normal job description? And there's probably a lot more flexibility than you think. You don't need to quit your job and become a podcaster to like get this flexibility. Probably whatever you do, there's some window where you can build that in.
Starting point is 02:56:54 That's awesome. Those are awesome stories. I also was just thinking about the janitor cleaning up the vomit, like to like restore some dignity to these people that clearly know they're making a mess and like, you know, humor being the ultimate bridge and darn it, why'd you make me have to choose between humor and the other thing? But because humor is so awesome. Sorry, now you're really,
Starting point is 02:57:15 humor is pretty good. Now I'm rethinking my answer. It's very brave to clean up vomit as well, I think, right? Yeah, and to bring humor to a place where, you know, some people might presume humor is not allowed, goodness. The signature strengths in the list of, you said 24 of them, where can people learn more about these signature strengths? I think this would be a really powerful exercise
Starting point is 02:57:36 and we can always find the link and put it in the show note captions, but is there like a place that people can find this stuff? The values in action is the viacharacterstrengths.org. So I can share the link and you can stick it in your show notes. But yeah, people can go on there for free and do one of these kind of formal psychometric tests
Starting point is 02:57:53 where you measure your strengths, see what they are. And it's a fun website too, because you get to kind of, they give you some suggestions. Because some of these values are like prudence is one. I'm just like, how do I exercise prudence? And they'll have, these are different thingsudence is one of them. It's like, how do I exercise prudence? And they'll have, you know, these are different things. They also make the suggestion, this is a homework assignment I give in my happiness class,
Starting point is 02:58:10 of suggesting you do this with a good friend or a romantic partner, have each of you do this and find strengths that you share together. And then you can go on what researchers call a strength state, where, you know, if you both have bravery, then that means you guys should do the, I don't know, the obstacle course or do some really scary hike. If you both have humor, now you go to a comedy show.
Starting point is 02:58:29 If you both love learning, now you go to a museum or something. So you find the thing that's like your convergent strengths, and you do something that exercises them. So that means you can use your strengths to get purpose, not just in your work, but in your leisure too. And I think this is another spot where we get stuff wrong. I think a lot of us have work that tends to use our strengths. We tend to gravitate towards careers, many of us, where we can use our strengths. A lot of folks aren't that lucky. But in our leisure time,
Starting point is 02:58:52 we don't often do that so much, right? Often our leisure time is like plop down, you know, watch Netflix for a lot of folks. Like, if you think about how you can build your strengths into your leisure time, it gets even more exciting. So you're talking about working with your hands and doing all this stuff like, you know, build the bravery and the humor into that somehow. Now you get your leisure time doing double duty for giving you a sense of purpose and meaning too. I love doing stuff with my hands.
Starting point is 02:59:15 And I also love doing things that are useful to other people. And years ago, I used to go set up fish tanks for people at their homes. And I don't know why, but I just kept setting up all these fish tanks for all these people and I delighted in it. And it makes me realize that I think for everybody, certainly not just me,
Starting point is 02:59:31 that we get tremendous pleasure from being useful to others in ways that really resonate with kind of who we feel we are and that these strengths, I think that's kind of the ultimate situation really. And if we're getting paid for it also, great, but you're saying work it into your recreational time as well. Yeah, and I'm glad you brought up this idea
Starting point is 02:59:50 of doing for others because we haven't talked about that, but this is another behavioral hack that's huge for happiness. And I think one that we get wrong as a culture in the US, but kind of broadly, there's all this talk about self care or treat yourself. If you look at any kind of article about happiness, maybe not so evidence-based, talking about self, self, self, self. If you look at any kind of article about happiness, maybe not so evidence-based,
Starting point is 03:00:05 talk about self, self, self, self. If you look at happy people though, happy people don't spend a lot of time on themselves. They tend to be very other-oriented. So controlled for income, happier people donate more money to charity than not-so-happy people. Controlled for the amount of free time people have, happy people tend to volunteer for others.
Starting point is 03:00:22 Broadly construed, whether it's helping formally or kind of donating time, they tend to help more than not-so-happy people. That again, correlation, it could be doing nice stuff for others helps you become happy. It could be that if you're happy, you do nice stuff for others, and for sure that link is true. There is this thing called the feel-good-do-good effect. But lots of experiments have forced people to do nice stuff for others and found that it winds up making them happier.
Starting point is 03:00:45 One study by Laura Ackman and colleagues did this study where they walk up to people on the street and hand them 20 bucks. So it's an awesome study to be in if you're some undergrad walking around campus like, oh cool, 20 bucks. But then you'll be told how to spend it. You either have to spend the 20 bucks to treat yourself, do something nice for yourself,
Starting point is 03:00:59 or spend the 20 bucks on someone else, do something nice for other people. And people at the end of the day, even kind of at later time skills, report being happier when they spend the exact same amount of money on someone else versus themselves. And I think this has a big message because sometimes, I don't know if you're in a, I don't know if you, like you, but if you're having a bad day, it's like, I'm going to treat myself for something.
Starting point is 03:01:18 I might buy something or spend some money on myself, buy myself a kind of cool experience. But if you gifted that experience to your brother or your good friend, your coworker, your spouse, it might actually make you happier than having that experience yourself, which is really counterintuitive, but it's what the data show. I've discovered this in recent years.
Starting point is 03:01:35 I love, love, love giving gifts. It just, it's the best feeling. It's the best feeling. Here's another hack you can do to help, to kind of help others oddly is to ask for help, which is something we forget is quite powerful. Think about the last time somebody asked you for advice, advice that you could give. Probably felt pretty good.
Starting point is 03:01:55 It probably made you feel a little competent and whatever. Probably liked helping that person. You get the happiness boost from helping that person. We forget that asking other people for help, especially when we know they can kind of do it, can be a way to sort of give them a little bit of gift and make them happy. This is one that can be hard for me because I like to think about my competence all the time
Starting point is 03:02:14 and I don't want to be a burden on people. I don't like being vulnerable. Yes. But it turns out, especially if you're a particularly self-sufficient person, when you ask people for help, it can be really useful. So that's another one, because I know some folks listening right now
Starting point is 03:02:28 might not have the financial means to be donating money or the time affluence and wherewithal to be doing gifts and these things, but remember that asking for help can be a gift to someone else and it's a little social connection too. That's awesome. I will also say your suggestion that people
Starting point is 03:02:45 fill out the Sign strengths site and then use that as a first date incentive I look forward to the day when a comment comes through on YouTube that people were married as a consequence of a first date Yeah, Tinder is going out of business if we start doing these strength dates. So yeah every once in a while someone will contact me and say that they watched the fertility episode and did that they watched the fertility episode and did a male and female fertility episode and that they now have a child on the way. I don't ask questions about when the child was conceived or what the relationship to the fertility episode was. I'm assuming it was the information in the fertility episode. But I'm like, whoa,
Starting point is 03:03:23 that's wild. So I bet you that at some point in the future, I'm creating a little bit of a time capsule here. You'll get contacted or something will legitimately fall into the comments about people deciding to spend their life together as a consequence of having done the signature strength first date. You heard it here first, Dr. Laurie Santos. And in all seriousness, Laurie, Dr. Santos,
Starting point is 03:03:47 I just want to say thank you so much for doing the work you do. It's awesome, awesome work. I mean, what's more important than our emotional state and to strive to be happy, but to understand happiness so that we're not pursuing something that either doesn't exist or that is an illusion that's been created for us. Like really, I think one of the amazing things
Starting point is 03:04:10 about what you do is you realistically frame happiness as attainable, but you frame it in the science of how to actually get it and what it means. And as people could probably detect, I love, love, love that you've studied this thing that we call happiness and other aspects of emotion and social cognition in the context of not just humans, but our non-human friends, cats and dogs,
Starting point is 03:04:35 and use that knowledge, like building up from basic understanding of how neural circuits and psychology work to a place that humans can really act on. And you've given us a tremendous number of actionable tools today. I mean, too many to list off here all at once. We'll put them in the timestamps as tools
Starting point is 03:04:51 so that people can get right to them and review them. But the social connection piece, obviously, the understanding of the contrast with difficult things to arrive at better states and different timescales and doing for others and just so much. There's too much here for me to list off without adding another 30 minutes
Starting point is 03:05:07 to this podcast and no one wants to hear me speak anymore. So I'm just going to say thank you for the research that you have done and continue to do. Thank you for doing your podcast. I'm going to start listening to your podcast. I love these issues and I think they're super, super timely and important for everybody. And thanks for taking time out of your schedule to come here and educate us today.
Starting point is 03:05:29 Thanks so much. It was a blast. Let's do it again. Definitely. Thank you for joining me for today's discussion with Dr. Laurie Santos. To learn more about her laboratory's work, her teachings, and to find a link to her excellent podcast, please see the show note captions. If you're learning from and or enjoying this podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube channel. That's a terrific zero cost way to support us.
Starting point is 03:05:50 Please also click follow for the podcast on both Spotify and Apple. And on both Spotify and Apple, you can leave us up to a five-star review. Please also check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning and throughout today's episode. That's the best way to support this podcast. If you have questions for me or comments about the podcast
Starting point is 03:06:06 or topics or guests that you'd like me to consider for the Huberman Lab podcast, please put those in the comment section on YouTube. I do read all the comments. And if you're not already following me on social media, I am Huberman Lab on all social media platforms. So that's Instagram, X, formerly known as Twitter, Facebook, threads, and LinkedIn.
Starting point is 03:06:24 And on all those platforms, I discuss science and science-related tools, some of which overlaps with the content of the Huberman Lab podcast, but much of which is distinct from the content on the Huberman Lab podcast. Again, that's Huberman Lab on all social media platforms. For those of you that haven't heard,
Starting point is 03:06:39 I have a new book coming out. It's my very first book. It's entitled, Protocols, An Operating Manual for the Human Body. This is a book that I've been working on for more than five years, and that's based on more than 30 years of research and experience.
Starting point is 03:06:51 And it covers protocols for everything from sleep, to exercise, to stress control, protocols related to focus and motivation. And of course, I provide the scientific substantiation for the protocols that are included. The book is now available by presale at protocolsbook.com. There you can find links to various vendors. You can pick the one that you like best.
Starting point is 03:07:13 Again, the book is called Protocols, an operating manual for the human body. And if you haven't already subscribed to our neural network newsletter, the neural network newsletter is a zero cost monthly newsletter that includes everything from podcast summaries to what we call protocols in the form of brief one to three page PDFs that cover things like how to optimize your sleep, how to regulate
Starting point is 03:07:33 your dopamine. We also have protocols related to deliberate cold exposure, get a lot of questions about that, deliberate heat exposure and on and on again, all available at completely zero cost. You simply go to hubermanlab.com, go to the menu tab in the top right corner, scroll down to newsletter and enter your email. And I should mention that we do not share your email with anybody. Thank you once again for joining me for today's discussion
Starting point is 03:07:55 with Dr. Laurie Santos. And last but certainly not least, thank you for your interest in science.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.