Lex Fridman Podcast - #140 – Lisa Feldman Barrett: Love, Evolution, and the Human Brain
Episode Date: November 20, 2020Lisa Feldman Barrett is a neuroscientist, psychologist, and author. Please support this podcast by checking out our sponsors: - Athletic Greens: https://athleticgreens.com/lex and use code LEX to get ...1 month of fish oil - Eight Sleep: https://www.eightsleep.com/lex and use code LEX to get $200 off - MasterClass: https://masterclass.com/lex to get 15% off annual sub - BetterHelp: https://betterhelp.com/lex to get 10% off EPISODE LINKS: Seven and a Half Lessons About the Brain (book): https://amzn.to/2Sp5ar9 How Emotions Are Made (book): https://amzn.to/2GwAFg6 Lisa's Twitter: https://twitter.com/LFeldmanBarrett Lisa's Website: https://lisafeldmanbarrett.com/ PODCAST INFO: Podcast website: https://lexfridman.com/podcast Apple Podcasts: https://apple.co/2lwqZIr Spotify: https://spoti.fi/2nEwCF8 RSS: https://lexfridman.com/feed/podcast/ YouTube Full Episodes: https://youtube.com/lexfridman YouTube Clips: https://youtube.com/lexclips SUPPORT & CONNECT: - Check out the sponsors above, it's the best way to support this podcast - Support on Patreon: https://www.patreon.com/lexfridman - Twitter: https://twitter.com/lexfridman - Instagram: https://www.instagram.com/lexfridman - LinkedIn: https://www.linkedin.com/in/lexfridman - Facebook: https://www.facebook.com/LexFridmanPage - Medium: https://medium.com/@lexfridman OUTLINE: Here's the timestamps for the episode. On some podcast players you should be able to click the timestamp to jump to that time. (00:00) - Introduction (07:27) - Falling in love (25:10) - Love at first sight (39:49) - Romantic (43:48) - Writing process (54:32) - Evolution of the human brain (1:08:41) - Nature of evil (1:17:23) - Love is an evolutionary advantage (1:21:59) - Variation in species (1:27:41) - Does evolution have a direction? (1:45:19) - Love with an inanimate object (1:49:37) - Just be yourself is confusing advice (1:59:49) - Consciousness (2:06:26) - Book recommendations
Transcript
Discussion (0)
The following is a conversation with Lisa Feldman Barrett, her second time on the podcast.
She is a neuroscientist at Northeastern University and one of my favorite people.
Her new book, called Seven and a Half Lessons About the Brain, is out now as of a couple of days ago,
so you should definitely support Lisa by buying it and sharing with friends if you like it.
It's a great short intro to the human brain.
Quick mention of each sponsor, followed by some thoughts related to the episode. I thought
it greens the all in one drink that I start every day with to cover all my nutritional
bases, ate sleep, a mattress that cools itself and gives me yet another reason to enjoy
sleep, masterclass, online courses that I enjoy from some of
the most amazing people in history and better help, online therapy with a licensed professional.
Please check out these sponsors in the description to get a discount and to support this podcast.
As a side note, let me say that Lisa, just like Manolis Kellis, is a local brilliant
mind and friend and someone I can see talking
to many more times.
Sometimes it's fun to talk to a scientist not just about their field of expertise but
also about random topics, even silly ones, from love to music to philosophy.
Ultimately it's about having fun, something I know nothing about.
This conversation is certainly that.
You may not always work, but it's worth a shot.
I think it's valuable to alternate
along all kinds of dimensions,
like between deeper technical discussions
and more fun random discussion,
from liberal thinker to conservative thinker,
from musician to athlete, from CEO to junior engineer, from friend to stranger.
Variety makes life and conversation more interesting.
Let's see where this little podcast journey goes.
If you enjoy this thing, subscribe on YouTube, review it with 5,000 on a podcast, follow on Spotify,
support it on Patreon, or connect with me on Twitter, Alex Friedman.
As usual, I'll do a few minutes of ads now and no ads in the middle.
I try to make these interesting, but I give you time stamps, so if you skip, please
still check out the sponsors by clicking the links in the description.
It's the best way to support this podcast.
This show is sponsored by Athletic Greens, the only one daily drink to support better
health and peak performance.
They replaced the multivitamin for me and went far beyond that with 75 vitamins and minerals.
I do intermittent fasting of 16 to 24 hours every day and always break my fast with Athletic
Greens.
I honestly can't say enough good things about these guys.
It helps me not worry whether I'm getting all the nutrients I need. One of the many reasons I'm a fan is that they keep iterating on the formula. As
an engineer, I love continuous improvement. Life is not about reaching perfection, it's
about constantly striving for it and making sure each iteration is a positive delta.
The other thing I've taken for a long time outside of athletic greens is fish oil.
So I'm especially excited now that they're selling fish oil and are offering listeners
of this podcast, free month supply of wild caught omega-3 fish oil.
When you go to Athletic Greens.com slash Lex to claim this special offer.
So you can be lazy like me and get all your nutrients from the same company.
Click the aflaticgreens.com slashlacks link in the description to get the fish oil and
the only one supplement I rely on for the nutritional foundation of my physical and mental
performance.
This show is also sponsored by 8Sleep and it's pod pro mattress.
It controls temperature with an app,
it's packed with sensors, it can cool down to as low as 55 degrees on each side of the
bed separately. It's been a game changer for me. I just enjoy sleep and power naps way
more now. I feel like I fall asleep faster and get more restful sleep. Combination of cool
bed and warm blanket is amazing.
Now, if you love your current mattress, but are still looking for temperature control,
8 sleeps new pod pro cover as dynamic cooling and heating capabilities onto your current
mattress.
You can cool down to 55 degrees, or like on a very cold day in Boston like today, heat up to 110 degrees and do so on each side
of the bed separately. Also, you can track a bunch of metrics like heart rate variability,
but honestly cooling alone is worth the money. Go to atsleep.com slash Lex. And when you buy
stuff there during the holidays, you'll get a special savings as listeners of this podcast.
That's a sleep.com slash Lex.
This show is also sponsored by Masterclass,
a $180 a year for an all-access pass
to watch courses from literally the best people in the world
on a bunch of different topics.
Let me list some that I really enjoyed watching. Chris Hadfield on Space Exploration, Neil deGrasse Tyson on Scientific Thinking
and Communication, Will Wright, creator of SimCity and Sims on Game Design, Carlos Santana on
guitar, Derek Asparov on chess, Daniel Nagrano on poker'm Poker, Neil Gaiman, I'm Storytelling, Martin Scorsese, I'm Filmmaking,
he's one of my favorite directors, Tony Hawk on skateboarding, and Jane Goodall on
conservation, and many more.
By the way, you can watch it on basically any device, sign up at masterclass.com slash
Lex, to get 15% off the first year of an annual subscription that's masterclass.com slash
LX.
This show is sponsored by BetterHelp spelled H-E-L-P-H-H-P.
They figure out what you need and match you with a licensed professional therapist in
other 48 hours.
I chat with a person on there and enjoy it.
Of course, I also have been talking to Mr.
David Goggins over the past few months, who is definitely not a licensed professional
therapist, but he does help me meet his and my demons and become comfortable to exist
in their presence. Everyone is different, but for me, I think suffering is a central
folk creation, but you can suffer beautifully in a way that
doesn't destroy you.
Therapy can help in whatever form that therapy takes, and so better help is an option worth
trying.
They're easy, private, affordable, available, worldwide.
You can communicate by text, anytime, and schedule weekly audio and video sessions.
I know you didn't ask me, but my two favorite
psychiatrists are Zigmund Freud and Carl Jung. Since in my youth I wanted to be a
psychiatrist their work was important in my intellectual development.
Anyway, check out BetterHelp.com slash Lex. That's BetterHelp.com slash Lex.
And now here's my conversation with Lisa Feldman Barrett. Based on the comments in our previous conversation, I think a lot of people will be very disappointed,
I should say, to learn that you are, in fact, married, as they say, all the good ones
are taken.
Okay.
So, I'm a fan of your husband as well, Dan.
He's a programmer and musician, so a man after my own heart.
Can I ask a ridiculously over-manticized question of when did you first fall in love with Dan?
It's actually, it's a really romantic story, I think.
So I was divorced by the time I was 26, 27, 26, I guess. And I was in my first academic
job, which was Penn State University, which is in the middle of Pennsylvania, surrounded by mountains.
So you have, it's four hours to get anywhere to get to Philadelphia, New York, Washington. I mean, you're basically stuck, you know.
And I was very fortunate to have a lot of other assistant professors who were
hired at the same time as I was. So there were a lot of us, we were all friends,
which was really fun. But I was single and I didn't want a data student.
And there were no, and I wasn't going to date somebody in my department.
That's just a recipe for disaster.
Yeah.
So even at 20, whatever you were, you were already wise enough to know that.
Yeah, a little bit maybe.
Yeah, I wouldn't call me wise at that age.
But anyways, not sure that I would say that I'm wise now.
But and so after, you know, I was spending probably 16 hours a day in the lab because
it was my first year and as an assistant professor and there's a lot to do and I was also
bitching and moaning to my friends that I you know I hadn't had sex and I don't know how
many you know months and it was I was I was starting to become unhappy with my life.
I think at a certain point they just got tired of listening to me bitch and moan and said,
just do something about it then. Do you know if you're unhappy.
The first thing I did was I made friends with a sushi chef in town. This is a
state college, Pennsylvania,
in the early 90s, was there was like a pizza shop,
and a sub shop, and actually a very good bagel shop.
And one good coffee shop, and maybe one nice restaurant.
I mean, there was really, but there was a second son
of a Japanese sushi chef who was not going to inherit
the restaurant, and so he moved to Pennsylvania
and was giving sushi lessons.
So I met this guy, the sushi chef,
and we decided to throw a sushi party at the coffee shop.
So we basically, it was the goal was to invite
every eligible bachelor really within like 20 mile radius.
We had a totally fun time.
I wore an awesome crushed velvet burgundy dress. It was beautiful dress.
I made a lot of friend, new friends, but it didn't not meet anybody. So then I thought,
okay, well, maybe I'll try the Personals ads, which I had never used before in my life.
I first tried the paper Personals ads.
Like a newspaper?
Like in the newspaper.
That didn't work.
And then a friend of mine said, oh, you know, there's this thing called net news.
So we're going, this is like 1992, maybe.
So there was this anonymous, you could do it anonymously.
So you would read, you could post or you could read ads and then respond to an address,
which was anonymous and that was yoked to somebody's real address.
And there was always a lag because it was this like a bulletin board sort of thing.
So at first I read them over and I decided to respond to one or two and, you know,
it was interesting. Sorry, this is not on the internet. Yeah, this is totally on the internet.
But it takes there's a delay of a couple days or whatever. Yeah, right. It's 1992. There's no web,
web pictures. There's no pictures. The web doesn't exist. It's all done in ASCII formats.
And, you know, but the ratio of men to women was like 10 to 1. I mean, there were many
more men because it was basically academics and the government. That was it. That was
no, I mean, I think AOL maybe was just starting to become popular.
And so the first person I met told me that he was a scientist at work for NASA.
And yeah, anyways, it turned out that he didn't actually.
This is how they brag.
It's like you elevate your, as opposed to saying you're taller than you are,
you say like your position is higher.
Yeah, and I actually, I would have been fine dating somebody who was a scientist.
It's just that they have, it's just that whoever I date has to just accept that I am and that I
I was pretty ambitious and was trying to make my career.
And, you know, that's not, that's not it.
I think it's maybe more common now for men to maybe accept that in their female partners,
but at that time, not so common.
It could be intimidating, I guess.
Yes, I, that has been said.
And so, and so then the next one I actually corresponded with and we actually got to the point of talking on the phone and we had this really kind of funny conversation where, you know, we're chatting and he said
He he introduces the idea that you know, he's really looking for a dominant woman and I'm thinking I'm a psychologist by training
So I'm thinking oh he means sex roles like I'm like no, I'm very assertive and I'm glad you by training. So I'm thinking, oh, he means sex roles. Like, I'm like, no, I'm very assertive. And I'm glad you think that, you know,
okay, anyways, long story short, that's not really what he meant.
Okay, got it. Yeah. So I just, you know, that will just show you my level of
naive and tail. Like I was like, I didn't completely understand. I was like,
well, yeah, you know, no. At one point he asked me how I felt about him wearing my lingerie and I was like,
I don't even share my lingerie with my sister. Like, I don't share my lingerie with anybody, you know,
no. The third one I interacted with was a banker who lived in Singapore.
And um,
a banker who lived in Singapore. And that conversation didn't last very long
because he made an analogy between me
and a character in the fountain head,
the woman who's raped in the fountain head.
And I was like, okay, that's not.
That's not a good, that's not a good,
no, that's not a good one.
Not that part, not that scene.
Not that scene.
So then I, so then I was like, okay, you know what,
I'm gonna post my own ad.
And so I did, I posted, well, first I wrote my ad
and then of course I checked it with my friends
who were all also assistant professors
that are like my little Greek chorus.
And then I posted it and I got something
like I don't know 80 something responses in 24 hours. I mean it was. Do you remember the pitch?
Like how you I guess condensed yourself. I don't remember it exactly although Dan
has it but actually for our 20th wedding, he took our exchanges and he printed them
off and put them in a leather bound book for us to read, which was really sweet.
Yeah, I think I was just really direct.
Like, I'm almost 30, I'm a scientist, I'm not looking just, you know, I'm looking for
something serious and, yeah, but the thing is, I forgot to say where my location was
and my age, which I forgot.
I got lots of, I mean, I'll say, so I printed off all of the responses and I had all my friends
over and we were, you know, had a big, I made a big pot of gumbo and we drank through
several bottles of wine, reading these responses. And I would say for the most part, they were really sweet.
Like earnest and genuine, as much as you could tell
that somebody's being genuine, it seemed,
you know, there were a couple of really funky ones,
like, you know, this one couple who told me
that I was their soulmate, the two of them,
then they were looking for, you know, a third person.
And I was like, mm, okay.
But mostly super, seemed like super genuine people.
And so I chose five men to start corresponding with.
And I was corresponding with them.
And then about a week later, I get this other email.
And okay, and then I post something the next day
that said, okay, thank you so much.
And I'm gonna answer every person back. But then after that, I said, okay, thank you so much. And I'm going to answer it every person back.
But then after that, I said, okay, and I'm not going to answer anymore.
You know, because they were still coming in, I couldn't, you know, I have a job and, you
know, a house to take care of and stuff.
So.
And then about a week later, I get this other email.
And he says, you know, he just describes himself, like, I'm this, I'm this, I'm this, I'm a chef, I'm a scientist, I'm a this, I'm a this.
And so I emailed him back and I said, you know, you see him interesting, you can write me at my actual address if you want. Here's my address. I'm not really responding.
I'm not really responding to other people anymore, but you see him interesting, you know, you can write to me if you want. And then he wrote to me, and I,
then I wrote him back, and it was a non-descript kind of email,
and I wrote him back, and I said,
thanks for responding, you know, I'm really busy right now.
I was in the middle of writing my first slate
of grant application, so I was really consumed,
and I said, I'll get back to you in a couple of days.
And so I did, I waited a couple of days, until my grants were, you know, safe, grant application safely out of the door. And then I emailed
them back and then he emailed me and then really across two days we sent a hundred emails.
And text only was there pictures and that's just. Text only, text only. And then, so this was like, if there was day and a Friday.
And then Friday, he said, let's talk on the weekend
on the phone, and I said, okay.
And he wanted to talk Sunday night,
and I had a date Sunday night.
So I said, okay, sure, we can talk Sunday night.
And then I was like, well, I don't really want to cancel my date.
So I'm just going to call them on Saturday.
So I just called, I call them on Saturday and a woman answered.
Oh, wow. That's not cool. Not cool.
And so she says, you know, hello, and I say, oh, you know,
it's down there. And she said, sure, can I ask who's calling?
And I said, it's Leesa.
And she went, oh my God, oh my God, I'm just a friend.
I'm just a friend.
I just wanted to tell you I'm just a friend.
And I was like, this is adorable, right?
She doesn't, and then he gets on the phone, not high.
Nice to meet the first thing he says to me,
she's just a friend.
So I was just so charmed,
really, by the whole thing. So it was, it was Yom Kapoor. It was the Jewish day of atonement
that was ending and they were baking cookies and going to a break fast. So people, you
know, as you know, fast all day and then they got a party and they break fast. So I thought, okay, I'll just cancel my date.
So I did, and I stayed home and we talked for eight hours,
and then the next night for six hours.
And it basically it just went on like that
and then by the end of the week,
he flew to stay college.
And we had gone through this whole thing
where I'd said, we're gonna take it slow,
we're gonna get to know each other.
And then really by, I think we talked like two or three times,
these like really long conversations
and then he said, I'm just gonna fly there.
And then so, of course there's,
I don't even know that there were facts machines
at that point, maybe there were,
but I don't even know that there were facts machines at that point. Maybe there were, but I don't think so.
Anyway, we decide we'll exchange pictures.
I take my photograph and I give it to my secretary and I say to my secretary.
Facts this.
I say that, send this priority mail.
And he goes, okay, I'll send a priority mail.
I'm going to make this priority mail.
He's like, I know a priority male. And he goes, okay, I'll send a priority male. I'm gonna make this priority male. He's like, I know a priority male.
Okay.
And then, so I get Dan's photograph in the male.
And, you know, it's him in a, in shorts.
And you can see that he's probably somewhere
like the Bahamas or something like that.
And it's like cropped.
So clearly what he's done is he's taken a photograph
where, you know, he's in it with someone else who turned
out to be his ex-wife.
So I'm thinking, well, this is awesome.
I've hit the jackpot.
He's very appealing to me, very attractive.
And then my photograph doesn't show up, and it doesn't show up.
So one day and then two days, and you know he's like you know you're
I said well I I asked my secretary to send a priority I mean I don't know you know
what he did and and he's like I said I'm like well you don't have to you know you don't have to come
and he's like no no no I'm gonna I'm gonna you know we've had like five dates the equivalent of five
dates practically.
And then, so he's supposed to fly on a Thursday or Friday, I can't remember.
And I get a call like maybe an hour before his flight's supposed to leave.
And he says, hi.
And I say, and it's just something in his voice, right?
And I say, because at this point, I think I've talked to him like for 25 hours.
I don't know.
And he says, hi.
And I'm like, you got the picture. And he's like, yeah. And I'm like, you don't know. And he says, hi, and I'm like, you got the picture. And he's like,
yeah, and I'm like, you don't like it. And he's like, well, I'm sure it's not. I'm sure it's
your, I'm sure it's just not a good, you know, it's not just probably not your best.
Oh, no, you know, you don't, you don't have to come. And he's like, no, no, no, I'm coming. And I'm like, no, you don't have to come. And he's like, no, no, no, I'm coming.
And I'm like, no, you don't have to come.
And he's like, no, no, I really want to,
I'm getting on the plane.
I'm like, you don't have to get on the plane.
He's like, no, I'm getting on the plane.
And so I go down to my, I go,
I mean, my office is happening, right?
So I go downstairs to my, one of my closest friends,
who's still actually one of my closest friends,
who is one of my of my closest friends, who is one of my
colleagues. And Kevin and I say, Kevin, and I go to Kevin, I go, Kevin, Kevin, Kevin,
he doesn't like the photograph. And Kevin's like, well, which photograph did you send?
And I'm like, well, you know the one where we're shooting pool and he's like, you sent
that photograph? That's a horrible photograph. I'm like, yeah, but it's the only one that I
had that was like where my hair was kind of similar to what it is now and he's like Lisa
Like do I have to check everything for you?
You should not have sent that you know, but still he flew over. So he flew where from by the way?
He was in he was in graduate school at Amherst. Yeah, at UM UMass Amherst. So he flew and I picked him up and at the airport
and he was happy.
So whatever the concern was was gone.
And I was dressed, you know, carefully, carefully dressed.
I was really, really nervous.
Cause I am not, I don't really believe in fate.
And I don't really think there's only one person
that you can be with.
But I think, some people are curvy.
They're kind of complicated.
And so the number of people who fit them
is maybe less than.
I like it mathematically speaking
Yeah, and so when I was going to pick him up at the airport. I was thinking well
This could I could be going to pick up the the person I'm gonna marry or
Not I mean like I really but I really you know like our conversations were just very authentic and very moving and
and we really connected and and I really felt like he understood me actually
um in in a way that a lot of people don't and um and and what was really nice was at the time
And what was really nice was at the time, you know, the airport was this tiny little airport out in a cornfield, basically.
And so, driving back to the town, we were in the car for 15 minutes completely in the
dark as I was driving.
And so, it was very similar to we had just spent, you know, 20- hours on the telephone, sitting in the dark,
talking to each other. So it was very familiar and we basically spent the whole weekend together
and you met all my friends and we had a big party and at the end of the weekend, I said,
okay, you know, if we're going to give this a shot, we probably shouldn't see
other people.
So it's a risk, you know.
Come in, man.
But I just didn't see how it would work if we were dating people locally and then also
seeing each other at distance because I've had long distance relationships with one
their heart and they take a lot of effort.
And so we decided we'd give it three months
and see what happened, and that was it.
This is an interesting thing.
Like, we're all, what is it?
There's several billion of us,
and we're kind of roaming this world,
and then you kind of stick together.
You find somebody that just like gets you.
And it's interesting to think about
there's probably thousands of not millions of people that would be sticky to you, depending
on the curvature of your space. But what is that? Could you speak to the stickiness, like
to the just the falling in love, like that somebody really gets you maybe by way of
Telling do you think do you remember there's a moment when you just
Realized
Damn it. I think I'm like I think that's this is the guy. I think I'm in love
We were having these conversations actually from the, really from the second weekend we were together.
So he flew back the next weekend to state college because my birthday, it was my 30th birthday.
My friends were throwing me a party and we went hiking and we hiked up some mountain and we were sitting on a cliff over this,
you know, over look and talking to each other and I was thinking and I actually said to him,
I'm like, I, I haven't really known you very long.
I feel like I'm falling in love with you, which can't possibly be happening.
I must be projecting.
But it's, but it certainly feels that way, right?
Like I don't believe and love it for sight.
So this can't really be happening,
but it sort of feels like it is.
And he was like, I know what you mean.
And so for the first three months or four months,
we would say things to each other like,
I feel like I'm in love with you.
But, you know,
but that can't, but things don't really work like that.
So, but, you know, so, and then it became a joke,
like, I feel like I'm in love with you, and then eventually,
you know, I think, but I think that was one moment
where we were talking about,
just, you know, not just all the great aspirations you have or all the
things, but also things you don't like about yourself, things that you're worried about,
things that you're scared of.
And then I think that was sort of solidified the relationship.
And then there was one weekend where we went to Maine in the winter, which I, I mean, I really love the beach
always, but in the winter particularly. Because it's just beautiful and calm and whatever. Yeah, and I also,
I do find beauty in starkness sometimes. So there's this grand majestic scene of you know this very powerful ocean and it's all
these like beautiful blue grays and it's just it's just stunning and so we were sitting on this huge
rock in Maine and where we'd gone for the weekend it was freezing cold and I honestly can't remember what he said or what I said or what what but I
definitely remember having this feeling of I absolutely want to
stay with this person like I and I don't know what my life will be like if I'm not
with this person like I need to be with this person.
Can we from a scientific and a human perspective dig into your belief that
first love it first sight is not possible.
You don't believe in it because there is, you don't think there's like a magic way you see
somebody in the Jack Kerrick way and you're like, wow, that's something. That's a special
little thing. Oh, I definitely, oh, I definitely think you can connect with someone
instant in an instance. And I definitely think you can say, oh, there's something there,
and I'm really clicking with that person romantically, but also just with friends. It's possible to do
that. You recognize a mind that's like yours or that's compatible with yours. There are ways
that you feel like you're being understood
or that you understand something about this person
or maybe you see something in this person
that you find really compelling or intriguing.
But I think, you know, your brain is predictive organ, right?
You're using your past.
You're projecting.
You're using your past to make predictions
and I mean, not deliberately,
that's how your brain is wired, that's what it does.
And so it's filling in all of the gaps
that you, you know, there are lots of gaps of information
that you don't, you know, information you don't have.
And so your brain is filling those in and And um, but it's not what love is. No, I don't think so actually. I mean, to some extent,
sure, you, you always, you know, there's research to show that people who are in love always
see the best in each other. And they, you know, when there's a, when there's a negative
interpretation or positive interpretation, you know, they there's a, when there's a negative interpretation or positive interpretation,
you know, they choose the positive ones. There's a little bit of positive illusion there, you know,
going on. That's what the research shows. But I think, I think that when you find somebody who,
somebody who not just appreciates your faults,
but loves you for them actually, like maybe even doesn't see them as a fault.
That's, so you have to be honest enough
about what your faults are.
So it's easy to love someone for all the things
that they, for all the things that they
For all the wonderful characteristics they have it's harder, I think to love someone despite their faults Or maybe even the faults that they see aren't really faults at all to you. They're actually something really special
But isn't isn't that can't you explain that by saying the brain kind of like you're projecting, it's your, you have a conception of a human being or just a spirit that really connects with you
and you're projecting that onto that person. And within that framework, all their faults
then become beautiful, like little... Maybe, but you just have to pay attention to the prediction error.
No, but maybe that's what love, like maybe you start ignoring the prediction error.
Maybe love is just your ability to ignore the prediction error.
Well, I think that there's some research that might say that, but that's not my experience, I guess. But there is some research that says, I mean, there's some research that might say that, but that's not my experience, I guess.
But there is some research that says, I mean, there's some research that says you have to
have an optimal margin of illusion, which means that you put a positive spin on smaller
things, but you don't ignore the bigger things, right?
And I think without being judgmental at all, when someone says to me,
you know, you're not who I thought you were. I mean, nobody says, I said that to me in a
really long time, but certainly when I was younger, that was, you know, you're not who I thought
you were. My reaction to that was, well, whose fault is that? You know, I'm a pretty,
I'm a pretty upfront person. I mean, I will though say that in my experience,
people don't lie to you about who they are.
They lie to themselves in your presence.
Yeah.
And so, you don't wanna get tied up in that,
tangled up in that. And I think from the get go, Dan and I were just for whatever reason. Maybe it's because we both have been divorced already. And you know, you know, he told me who he thought he was. And he was pretty accurate, it's more like pretty much actually. I mean, I, there's very,
I can't say that I've ever come across a characteristic in him that really surprised me
in a bad way. It's hard to know yourself. It is hard to know yourself.
I took a communicate that. For sure. I mean, I'll say, you know, I had the advantage of
training as a therapist, which meant for five years I was under a
fucking microscope. When I was training as a therapist, it was hour for hour supervision, which meant
if you were in a room with a client for an hour, you had an hour with a supervisor. So that supervisor
was behind the mirror for your session and then you went and had an hour of
discussion about what you said, what you didn't say, learning to use your own, your own feelings and
thoughts as a tool to probe the mind of the client and so on. And so you can't help but learn a lot
of, you can't learn help but learn a lot about yourself in that process.
Do you think knowing or learning how the sausage is made,
ruins the magic of the actual experience,
like using your side to study the brain,
do you think it ruins the magic of love at first sight?
Or do you consciously still able to lose yourself
in the moment?
I'm definitely able to lose myself in the moment.
Is wine involved?
Not always.
Chocolate, I mean, some kind of vinyl
during self-stints, right?
But yeah, for sure.
I mean, I guess what I would say, though,
is that for me, part of the magic is the process.
So I remember, while I was working on this book of essays, I was in New York.
I can't remember why I was in New York, but I was in New York for something.
And I was in Central Park, and I was looking at all the people with their babies.
And I was thinking, every, every, that each one of these, there's a tiny little brain.
That's wiring itself right now. And I, and I, I just, I felt in that moment, I was like,
I'm never going to look at an infant in the same way ever again. And so to me, I mean, honestly, before I started learning about brain development,
I thought babies were cute, but, you know, not that interesting until they could
do interact with you and do things.
Of course, my own infant, I thought was extraordinarily interesting, but, you know,
they're kind of like lumps.
That's, you know, until they can, you know, interact with you, but they are anything but lumps. I mean, like, you know, so, and part of the,
I mean, I, all I can say is I have deep affection now for like tiny little babies in a way that I
didn't really before, because of the, I'm just so curious.
But the actual process, the mechanisms of the wiring,
or the brain, the learning, all the magic of the neurobiology.
Yeah.
And, or something like, when you make eye contact
with someone directly, sometimes, you know, you feel something, right?
And, um, that's weird.
What is it? And what is that?
And so, so to me, that's not, um, that's not backing away from the moment.
That's like expanding the moment.
It's like, that's incredibly cool.
You know, when I was, um, I'll just say that when I was, when I was in graduate
school, I also was in therapy because it's almost a given that you're going to be in therapy
yourself if you're going to become a therapist. And I had a deal, you know, with my therapist,
which was that I could call time out at any moment that I wanted to, as long as I was
being responsible about it, and I wasn't using it as a way to get out of something. And he could tell me, no, you know, he could decline and
say, no, you're, you know, you're using this to get out of something. But I could call time out
whenever I want and say, what are you doing right now? Like, what are you, here's what I'm experiencing,
what are you trying to do? Like, I wanted to use my own experience to interrogate what the process was.
And that made it more helpful in a way.
Do you know what I mean?
So, yeah, I don't think learning how something works makes it less magical, actually.
But that's just me, I guess.
I don't know.
Would you?
Yes.
I tend to have two modes.
One is an engineer and one is romantic.
And I'm conscious of the gear.
Like there's two rooms into the one, the engineer room.
And I think that ruins the romance.
So I tend to, there's two rooms.
One is the engineering room,
think from first principles,
how do we build the thing that creates this kind of behavior
and then you go into the romantic room
where you're like emotional with the rollercoaster
and then you're the thing.
So let's take a slow and then you get married the next night
that you just this giant mess and you write a song
and then you cry and then you send married the next night, that you're just this giant mess, and you write a song, and then you cry,
and then you send a bunch of texts, and anger,
and whatever, and somehow you're in Vegas,
and there's random people, and you're drunk,
and whatever, all that, like in poetry,
and just mess of it, fighting, yeah, that's not...
Those are two rooms, and you go back between them.
But I think the way you put it is quite poetic.
I think you're much better at adulting with love
than perhaps I am.
Because there's a magic to children.
I also think of adults as children.
It's kind of cool to see.
It's a cool thought experiment to look at adults
and think like that used to be a baby. And then that's like a fully wired baby. And it's just walking
around pretending to be like all serious and important, wearing a suit or something. But that used
to be a baby. And then you think of like the parenting and all the experiences that they had.
Like it's cool to think of it that way.
But then I start thinking of like
from a machine learning perspective,
but once you're like the romantic moments,
all that kind of stuff, all that falls away,
I forget about all that.
I don't know.
That's the most important thing.
Maybe, maybe.
But I also think it might be an age thing
or maybe an experience thing.
So I think we all, I mean, if you're exposed to Western culture at all, you are exposed
to the sort of idealized, stereotypic, romantic, you know, exchange.
And what does it mean to be romantic? And so here's a test.
I'm gonna say how to phrase it.
Okay, so not really a test,
but this tells you something about your own ideas
about romance.
For Valentine's Day, one year,
my husband bought me a six-way plug.
Is that romantic or not romantic?
Like, sorry, six-way plug. Is that second out? Like a...
Yeah, like to put it in an outlet. Is that romantic or not romantic?
I mean, it depends the look in his eyes when you does it.
I mean, it depends on the conversation that led up to that point.
It depends how much...
It's like the music, because you have a very...
You're both from my experiences with you as a fan.
You have both a romantic niche, but you have a very pragmatic, like, you cut through
the bullshit of the fuzziness.
And there's something about a six-way plug
that cuts through the bullshit
that connects to the human.
Like he understands who you are.
Exactly.
Exactly.
That was the most romantic gift he could have given me
because he knows me so well.
He has a deep understanding of me,
which is that I will sit and suffer and complain about
the fact that I have to plug and unplug things.
And I will bitch and mone until the cows come home, but it would never occur to me to go
by a bloody six-way plug.
Whereas for him, he bought it, he plugged it in, he arranged, he taped up all my wires, he
made it like really usable.
And for me, that was the best, it was the most romantic thing because he understood who
I was and he did something very, or you know, just the casual, like we moved into a house that went,
we went from having a two car garage to a one car garage.
And I said, okay, you know, I'm from Canada,
I'm not bothered by snow.
Well, I mean, I'm a little bothered by snow,
but he's very bothered by snow.
So I'm like, okay, you can park your car in the garage,
it's fine.
Every day when it snows, he goes out and cleans my car.
Every day, like I never asked him to do
it. He just does it because he knows that I'm cutting it really close in the morning,
you know, when we, when we all used to go to work. I have a time to the second so that
I can get up as late as possible, work out as long as possible, you know, just to, and
make it into my office like a minute before my first meeting.
And so if it snows unexpectedly or something, I'm screwed because now that's an added, you
know, an added 10 or 15 minutes and I'm going to be late.
Um, anyways, you know, it's just these little tiny things that he's, he's, um, he's, he's
a really easygoing guy and he doesn't look like somebody who pays attention to detail.
He doesn't fuss about detail, but somebody who pays attention to detail. He doesn't fuss about detail,
but he definitely pays attention to detail.
And it is very, very romantic in the sense that he,
you know, he loves me despite my little details.
It understands you, but it is kind of hilarious
that that is the six-way plug
is the the most fulfilling richest display of romance in your life. I love it. I love it.
I love it. That's what I mean about romance. Romance is really it's not all about chocolates and
flowers and you know whatever. I mean those all nice too but um sometimes it's about the
six way plan. Sometimes it's about the six way plan. So um maybe one way I could ask before we
talk about the details. You also have the author of another book as we talked about how emotions are made
so it's interesting to talk about the process of writing. You mentioned you were in New York
what have you learned from writing these two books about the actual process of writing? And maybe, I don't know what's the most interesting
thing to talk about there, maybe the biggest challenges, or the boring, mundane, systematic,
like day to day of what worked for you, like hacks, or even just about the neuroscience
that you've learned through the process of trying to write them.
Here's the thing I learned. If you think that it's going to the process of trying to write them. Here's the thing I learned.
If you think that it's going to take you a year to write your book,
it's going to take you three years to write your book.
That's the first thing I learned is that no matter how organized you are,
it's always going to take way longer than what you think.
way longer than what you think. In part because very few people make an outline and then just stick to it, you know, the some of the topics really take on a life of their own
and to some extent you want to let them, you want to let them have their voice, you know,
you want to follow leads until you feel satisfied that you've dealt with the topic appropriately.
But I, and that part is actually fun.
It's not fun to feel like you're constantly behind the eight ball in terms of time.
But it is the exploration and the foraging for information is incredibly fun.
For me anyways, I found it really enjoyable.
And if I wasn't also running a lab at the same time and trying to keep my family going, you know, it would have been the whole thing
would have just been fun. But I would say the hardest thing about the most important thing
I think I learned is also the hardest thing and that it for me, which is knowing what to leave out. A really good storyteller knows what to leave out.
In academic writing, you shouldn't leave anything out.
All the details should be there. I've written or participated in writing over 200 papers, peer review papers.
So I'm pretty good with detail.
Knowing what to leave out and not harming the validity of the story.
That is a tricky, tricky thing.
It was tricky when I wrote how emotions are made, but that's a standard
Popular science book so it's 300 something pages and then you know, it has like a thousand end notes and then
Each of the end notes is attached to a web note, which is also long. So I mean, you know, it's
And it's start and I mean, you know, it's, and it's start, and I mean the final draft, I mean, I wrote
three drafts of that book actually, and the final draft, and then I had to cut by a third.
I mean, or, I mean, I, you know, it was like a hundred and fifty thousand words or something,
and I had to cut it down to like a hundred and ten. So obviously, I struggle with what to leave out. You know, brevity is not my strong suit.
I'm always telling people that it's a warning. So that's why this book was a, I, you know, I'd always
been really fascinated with essays. I love reading essays and after reading a small set of essays by
Ann Fadiman called at large and at small, which I just loved these little essays.
What's the topic of those essays?
They are, they're called familiar essays.
So the topics are like everyday topics, like male coffee, chocolate.
I mean, just like, and what she does is she weaves her own experience.
It's a little bit like these conversations that you're so good at curating, actually.
You're weaving together history and philosophy
and science and also personal reflections
and a little bit you feel like you're,
like, Eve's dropping on someone's train of thought in a way.
It's really, they're really compelling to me.
Even if it's just a commandean topic.
Yeah, but it's so interesting to learn about
like all of these little stories
in the wrapping of the history of like male.
Like that's really interesting.
And so I read these essays and then I wrote to her
a little fan girl email.
This was many years ago and I said,
I just loved this book and how did you learn
to write essays like this?
And she gave me a reading list of essays
that I should read, like writers.
And so I read them all.
And anyway, so I decided it would be a really good challenge
for me to try to write something really brief
where I could focus on, you know,
one or two really fascinating tidbits of neuroscience,
connect it to connect each one to something philosophical
or, you know, like just a question about human nature, do it in a really brief format without
violating the validity of the science. That was a, I just sent myself this,
what I thought of as a really, really big
challenge in part because it was an incredibly hard thing for me to do in the first book.
Yeah, we should say that this is the seven and a half lessons, a very short book. I mean,
it's a, it's like, it embodies brevity, right? The whole point throughout is just, I mean,
you could tell that there's editing, like, there's pain in trying to
bring as brief as possible, as clean as possible. Yeah. Yeah. So it's, the way I think of it is,
you know, it's a little book of big science and big ideas. Yeah, really big ideas and
in brief little packages. And, you know, I wrote it so that people could read it. I love reading on the beach.
I love reading essays on the beach. I read it. I wrote it so people could read it on the beach or
in the bathtub or you know a subway stop. Even if the beach is frozen over in the snow. So my husband
Dan calls it the first neuroscience beach read. That's his, that's his phrasing.
Yeah.
And like you said, you learn a lot about writing
from your husband and like you were saying offline.
Well, he's, he is of the two of us.
He is the better writer.
He's a masterful writer.
He's also, I mean, he, you know, he's a PhD in computer science.
He's, he's a software engineer mean, he's a PhD in computer science.
He's the software engineer, but he's also really good at organization of knowledge.
So he built for a company he used to work for.
He built one of the first knowledge management systems and he now works at Google where
he does engineering education. He understands how to tell a good story, just
about anything, really. He's got impeccable timing. He's really funny. Luckily for me,
he knows very little about psychology or neuroscience. Now he knows more, obviously, but so he was really, when how motions were made, he was really, really helpful
to me because the first draft of every chapter
was me talking to him about what I would talk out loud
about what I wanted to say and the order in which I wanted
to say it.
And then I would write it.
And then he would read it, and then he would read it
and tell me all the bits that could be excised.
And sometimes we would, I should say,
I mean, he and I don't really argue about much
except directions in the car.
Like, we're gonna have an argument
that's gonna be where it's gonna happen, where.
What's the nature of the argument
about directions exactly?
I don't really know.
It's just that we're very, I think it's that spatially, you know, he, he, um, I use egocentric
space.
So I want to say, you know, turn left like I was, I'm, I'm reasoning in relation to like
my own physical corporeal body.
So you know, you walk to the church and you turn left and
you, then you know, whatever, you know, I'm always like, and his, you know, he gives directions
allocentrically, which means, um, organized around north south east west.
So to you, the, the earth is at the center of the solar system and to him,
no, I'm at the center. I'm at the center. reasonably you're the center of the social system. Okay. So anyway,
so we, but here we, you know, we, we had some really rip roaring arguments, like really
rip roaring arguments where he would say, like, who is this for? Is this for the 1%? And I'd
be like 1% meaning not, you know, not wealth, but like civilians versus
academics.
You know, are these for the scientists or for the, is this for the civilians, right?
So he speaks for the, for the people for the civilians.
He speaks for the people and I'd be like, no, you have to.
And so he made, you know, after one terrible argument that we had where it was really starting
to affect our, our relationship because we were so mad at each other all the time.
He made these little signs, writing and science. And we only used them. This was like
when you when you pulled out a sign, that's it. Like the other person just wins and you have to stop fighting about it. And that's it. And so we just did that. And we didn't really have to use it too much for this book because this book was in some ways.
You know, I didn't have to learn a lot of new things for this book. I had to learn some, but I a lot of what I learned for seven for how motions are made really stood stood me in good stead for this book.
So there was a little bit, each essay
was a little bit of learning.
A couple of work was a little more than the small amount.
But I didn't have so much trouble here.
I had a lot of trouble with the first book.
But still even here,
he would tell me that I could take something out.
And I really wanted to keep it.
And I think we only use the signs once.
Well, if we could dive in some aspects of the book, I would love that.
Yeah.
Can we talk about?
So one of the essays looks at evolution.
Let me ask the big question. did the human brain evolved to think? That's essentially
the question that you address in the essay. Can you speak to it?
Sure. The big caveat here is that we don't really know why brains evolved. The big why questions are called t-liological questions, and in general,
scientists should avoid those questions, because we don't know really why. We don't know the why.
However, for a very long time, the assumption was that evolution worked in a progressive upward
scale, that you start off with
simple organisms and those organisms get more complex and more complex and more complex.
Now obviously that's true in some like really general way right that that life started
off as single cell organisms and you know things got more complex but the idea that
more complex. But the idea that brains evolved in some upward trajectory from simple brains in simple animals to complex brains in complex animals is called a phylogenetic scale.
And that phylogenetic scale is embedded in a lot of evolutionary thinking, including Darwin's actually. And it's been
seriously challenged, I would say, by modern evolutionary biology. And so, you know, thinking
is something that rationality is something that humans, at least in the West, really prize as a great human achievement.
And so the idea that the most common evolutionary story is that, you know, brains evolved in,
like, sedimentary rock with, you know, a layer for instincts.
That's your lizard brain and a layer on top of that for emotions. That's your limbic system,
limbic meaning border. So it borders the parts that are for instincts. Oh, interesting.
And then the neocortex or new cortex where rationality is supposed to live. That's the sort
of traditional story. It just keeps getting layered on top by evolution.
Right. And so you can think about, you know, I mean, sedimentary rock is the way typically
people describe it. The way I sometimes like to think about it is, you know, thinking
about the cerebral cortex like icing on an already baked cake, where the cake is your inner beast,
like boiling instincts and emotions
that have to be contained.
And by the cortex,
and it's just a fiction, it's a myth.
It's a myth that you can trace all the way back
to stories about morality in ancient Greece.
But what you can do is look at the scientific record and say, well, there's other stories that
you could tell about brain evolution and the context in which brains evolved. So when you look at creatures who don't have brains
and you look at creatures who do, what's the difference?
And you can look at some animals.
So we call, scientists call an environment
that an animal lives in, a niche, their environmental niche.
What are the parts of the environment that matter to that animal?
So there are some animals whose niche hasn't changed in 400 million years.
These creatures are modern creatures, but they're living in a niche that hasn't changed much.
And so their biology hasn't changed much. And you can kind of verify that by looking at the genes that lurk deep, you know, in the molecular structure of cells.
And so you can, by looking at various animals in their developmental state, meaning not you don't look at adult animals, you look at embryos of animals and developing animals, you can see, you can piece together a different story.
And that story is that brains evolved under the selection pressure of hunting, that in the Cambrian period,
hunting emerged on the scene where animals deliberately ate one another. And what, so, you know, before the Cambrian period,
the animals didn't really have, well, they didn't have brains,
but they also didn't have senses, really.
The very, very rudimentary senses.
So the animal that I wrote about in 7 and a half lessons
is called an amphyoxis or a lancelot.
And a little amphyoxys has no eyes.
It has no ears.
It has no nose.
It has no eyes.
It has a couple of cells for detecting light and dark for circadian rhythm purposes. And it can't hear, it has a vestibular cell
to keep its body upright.
It has a very rudimentary sense of touch
and it doesn't really have any internal organs
other than this like basically stomach.
It's like a, just like a,
it doesn't have an interic nervous system.
It doesn't have like a gut that moves. It doesn't have a gut that moves.
Like we do, it just has basically a tube.
So it's like a little container.
Like a little container, yeah.
And really, it doesn't move very much.
It can move.
It just sort of riggles.
It doesn't have very sophisticated movement.
And it's this really sweet little animal.
It sort of riggles its way to a spot and then
plants itself in the sand and just filters food as the food goes by. And then when the food
concentration decreases, it just ejects itself, riggles to some spot randomly where probabilistically there will be more food
and plants itself again.
So it's not really aware, very aware that it has an environment.
It has a niche, but that niche is very small
and it's not really experiencing that niche very much.
So it's basically like a little stomach on a stick.
That's really what it is.
And, but when animals start to literally hunt each other, all of a sudden it becomes important
to have to be able to sense your environment. Because you need to know, is that blob up ahead
to be able to sense your environment. Because you need to know, is that blob up ahead
gonna eat me or should I eat it?
And so all of a sudden you want distant senses
are very useful.
And so in the water,
distant senses are vision and a little bit hearing
well, faction smelling and touch. in the water touches a distance sense
because you can feel the vibration. So it's right. So in on air, in on land, you
know, vision is a distance sense. Touch not so much, but for elephants maybe.
Right. The vibrations.
Vibrations. All faction definitely because of the concentration of, you know, more concentrated
something is the more likely it is to be close to you.
So animals developed senses, they developed a head, like a literal head.
So ampheus doesn't even have a head, really.
It's just a, you know, one.
What's the purpose of a head?
That's a great question.
Is it to have a jaw? That's a great question. Is it to have a jaw?
That's a great question.
So, jaw, so yes, jaws are a major.
Useful feature.
Yeah, I was gonna say they're a major adaptation
after there's a split between vertebrates and invertebrates.
So, amphyoxys is thought to be very, very similar
to the animal that's before that split.
But then after the development, very quickly after the development of a head is the development
of a jaw, which is a big thing.
What goes along with that is the development of a brain.
Is that just a coincidence that the thing, the part of our body, of the mammal, I think, body that we eat with,
and like attack others with is also the thing that contains the, all the majority of the
brain type of stuff.
Well, actually, the brain goes with the development of a head and the development of visual system and an auditory system and an olfactory system and so on.
So your senses are developing and the other thing that's happening right is that animals are getting bigger.
Yeah.
Because they're and also they're niches getting bigger.
Well, this is the coach.
Sorry to take a tiny tangent on the niche thing is it seems like the
niche is getting bigger, but not just bigger, like more complicated, like shaped in weird
ways.
So like predation seems to create, like, like the whole world becomes your oyster, whatever.
But like you also start to carve out like the places in which you can operate the best.
Yeah. you also start to carve out the places in which you can operate the best. Yeah, and in fact, that's absolutely right.
And in fact, some scientists think that theory of mind,
your ability to make inferences about the inner life of other creatures actually developed
under the selection pressure of predation, because it makes you a better predator.
Because it makes you a better predator. Do you ever look at, you just said you looked at babies as these wiring creatures.
Do you ever think of humans as just clever predators?
Like that there is underneath it all is this, the Nietzschean will the power in all of its forms?
Or are we now friend there?
Yeah, so it's interesting.
I mean, there are zeitgeists
in how humans think about themselves, right?
And so if you look in the 20th century,
you can see that the idea of an inner beast
that we're just predators,
were just basically animals, baseless animals, violent animals that have to be contained by culture and by our prodigious neocortex
really took hold, particularly after World War I, and really held sway for much of that century and then around at least in Western
writing, I would say, we're talking mainly about Western scientific writing, Western philosophical
writing. And then late 90s maybe, you start to see books and articles about our social nature that were
social animals.
And we are social animals, but what does that mean exactly?
And about...
It's just carving out different niches in the space of ideas.
It looks like...
I think so. So do humans, can humans be violent?
Yes.
Can humans be really helpful?
Yes, actually.
And humans are interesting creatures because other animals can also be helpful to one another. In fact, there's a whole literature, booming literature on how other animals are, you know,
support one another.
They regulate each other's nervous systems in interesting ways, and they will be helpful
to one another, right?
So for example, there's a whole literature on rodents and how they signal one another, what is
safe to eat, and they will perform acts of generosity to their conspecifics that are related
to them or who they were raised with.
So if an animal was raised in a litter that is, that they were raised in,
although not even at the same time,
they'll be more likely to help that animal.
So there's always some kind of physical relationship
between animals that predicts whether or not
they'll help one another.
For humans,
humans,
we have ways of categorizing who's in our, who's in our group and who is
in by non-physical ways, right, by even by just something abstract like an idea.
And we are much more likely to extend help to people in our own group, whatever that
group may be, at that moment, whatever're, whatever feature you're using to define
who's in your group and who isn't. We're more likely to help those people than even members of
our own family at times. So, humans are much more flexible in their, in the way that they help
one another, but also in the way that they harm one another.
So I don't, I don't think I subscribe to, you know, we are primarily this or we are
primarily that.
I don't think have humans have essences in that way, really.
I apologize to take this in this direction for a brief moment, but I've been really deep
on Stalin and Hitler recently in terms of reading.
And is there something that you think about in terms of the nature of evil from a neuroscience
perspective? Is there some lessons that are sort of hopeful about human civilization
that we can find in our brain with regard to the hitlers of the world?
Do you think about the nature of evil?
Yeah, I do.
I don't know that what I have to say is so useful from a...
I don't know that I can say as a neuroscientist. Well, here's a study that, you know, so I sort
of have to take off my lab coat, right? And now I'm going to now conjecture as a human
who just also, who has opinions, but who also maybe has some knowledge about neuroscience.
But I'm not speaking as a neuroscientist when I say this,
because I don't think neuroscientists know enough
really to be able to say.
But I guess the kinds of things I think about are,
so I have always thought, even before I knew anything
about neuroscience, I've always thought that I don't think anybody could become Hitler,
but I think the majority of people can be,
are capable of doing very bad things.
It's just the question is really how much encouragement does it take
from the environment to get them to do something bad
That's what I kind of when I look at the life of Hitler
It seems like there's so many places where
Something could have intervened
No, it changed completely the person. I mean there's like the caricature like the obvious places where he was an artist
And if he wasn't
rejected as an artist, he was a reasonably good artist.
So that could have changed.
But just his entire, like, where he went and Vienna and all these kinds of things, like
little interactions could have changed.
And there's probably millions of other people who are capable, who the environment may be able to mold in the same way it did
this particular person to create this particular kind of charismatic leader in this particular
moment of time.
Absolutely.
And I guess the way that I would say, I would agree 100% and I guess the way that I would
say it is like this, in the West, we have a way of reasoning about causation, which focuses
on single, simple causes for things.
You know, there's an essence to Hitler.
There's an essence to his character.
He was born with that essence, or it was forged very, very early in his life.
And that explains the landscape of his, the horrible landscape of his behavior. But there's
another way to think about it, a way that actually is much more consistent with what we know
about biology, how biology works in the physical world. And that is that most things are
complex, not as in wow this is really complex and hard, but complex as in
complexity, that is more than the sum of their parts, and that most phenomena
have many, many weak non-linear interacting causes.
And so little things that we might not even be aware of
can shift someone's developmental trajectory from this to that.
And that's enough to take it on a whole set of other paths
that, you know, that, and that these things
are happening all the time.
So it's not random and it's not
really, it's not deterministic in the sense that like everything you do determines your
outcome. But it's a little more like, you know, you're nudging someone from one set of
possibilities to another set of possibilities. And I think the thing is the thing that I find optimistic is that the
the other side of that coin is also true. Right. So look at all the people who risk their lives
to help people they didn't even know. I mean, I just watched Borat, the new Borat movie, and the thing that
I came away with, but you know, the thing I came away with was, look at how like generous
people were in that. Oh, he's making, there are a lot of people he makes fun of, and that's
fine. But think about like those two guys, those, those, the, those, the Trump supporter guys, excellent. The Trump supporter guys.
Those guys, those kindness in them, right?
They took a complete stranger in a pandemic
into their house.
Who does that?
Like that's a really nice thing,
or there's one scene,
I mean, I want to spoil it for people I haven't seen it,
but there's one scene. I mean, I want to spoil it for people I haven't seen it. But there's one scene where he dresses up as a Jew.
I laugh myself sick at that scene seriously.
But he goes in and there are these two old Jewish ladies.
What a bunch of sweethearts.
Oh my gosh.
Like really.
I mean, that was what I was struck by actually.
I mean, there are other ones or like the babysitter, right?
I mean, she was really kind.
And yeah, so that's really what I was more struck by.
Like, you know, sure, there are other people who,
you know, who do very bad things or say bad things
or whatever, but, you know, or like there's one guy who's completely stoic, like the guy
at the, who's doing the, like, you know, sending the message, I don't know if it's facts or whatever.
He's just completely stoic, but he's doing his job actually, you know, like, you can't, you don't know
what he was thinking inside his head, you don't know what he was feeling, but he was totally professional doing his job actually. You know, like, you can't, you don't know what he was thinking inside his head. You don't know what he was feeling, but he was totally professional doing
his job. Okay. So I guess I just, I had a bit of a different, you know, view, I guess.
And I, so I also think that about people, I think everybody is capable of kindness. And
but, but, you know, it's the question is how much does it take and what are the circumstances?
So for a lot, some people, it's going to take a lot.
And for some people, they don't take a little bit.
But are we actually cultivating an environment for the next generation that provides opportunities
for people to go in the direction of caring and kindness.
Or, you know, I'm not saying that as like a, you know, polyanna-ish person,
you know, I think there's a lot of room for competition and debate and so on.
But I don't see Hitler as an anomaly.
And I never have, that was even before I learned anything
about neuroscience.
And now I would say knowing what we know
about developmental trajectories and life histories
and how important that is, knowing what we know about,
that the whole question of like nature versus nurture
is a completely wrong question.
We have the kind of nature that requires nurture.
We have the kind of genes that allow infants to be born
with unfinished brains, where their brains are wired
across a 25-year period with wiring instructions
from the world that is created for them.
And so I don't think Hitler is an anomaly. You know, even if it's
even if it's less probable that that would happen, it's possible that it could happen again.
And it's not, it's not like, you know, he's a bad seed. I mean, that doesn't, I just want
to say for, like, of course, he's completely 100% responsible for his actions and all the
bad things that happen.
So I'm not in any way.
This is not me saying.
But the environment is also responsible, in part, for creating the evil in this world.
So like Hitler's in different versions of more subtle, more smaller scale versions of
evil. But I tend to believe that there's a much stronger,
I don't like to talk about evolutionary advantages,
but it seems like it makes sense for love
to be a more powerful, emerging phenomena
of our collective intelligence versus hate
and evil and destruction.
Because from a survival, from a niche perspective, it seems to be, like, in my own life and my
thinking about the intuition about the way humans work together solve problems, it seems
that love is a very useful tool. I definitely agree with you, but I think the caveat here is that, you know, humans, the
research suggests that humans are capable of great acts of kindness and great acts of
generosity to people in their in-group. Right. And-
So we're also tribal.
Yeah, I mean, that's the, that's the
kitschy way to say it.
We're tribes, we're tribal.
Yeah.
So that's the kitschy way to say it.
What I would say is that, you know, there are a lot of features that you can use to describe
yourself.
You don't have one identity, you don't have one self, you have many selves, you have many teachers that you can use to describe yourself.
You don't have one identity.
You don't have one self.
You have many selves.
You have many identities.
Sometimes you're a man.
Sometimes you're a scientist.
Sometimes you're a, do you have a brother or a sister?
So sometimes you're a brother.
You know, you sometimes you're a friend.
Sometimes you're a human, so you can keep zooming out.
Yes, living organism on Earth.
Yes, exactly.
That's exactly right.
And so there are some people who,
there is research which suggests that there are some people
who will tell you, I think it's appropriate and better
to help, I should help my family
more than I should help my neighbors, and I should help my neighbors more than I should
help the average stranger, and I should help, you know, the average stranger in my country,
more than I should help somebody outside my country, and I should help humans more than
I should help, you know, other animals.
Right.
So there's a clear hierarchy of helping. And there are other people who, you know those attitudes are because I don't think the
research really tells us that, but in any case, there are, you know, and there are beliefs,
people also have beliefs about, there's this really interesting research in, really an
anthropology that looks at what are cultures particularly afraid of?
Like what the people in a particular culture are organizing their social systems to prevent
certain types of problems.
So what are the problems that they're worried about?
And so there are some cultures that are much more hierarchical and some cultures that
are much more hierarchical and some cultures that are much more egalitarian.
There are some cultures that in the debate of getting along versus getting ahead, there
are some cultures that really prioritize the individual over the group and there are
other cultures that really prioritize the group over the individual.
It's not like one of these is right and one of these is wrong.
It's that different combinations of these features are different solutions that humans
have come up with for living in groups, which is a major adaptive advantage of our species.
And it's not the case that one of these is better and one of these is worse, although
as a person, of course, I have opinions about that. And as a person, I can say, I would very much prefer certain,
I have certain beliefs, and I really want everyone in the world to live by those beliefs.
But as a scientist, I know that it's not really the case that for the species,
any one of these is better than any other.
There are different solutions that work
differentially well in particular,
you know, ecological parts of the world.
But for individual humans,
there are definitely some systems that are better
and some systems that are worse, right?
But when anthropologists or when neuroscientists
or biologists are talking,
they're not usually talking about the lives of individual people.
They're talking about the species, what's better for the species,
the survivability of the species, and what's better for the survivability of the species
is variation that we have lots of cultures with lots of different solutions,
because if the environment were to change drastically,
some of those solutions will work better than others,
and you can see that happening with COVID.
Right, so some people might be more susceptible
to this virus than others.
And so variation is very useful.
Say COVID was much, much,
more destructive than it is. And like, I don't know, 20% of the population was died.
You know, that's good to have variability because then at least some percent will survive.
Yeah, I mean, the, you know, the way that I used to describe it was, you know,
The way that I used to describe it was using those movies like The War of the Worlds or Pacific Rim, where aliens come down from outer space and they want to kill humans.
And so all the humans band together as a species.
And they all, all the little squabbling from countries and whatever, all, you know, goes away and everyone is just one big, you know, well, that, you know, that doesn't happen.
I mean, because COVID is, you know, the virus, like COVID-19 is like a creature from outer space.
And that's not what you see happening.
What you do see happening, it is true that some people,
I mean, we could use this as an example of essentialism
also.
So just to say, like exposure to the virus
does not mean that you will become infected with a disease.
So, I mean, in controlled studies,
one of which was actually a coronavirus, not COVID, but
this was, these are studies from 10 years ago.
You know, only somewhere between 20 and 40% of people were developed respiratory illness
when a virus was placed in their nose.
And so...
And there's a dose question, all those. Well, not in these studies, actually. So in
these studies, the dose was consistent across all people. And everything, you know, they were
sequestered in hotel rooms and what they ate was, you know, measured out by scientists and so on.
And so when you hold dose, I mean, the dose issue is a real issue in the real world. But
So when you hold dose, I mean, the dose issue is a real issue in the real world, but in these studies, that was controlled.
And only somewhere between, depending on the study, between 20 and 40% of people became
infected with a disease.
So exposure to a virus doesn't mean de facto that you will develop an illness.
You will be a carrier and you will spread the virus to other people,
but you yourself may not, your immune system may be in a state that you can make enough antibodies
to not show symptoms, not develop symptoms. And so of course what this means is, again, is that, you know, like if I asked you,
do you think, you know, a virus is the cause of a common cold or, you know, most people,
if I asked this question, I can tell you, because I asked this question.
So, do you think a virus is the cause of a cold?
Most people would say, yes, I think it is.
And then I say, yeah, well, only 20 to 40% of people develop respiratory illness in exposure to a virus. So clearly,
it is a necessary cause, but it's not a sufficient cause. And there are other causes. Again, so
not simple single causes for things, right? Multiple interacting influences. So it is true that individuals vary in their susceptibility to illness upon exposure.
But different cultures have different sets of norms and practices that allow,
that will slow or speed the spread.
And that's the point that I was actually trying to make here that, that, you
know, when the environment changes, that is, there's a mutation of a virus that is incredibly
infectious, some cultures will succumb, people in some cultures will succumb faster because of the particular norms
and practices that they've developed in their culture versus other cultures.
Now, there could be some other thing that changes that those other cultures would do
better.
So, very individualistic cultures like ours may do much better under other types of selection
pressures.
But for COVID, for things like COVID, my colleague Michelle Galfont, her research shows
that she looks at like loose cultures and tight cultures.
So cultures that have very, very strict rules versus cultures that are
much more individualistic and where personal freedoms are more valued.
And she, you know, her research suggests that for pandemic circumstances, tight cultures
actually the people survive better.
Just a little bit longer.
We started this part of the conversation talking about, you know, the humans evolved to think,
the human brain evolved to think.
Implying is there like a progress to the thing that's always improving.
That's right, we never, yeah.
And so the answer is no.
But let me sort of push back.
So your intuition is very strong here, not your intuition, the way you describe this.
But is it possible there's a direction to this evolution?
Like do you think of this evolution as having a direction?
Like it's like walking along a certain path towards something.
Is it, you know, what is it? Is it Elon Musk said like this
the earth got bombarded with photons and then all of a sudden like a Tesla was launched into
space or whatever rockets started coming? Like is there a sense in which even though in the
like within the system,
the evolution seems to be this massive variation, we're kind of trying to find our niches and
so on. But do you think they're ultimately when you zoom out, there is a direction that's
strong that does tend towards greater complexity and intelligence?
No.
So, I mean, and again, I would say is I'm really,
I'm really just echoing people who are much smarter
than I am about this.
But see, you're saying smarter.
I thought there's no smarter.
No, I didn't say there's no smarter.
I said there's no direction.
So I think the thing to say, or what I understand to be the case is that there's variation.
It's not unbounded variation. And there are selectors. There are pressures that will select.
And so not anything is possible because we live on a planet that has certain physical realities to it.
Right. But those physical realities are what constrain the possibilities,
the physical realities of our genes and the physical realities of our corporeal bodies and
the physical realities of life on this planet.
So what I would say is that there's no direction,
but there is, it's not infinite possibility
because we live on a particular planet
that has particular statistical
regularities in it and some things will never happen.
And so all of those things are interacting with our genes and so on
and are the physical nature of our bodies to make some things more possible
and some things less possible.
Look, humans have very complex brains, but birds have complex brains.
And so do octopuses have very complex brains.
And all three sets of all three of those brains are somewhat different from one another.
Some birds have very complex brains.
Some even have rudimentary language.
They have no cerebral cortex.
I mean, admittedly, they have, this is now lesson two, right?
They have, is it lesson two or lesson one?
Let me think.
No, this is lesson one.
They have, they have the same neurons.
The same neurons that in a human become this ripple cortex, birds have
those neurons. They just don't form themselves into a ripple cortex. I mean, crows, for example,
are very sophisticated animals. They can do a lot of the things that humans can do. In fact,
all of the things that humans do that are very special, that seem very special, there's
at least one other animal on the planet that can do those things too.
What's special about the human brain is that we put them all together. So we learn from
one another. We don't have to experience everything ourselves. We can watch another animal or
another human experience something and we can learn from that. Well, there are many other
animals who can learn by copying. We communicate with each other very, very efficiently. We
have language. But we're not the only animals who are efficiently by copying. Yeah. That we communicate with each other very, very efficiently. We have language, but we're not the only animals
who are efficiently efficient communicators.
There are lots of other animals who can efficiently communicate,
like bees, for example.
We cooperate really well with one another to do grand things,
but there are other animals that cooperate too.
And so every innovation that we have,
other animals have too.
What we have is we have all of those together interwoven
in this very complex dance in a brain that is not unique
exactly, but that is, you know, it does have some features that
make it use, that make it particularly useful for us to do all of these things,
you know, to have all of these things intertwined.
So, you know, our brains are actually the last time we talked, I made a mistake because
I said, in my enthusiasm, I said, you know, our brains are not larger,
are relative to our bodies, our brains are not larger
than other primates.
And that's actually not true actually.
Our brains relative to our body size is somewhat larger.
So an ape who's not a human, that's not a human,
their brains are larger than their body sizes than say relative
to like a smaller monkey. And a human's brain is larger relative to its body size than
a good approximation of your of whatever of the bunch of stuff that you can shove in there.
But what I was going to say is, but our cerebral cortex is not larger than what you would
expect for a brain of its size.
So relative to say an ape, like a gorilla or a chimp, or even a mammal, like a dolphin
or an elephant, our brains, our cerebral cortex is as large as you would expect it to be for
a brain of our size.
So, there's nothing special about our cerebral cortex, and this is something I explain in
the book where I say, okay, you know, like, by analogy, if you walk into somebody's house
and you see that they have a huge kitchen, you might
think, well, maybe this is a place I really definitely want to eat dinner at because
these people must be gourmet cooks.
But you don't know anything about what the size of their kitchen means unless you consider
it in relation to the size of the rest of the house.
If it's a big kitchen in a really big house. It's not telling you anything special, right?
If it's a big kitchen in a small house, then that might be a place that you want to
stay for dinner because it's more likely that that kitchen is large for a special reason.
And so the cerebral cortex of a human brain isn't in and of itself special because of its
size. However, there are some genetic changes
that have happened in the human brain as it's grown to whatever size is typical for the
whole brain size. There are some changes that do give the human brain slightly more of some capacities.
They're not special, but we can do some things much better than other animals.
Correspondingly, other animals can do some things much better than we can.
We can't grow back limbs.
We can't lift 50 times our own body weight.
Well, I mean, maybe you can,
but I can't lift 50 times my body.
Yeah, and ants with that regard are very impressive.
And then you're saying with the,
with the frontal cortex,
like that's the size is not always the right measure
of capability, I guess.
So size isn't everything.
Size isn't everything.
That's a quote of, but, but you know people like it when I disagree
So let me disagree with you on something or just like play devil's advocate a little bit
So you've painted a really nice picture that evolution doesn't have a direction
But is it possible if we just ran earth over and over again like this video game
That the final result would be the same.
So in the sense that we're eventually, there'll be an AGI type,
Hal9000 type system that just like flies and colonizes nearby
Earth-like planets, and it's always will be the same.
And the different organisms and the different
evolution of the brain like it doesn't feel like it has like a direction but given the constraints of
Earth and whatever this imperative whatever the hell is running this universe like it seems like it's
running towards something is it possible that will always be the same?
Thereby it will be a direction.
Yeah, I think, you know, as you know better than anyone else, that that answer to that question
is, of course, there's some probability that that could happen, right?
It's not a yes or no answer.
It's what's the probability that that would happen. And there's a whole distribution
of possibilities. So maybe we end up, what's the probability we end up with exactly the
same complement of creatures, including us. What's the likelihood that we end up with,
you know, creatures that are similar to humans, but similar in certain ways, let's
say, but not exactly humans, or all the way to a completely different distribution of
creatures.
What's your intuition?
If you were to bet money, what does that distribution look like if we ran Earth over
and over?
I would say given the, you're now asking me questions that
this is not science. This is not science.
So, but I would say, okay, well, what's the probability that it's going to be a carbon life form?
Probably high. Yeah.
But that's because I don't know anything about
really. I'm not really well versed that. What's the probability that, you know, I don't I'm not I'm not really well versed that what what's the probability that you know
So what's the probability that the animals will begin in the ocean and crawl out onto land versus the other way I would say probably high
I
Don't know but you know, but do I think what's the likelihood that we would end up with exactly the same or very similar
I think it's low actually.
I wouldn't say it's low, but I would say it's not 100%,
and I'm not even sure it's 50%.
I would say, I don't think that we're here by accident
because I think, like I said, there are constraints.
There are some physical constraints about Earth.
Now, of course, if you were a cosmologist,
you could say, well, the fact that the Earth is if you
were to do the big bang over again and keep doing it over and over and over again would you still get
the same solar systems would you still get the same planets would you know would you still get the
same galaxies the same solar systems the same planets you know I don't know but my guess is probably not
because there are random things that happen that can,
again, send things in one,
make one set of trajectories possible
and another set impossible.
So, but I guess my,
my, my, if I were gonna bet something,
money or something valuable,
I would probably say, it's not zero and it's not 100% and it's
probably not even 50%. So there's some probability. But I will be similar.
There will be similar, but I don't think I just think there are too many degrees of freedom.
There are too many degrees of freedom. I mean, one of the real tensions in writing this book
One of the real tensions in writing this book is to, on the one hand, there's some truth in saying
that humans are not special.
We are just, you know, we're not special in the animal kingdom.
All animals are well adapted.
If they're survived, they're well adapted to their niche.
It does happen to be the case that our niche is large.
For any individual human, your niche is whatever it is.
But for the species, right, we live almost everywhere, not everywhere, but almost everywhere
on the planet.
But not in the ocean. And actually other animals like bacteria,
for example, have us beat miles, you know, hands down, right? So by any definition, we're
not special. We're just, you know, adapted to our environment.
But bacteria don't have a podcast enough. Exactly, exactly.
They're not able to.
So that's the tension, right?
So on the one hand, we're not special animals.
We're just particularly well adapted to our niche.
On the other hand, our niche is huge.
And we don't just adapt to our environment.
We add to our environment.
We make stuff up, give it a name, and then it becomes real.
And so, no other
animal can do that. And so, I think the thing, the way to think about it, from my perspective,
or the way I made sense of it is to say, you can look at any individual single characteristic
that a human has that seems remarkable. And you can find that in some other animal.
that seems remarkable, and you can find that in some other animal. What you can't find in any other animal is all of those characteristics together in a brain
that is souped up in particular ways like arses.
And if you combine these things, multiple interacting causes, right?
Not one essence like your cortex, your big neocortex, but which isn't really that big.
I mean, it's just big for your big brain, for the size of your big brain.
It's the size it should be.
If you add all those things together and they interact with each other,
that produces some pretty remarkable results.
And if you're aware of that,
then you can start asking different kinds of questions about what it means to be human
and what kind of a human you want to be and what kind of a world do you want to curate for the next
generation of humans?
So I think that's the goal anyways, right?
It's just to have a glimpse of, instead of thinking about things in a simple, linear way,
just have a glimpse of the, some of the things that matter, that seem to that evidence suggests matters to the kind of
brain, in the kind of bodies that we have.
Once you know that you can work with it a little bit.
You write words have power over your biology.
Right now I can text the words I love you from the United States to my close friend in Belgium. And even though she cannot hear my voice or see my face, I will change her heart rate,
her breathing and her metabolism.
By the way, beautifully written.
Or someone could text something ambiguous to you like, is your door locked and odds are
that it will affect your nervous system in an unpleasant way. So, I mean, there's a lot of stuff to talk about here, but just one way to ask is,
why do you think words have so much power over our brain?
Well, I think we just have to look at the anatomy of the brain to answer that question. So if you look at the parts of the brain, the whole, the, the, the systems that are important
for processing language, you can see that some of these regions are also important for
controlling your major organ systems and your, like your autonomic nervous system that controls your
cardiovascular system your respiratory system and so on that you know these
regions control your
Endocrine system your immune system and so on so and you can actually see this in other animals too
so in birds for example the
Neurons that are responsible for birdsong also control
the systems of a bird's body. And the reason why I bring that up is that there's some scientists
think that the anatomy of a bird's brain, that control birdsong, are homologous or structurally,
have a similar origin to the human system for language.
So, the parts of the brain that are important for processing language are not unique
in and specialized for language. They do many things. And one of the things they do is control
your major organ systems.
Do you think we can fall in love, have arguments about this all the time? Do you think we can fall in
love based on words alone?
Well, I think people have been doing it for centuries.
I mean, they used to be the case of people wrote letters to each other.
You know, and then that was how they communicated and I guess that's how you
end in exactly, exactly, exactly, yeah, exactly.
So is the answer a clear yes there?
Because I get a lot of pushback from people often
that you need to touch in the smell
and the bodily stuff.
I think the touch in the smell and the bodily stuff helps.
Okay.
But I don't think it's necessary.
Do you think you're gonna have a lifelong monogamous relationship with an AI system
that only communicates with you on text, romantic relationship?
Well, I suppose that's an empirical question that hasn't been answered yet, but I guess
what I would say is I don't think I could.
Could any human, could the average human, you know, so if I, if I, I, even, I want to,
I want to even modify that and say, I'm thinking now of Tom Hanks and the movie.
Castaway.
Yeah, you know, with Wilson.
Yeah.
I think if that was, if you had to make that work, if you had to make that work.
With a volleyball, yeah.
If you had to make it work, could you, could you, could you prediction in simulation,
right?
So if you had to make it work, could you make it work using simulation and, you know,
your past experience, could you make it work?
Could you make it work?
You as a human.
Could you, could you, like, could you have a, could you have a relationship
literally with an inanimate object and have it sustain you in the way that another human
could?
Your life would probably be shorter because you wouldn't actually derive the body budgeting
benefits from, right?
So we've talked about how your brain, its most important job is to control your body and
you can describe that as your brain running a budget for your body. And there are metaphorical deposits and withdrawals into your body budget.
And you also make deposits and withdrawals in other people's body budgets
figuratively speaking. So you wouldn't have that particular benefit.
So your life would probably be shorter. But I think it would be harder for some people than for other people.
Yeah, I tend to mind tuition is that you can have a deep fulfilling relationship with a volleyball.
I think a lot of the environments that set up, I think that's a really good example.
Like the constraints of your particular environment
define the like, I believe like scarcity is a good catalyst for deep meaningful connection
with other humans and with inanimate objects. So the less you have, the more fulfilling those
relationships are. And I would say, the relationship with a volleyball,
the sex is not great, but everything else,
I feel like it could be a very fulfilling relationship,
which I don't know from an engineering perspective,
what to do with that.
And just like you said, it is an empirical question.
But there are places to learn about that, right?
So for example, think about children and their blankets.
So there, there's something tactile
and there's something olfactory.
And it's very comforting.
I mean, even for non-human little animals,
like puppies and so I don't know about cats, but...
Cats are cold harder, there's nothing going on there. I don't know. There are some cats that are very dog-like. I mean, really. So.
Some cats identify as dogs, yes. I think that's true. Yeah. They're, they're species fluid.
So you also write when it comes to human minds, variation is the norm. And what we call, quote, human nature is really many human natures.
Again, many questions I can ask here, but maybe it's an interesting one to ask is,
I often hear, you know, we often hear this idea of be yourself.
Is this possible to be yourself?
Is it a good idea to strive to be yourself?
Does that even have any meaning?
It's a very Western question, first of all,
because which self are you talking about?
You don't have one self.
There is no self that's an essence of you.
You have multiple selves.
Actually, there's research on this, you know, to quote the
great social psychologist, Hazel Marcus, you're never, you cannot be a self by yourself.
You, you know, you, and so different contexts pull for or draw on different features of
your, of who you are or what you, what you believe, what you feel, what your actions are.
Different contexts will put certain things or make some features be more in the foreground
and in the background. It takes us back right to our discussion earlier about
Stalin and Hitler and so on. The thing that I would caution, in addition to the fact
that there is no single self, that you have multiple selves
who you can be, and you can certainly choose the situations
that you put yourself in to some extent.
Not everybody has complete choice,
but everybody has a little bit of choice.
And I think I said this to you before that,
one of the pieces of advice that we gave
Sophia, you know, when she went our daughter when she was going off to college was try to spend time
around people choose relationships that allow you to be your best self. We should have said your best
selves, but the pool of selves given the environment.
Yeah, but the one thing I do want to say is that the risk of saying be yourself, just be yourself,
is that that can be used as an excuse. Well, this is just the way that I am. I'm just like this.
And that I think should be tremendously resisted
So that's one that's the that's for the excuse side, but you know, I'm really self critical often
I'm full of doubt and people often tell me it's just don't worry about it. Just be yourself man
and it's the thing is it almost it's not from an engineering perspective.
It does not seem like actionable advice because I guess constantly worrying about who, what
are the right words to say to express how I'm feeling is, I guess, myself.
There's a kind of line, I guess,
that this might be a Western idea,
but something that feels genuine
and something that feels not genuine.
And I'm not sure what that means,
because I would like to be fully genuine and fully open,
but I'm also aware, like this morning, I was like very like
silly and giddy out like out.
It was just being funny and relaxed and light.
Like there's nothing that could bother me in the world.
I was just smiling and happy.
And then I remember last night, it was just feeling like very grumpy.
Like, like stuff was bothering me, like certain things
were bothering me.
And like, what are those?
Those are different selves.
Like, who am I in that?
And what do I do?
Because if you take Twitter as an example, if I actually send a tweet last night and
a tweet this morning, it's going to be very two different people tweeting that. And I don't know what
to do with that because one does seem to be more me than the other, but that's maybe because
there's a narrative, the story that I'm trying, there's something I'm striving to be like
the ultimate human that I might become. I have maybe a vision of that and I'm trying
to become that. But it does seem like there's a lot of different
minds in there. And they're all like having a discussion and a battle for who's going to win.
I suppose you could think of it that way, but there's another way to think of it, I think,
and that is that maybe the more Buddhist way to think of it, or a more contemplative way to
think about it, which is not that you have multiple personalities
inside your head, but your brain has this amazing capacity.
It has a population of experiences
that you've had, that it can regenerate, reconstitute.
And it can even take bits and pieces
of those experiences and combine them into something new.
And it's often doing this to predict
what's going to happen next and to plan your actions.
But it's also happening, this also happens just,
that's what mind-wondering is or just internal thought and so on.
That's the same mechanism, really.
And so, a lot of times we hear the saying, you know, just think, if you think differently,
you'll feel differently.
But your brain is having a conversation continually with your body.
And your brain is trying to control, your body is sending information back to the brain.
And in part, the information that your body sends back to your brain, just like the information
coming from the world, initiates the next volley of predictions or simulations.
So in some ways, you could also say, coming from the world initiates the next volley of predictions or simulations.
So in some ways you could also say the way that you feel, I think we talked before about
affective feeling or mood coming from the sensations of body budgeting, influences what you think and as much as so feelings influence thought as much as
thought influence feeling and maybe more.
But just the whole thing doesn't seem stable.
Well, it's a dynamic system, Mr. Engineer.
Yeah.
Right?
It's a dynamic, it's a dynamical system, right?
Nonlinear dynamical system.
And I think that's, I'm actually writing a paper with a bunch of engineers about this,
actually. But I mean, other people have talked about the brain as a dynamical system before,
but you know, the real tricky bit is trying to figure out how do you get mental features
out of that system? I guess one thing to figure out how you get a motor movement out of
that system, it's another thing to figure out how you get a motor movement out of that system. It's another thing to figure out how you get a mental feature, like a feeling
of being loved or feeling of being worthwhile or a feeling of, you know, just basically feeling
like shit. How do you get a feeling, a mental features out of that system? So I would say
is that you aren't the Buddhist thing to say is that you're not one person
and you're not many people.
You are the sum of your experiences and who you are in any given moment, meaning what
your actions will be, is influenced by the state of your body and the state of the world
that you've put yourself in.
And you can change either of those things. One is a little easier to change than the other,
right? You can change your environment by literally getting up and moving,
or you can change it by paying attention to some things differently and letting other
some features come to the fore and other features, be background it like I'm looking around
your place.
Oh no.
And I see not something you should do.
No, I'm not, but I'm going to say one thing.
No green plants.
No green plants.
Because green plants mean a home and I want this to be temporary.
Fair.
Fair.
But.
What's what's what goes through your mind when you see no green plants?
No, I'm just making the point that
what if you again, you know, not everybody has control over their environment. Some people don't
have control over the noise or the temperature or you know any of those things. But everybody is a little bit of control and you can place things in your environment,
photographs, plants, anything that's meaningful to you and use it as a shift of environment
when you need it. You can also do things to change the conditions of your body. When
you exercise every day, you're making an investment in your body. Actually,
you're making an investment in your brain too. It makes your, even though it's unpleasant
and, you know, there's a cost to it, if you replenish, if you invest and you make up
that, you make a deposit and you make up that, what you've spent, you're basically making
an investment in making it easier for your brain to control your body in the future.
So, you can make sure you're hydrated. Drink water. You don't have to bite drink bottled water. You can drink water from the tap.
This is in most places, maybe not everywhere, but most places in the developed world. You can try to get enough sleep.
Not everybody has that luxury,
but everybody can do something to make their,
you know, body budgets a little more solvent.
And that will also make it more likely
that certain thoughts will emerge
from that prediction machine.
That's the control you do have.
Yeah.
Yeah, being able to control the environment,
that's really well put.
I don't think we've talked about this.
So let's go to the biggest unanswerable questions
of consciousness.
What is, you just roll your eyes.
I did.
That was my, yeah.
So what is consciousness from a neuroscience perspective? I know you I mean
I made notes, you know, because you gave me some questions in advance and I made notes for every single except that one
Yeah, well that one I had what the fuck
So is there something interesting because you're so pragmatic? Is there something interesting to say about intuition building about consciousness?
Or is this something that we're just totally coolist about that this is a let's focus on
the body, the brain, this is the body, the body speaks to the brain and let's just figure
this piece out and then consciousness will probably emerge somehow after that.
No, I think, you know, well, first of all, I'll just say up front, I am not a philosopher
of consciousness and I'm not a neuroscientist who focuses on consciousness.
I mean, in some sense, I do study it because I study affect and mood and that is the, you know, to use the phrase, that is the hard question of consciousness.
How is it that your brain is modeling your body?
Brain is modeling the sensory conditions of your body.
And it's being updated, that model is being updated by the sense data that's coming from
your body and it's happening continuously your whole life.
And you don't feel those sensations directly.
What you feel is a general sense of pleasantness or unpleasantness, comfort, discomfort, feeling
worked up, feeling calm.
So we call that affect, you know, most people call it mood.
So how is it that your brain gives you this
very low dimensional feeling of mood or affect when it's presumably receiving a very high
dimensional array of sense data. And the model that the brain is running of the body has
to be high dimensional because there's a lot going on in there, right? You're not aware, but as you're
sitting there quietly, as your listeners or our viewers are sitting, um, there might be working out,
running, uh, or as many of them write to me, they're laying a bed, smoking weed with their
eyes closed and that's fair. So maybe we should say that bit again then. So if, so some people may be working out some people may be relaxing. But you know,
even if you're sitting very still while you're watching this or listening to this,
there's a whole drama going on inside your body that you're largely unaware of. Yet your brain
makes you aware or gives you a status report in a sense,
by virtue of these mental features of feeling pleasant,
feeling unpleasant, feeling comfortable,
feeling uncomfortable, feeling energetic,
feeling tired and so on.
And so how the hell is it doing that?
That is the basic question of consciousness.
And like the status reports seem to be,
in the way we experience them,
it seemed to be quite simple.
Like, it doesn't feel like there's a lot of data.
Yeah, no, there isn't.
So when you feel, when you feel discomfort,
when you're feeling basically like shit,
you feel like shit, what does that tell you?
Like, what are you supposed to do next?
What caused it? I mean, the thing is not one thing caused it, right? It's multiple factors,
probably influencing your physical state. It says very high dimensional. Yeah. Very high dimensional.
And that, and the, there are different temporal scales of influence, right? So,
And there are different temporal scales of influence, right? So the state of your gut is not just influenced by what you ate five minutes ago.
It's also what you ate a day ago and two days ago and so on.
So I think when I'm not trying to weasel out of the question.
I just think it's the hardest question actually.
Do you think we'll ever understand it?
I think that we will understand it as well as we understand other things like the birth of the universe
or the nature of the universe. I guess I would say. So do I think we can get to that level
of an explanation? I do actually, but I think that we have to start asking somewhat different
questions and approaching the science somewhat differently than we have in the past.
I mean, it's also possible that consciousness is much more difficult to understand than
the nature of the universe.
It is, but I wasn't necessarily saying that it was a question that was of equivalent complexity.
I was saying that I do think that we could get to some, I am optimistic that I would not, I would be very willing to invest the
time, my time on this earth as a scientist in trying to answer that question if I could
do it the way that I want to do it, not the way that is currently being done.
So like rigorously.
I don't want to say unregurously.
I just want to say that there are certain set of assumptions that, you know, scientists
have what I would call ontological commitments.
There are commitments about the way the world is or the way that nature is
and these commitments lead scientists sometimes blindly without
they don't, scientists sometimes, sometimes scientists are aware of these commitments
but sometimes they're not. And these commitments, on the less, influence
how scientists ask questions, what they measure, how they
measure, and I just have very different views than a lot of
my colleagues about the ways to approach this, not everybody,
but the way that I would approach it would be different,
and it would cost more, and it would take longer.
It doesn't fit very well into the current incentive structure of science.
And so, do I think that doing science the way science is currently done with the budget
that it currently has and the incentive structure that it currently has, will we have an answer?
No, I think absolutely not.
Good luck is what I would say.
People love book recommendations. Let me ask what three books? Oh, you can't just like, you can't just give me three. I mean, like really three. What seven and a half books you can recommend.
So you're also about seven and a half lessons about the brain, your author of how emotions
are made.
Okay.
So definitely those are the top two recommendations of all the two greatest books of all
them.
Other than that, are there books that technical fiction, philosophical that you've enjoyed
or you might recommend to others?
Yes.
Actually, you know, every PhD student when they graduate with their PhD, I give them a
like a little library, like a set of books, you know, some of which they've already read,
some of which I want them to read or, but I think nonfiction books, I would read the things
I would recommend are The Triple Helix by Richard Lwanton.
It's a little book published in 2000, which is I think a really good introduction to complexity and population thinking as opposed to essentialism.
So this idea, essentialism is this idea that there's an essence to each person, whether it's a soul or your genes or what have you.
As opposed to this idea that you, we have the kind of nature that requires a nurture. We are, you are the product of a complex dance between an environment, between a set of genes and an environment
that turns those genes on and off to produce your brain and your body and really who you are at any given moment.
It's a title for that triple helix. So playing on the double helix where it's just the biology, it's bigger than the biology.
Exactly. It's a wonderful book. I've read it probably six or seven times throughout the year. He has another book too, which is
It's more I think scientists would find it. I don't know. I've loved it. It's called biology as ideology
And it really is all about I wouldn't call it one of the best books of all time, but I love the book because it really does point out
You know that love the book because it really does point out that science is
currently practiced. I mean, the book was written in 1991, but it actually, I think, still holds.
The science is currently practiced has a set of ontological commitments, which are somewhat
problematic.
So these assumptions are limiting?
Yeah. In ways that you, it's, you know, it's like your fish and water and you don't, like,
okay. So, yeah. So here's the water. David Foster Wallace stuff. Yeah. But, you know, but here like your fish and water and you don't like, okay, so yeah, so here's a
Foster wall is stuff, but you know, but here's a really cool thing I just learned recently
Is it okay to go off on this tangent for a minute? Yeah, yeah, it's good tangency great
I was just gonna say that I just learned recently that we don't have
Water receptors on our skin. So how do you know when you're sweating? How do you know when when a rain drop
When you know when it's gonna rain and you know like a rain drop hits your skin and you can feel that little
Drop of wetness. How is it that you feel that drop of wetness when we don't have
Water receptors in our skin and I was when I mind blown already. Yeah, I that was I have my reaction too, right? I was like
Of course we don't,
because we evolved in the water. Like, why would we need, you know, it just, it was just this,
like, you know, you have these moments where you're like, oh, of course, let's go. Yeah, so,
you'll never see rain the same way again. So the answer is it's a, it's a, it's a combination of
temperature and touch. But it's a complex sense that's only computed in your brain.
There's no receptor for it.
Anyways.
Yeah, that's why like snow versus cold rain versus warm rain all feel different because
you're trying to first stuff from the temperature and the size of the droplets is fascinating.
Yeah, your brain is a prediction machine.
It's using lots and lots of information and combining it.
Yeah, anyway, so.
But so biology is ideology is, I wouldn't say it's one of the greatest books of all time,
but it is a, it is a really useful book.
There's a book by, if you're interested in psychology or the mind at all, there's a
wonderful book, a little,
it's a fairly small book called naming the mind by Kurt Danziger, who's a historian of
psychology.
Everybody in my lab reads both of these books.
So what was the book?
It's about the origin of the, where did we get the theory of mind that we have, that the
human mind is populated by thoughts and feelings and perceptions, and where did those categories
come from?
Because they don't exist in all cultures.
Also, this isn't, that's the cultural construct.
The idea that you have thoughts and feelings and they're very distinct is definitely a cultural
construct.
It's another mind blowing thing, just like the rain.
So Kurt Danzinger is an opening chapter in that book is absolutely mind blowing.
I love it.
I love it. I just think it's fantastic.
And I would say that there are many, many popular science books that I could
recommend that I think are extremely well written in their own way. You know,
before I, maybe I said this to you, but before I undertook writing, how emotions are made, I read, I don't
know, somewhere on the order of 50 or 60 popular science books to try to figure out how to write
a popular science book because while there are many books about writing, Stephen King has a great
book about writing and you know where he gives tips
Interlaced with his own personal history
That was where I learned you write for a specific person you have a specific person in mind and that's for me that person is
My is down
This fast. I mean, that's a whole another conversation to have like which popular science books like what you learned
from that search.
Because there's, I have some, for me, some popular science books are like, I just roll my
eyes like this is two.
It's the same with TED Talks.
Like some of them go too much into the flowery and don't, I don't, I would say don't give
enough respect to the intelligence of the reader
and but this this is my own bias. I completely agree with you and in fact I have a colleague his name
is Van Yang who you know he produced a cinematic lecture of how motions are made that we wrote together with
Joseph Friedman in a relation. Well, I mean you and I are probably, you know,
some yeah. But it's the memories are in there somewhere. Yeah, it's from many,
many, many generations ago. Well, Half my family is Russian, so from the good half.
The good half, right?
But, you know, he,
when his goal actually is to produce,
you know, videos and lectures that are beautiful and educational and that don't
don't dump the material down.
And he's really remarkable at it actually. I mean just
but again, you know, that's good. That that requires a bit of a paradigm shift.
We could have a whole conversation about the split between
entertainment and education in this country and why it is the way it is but
That's that's another conversation to be continued
But I would say the if I were to pick one book that I think is a really good example of good science writing it would be the beak of the finch
Which is it want to it want a Pulitzer Prize a number of years ago. And I'm not I'm
not remembering the author's name. I'm blanking. But the I'm guessing is it is it focusing
on birds and the evolution of birds? Actually, there's also the evolution of beauty. Yeah, which is also a great book. But no, the beak of the finch is,
it has two storylines that are interwoven. One is about Darwin and Darwin's
explorations in the Galapagos Island. And then modern day researchers from Princeton who have a research program in the Galapagos
looking at Darwin's Finches.
And it's just a really, first of all,
there's top notch science in there.
And really, like, evolutionary biology
that a lot of people don't know.
And it's told told really really well. It sounds like there also there's a narrative in there. There's it's a storytelling too
Yeah, I think all good
Popular science books are are storytelling for just you know, but storytelling grounded constrained by you know the evidence
And then I just want to say that there are
Perfection. I'm a really big fan of
love stories just to return us to the topic that we began with. And so my, some of my favorite
love stories are major pedigrues last stand by Helen Simonson. It's a, it's a love story
about people who you wouldn't expect to fall in love and
all the people around them who have to overcome their prejudices. And I love this book.
What do you like? Like what makes a good love story? There isn't one thing. You know, there are
many different things that make a good love story. But I think in this case, you can feel the journey. You can feel the journey that these characters are
on, and all the people around them are on this journey, too, basically, to come to grips
with this really unexpected love, really profound love that develops
between these two characters who are very unlikely
to have fallen in love, but they do.
And it's just, it's very gentle.
Another book like that is the story, life of AJ Feirke,
which is also a love story. But in this case, it's a love story
between a little girl and her adopted dad. And the dad is this like real chermogyny, you know,
guy. But of course, there's a story there. And it's just a beautiful love story.
But it also, it's like everybody in this community
falls in love with him because he falls in love with her.
And she just gets left at his store, his book story,
his failing book store.
And he discovers that, you know, he feels like inexplicably
this need to take care of this little baby,
and this whole life emerges out of that one decision,
which is really beautiful, actually.
Very poignant.
Do you think the greatest stories have a happy ending or a heartbreak at the end?
That's such a Russian question. It's like Russian tragedies, you know. So I would say the answer
to that for me, there has to be heartbreak. Oh yeah, I really don't like heartbreak. I don't like heartbreak. I want there to be a happy ending or at least a hopeful ending.
But, you know, like Dr. Shavago, like, or the English patient, oh my goodness, like why?
It's just, yeah, no. Well, I don't think there's a better way to end it on a happy note,
Well, I don't think there's a better way to end it on a happy note like this. Lisa, like I said, I'm a huge fan of yours.
Thank you for wasting yet more time with me talking again.
People should definitely get your book and maybe one day I can't wait to talk to your
husband as well.
Well, right back at you, Alexi.
Thanks for listening to this conversation with Lisa Feldman-Barritt, and thank you to our
sponsors.
I fly to Greens, the all-in-one drink that I start every day with to cover all my nutritional
bases, eight sleep, a mattress that cools itself and gives me yet another reason to enjoy
sleep, masterclass, online courses that I enjoy from some of the most amazing
humans in history, and better help, online therapy with a licensed professional.
Please check out these sponsors in the description to get a discount and to support this podcast.
If you enjoyed this thing, subscribe on YouTube, review it with 5 stars and up a podcast,
follow on Spotify, support on Patreon,
or connect with me on Twitter at Lex Friedman.
And now, let me leave you some words from SunZoo and the art of war.
There are not more than 5 musical notes, yet the combination of these 5 give rise to
more melodies that can ever be heard.
There are not more than 5 primary colors, yet in combination they produce more hues
that can ever be seen.
There are not more than five cardinal tastes, and yet combinations of them yield more
flavors that can ever be tasted.
Thank you for listening, and hope to see you next time.
you