Lex Fridman Podcast - #302 – Richard Haier: IQ Tests, Human Intelligence, and Group Differences
Episode Date: July 14, 2022Richard Haier is a psychologist specializing in the science of human intelligence. Please support this podcast by checking out our sponsors: - Calm: https://calm.com/lex to get 40% off - Linode: https...://linode.com/lex to get $100 free credit - BiOptimizers: http://www.magbreakthrough.com/lex to get 10% off - SimpliSafe: https://simplisafe.com/lex and use code LEX - MasterClass: https://masterclass.com/lex to get 15% off EPISODE LINKS: Richard's Twitter: https://twitter.com/rjhaier Richard's Website: https://richardhaier.com/ Documents & Articles: 1. Child IQ and survival to 79: https://ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5491698/ 2. Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth: https://my.vanderbilt.edu/smpy/files/2013/02/DoingPsychScience2006.pdf Books: 1. The Neuroscience of Intelligence: https://amzn.to/3n50DcC 2. The Book of Five Rings: https://amzn.to/3y4Xcc6 3. The Rise and Fall of the Third Reich: https://amzn.to/3zPAW7q 4. Flowers for Algernon: https://amzn.to/3OfRKZS 5. The Bell Curve: https://amzn.to/3Ng4RJe 6. The Mismeasure of Man: https://amzn.to/3N9IkxB 7. Human Diversity: https://amzn.to/3O7Trsc 8. Facing Reality: https://amzn.to/3bfzqkX PODCAST INFO: Podcast website: https://lexfridman.com/podcast Apple Podcasts: https://apple.co/2lwqZIr Spotify: https://spoti.fi/2nEwCF8 RSS: https://lexfridman.com/feed/podcast/ YouTube Full Episodes: https://youtube.com/lexfridman YouTube Clips: https://youtube.com/lexclips SUPPORT & CONNECT: - Check out the sponsors above, it's the best way to support this podcast - Support on Patreon: https://www.patreon.com/lexfridman - Twitter: https://twitter.com/lexfridman - Instagram: https://www.instagram.com/lexfridman - LinkedIn: https://www.linkedin.com/in/lexfridman - Facebook: https://www.facebook.com/lexfridman - Medium: https://medium.com/@lexfridman OUTLINE: Here's the timestamps for the episode. On some podcast players you should be able to click the timestamp to jump to that time. (00:00) - Introduction (08:06) - Measuring human intelligence (22:34) - IQ tests (45:23) - College entrance exams (53:59) - Genetics (59:58) - Enhancing intelligence (1:07:27) - The Bell Curve (1:19:58) - Race differences (1:39:11) - Bell curve criticisms (1:48:21) - Intelligence and life success (1:57:57) - Flynn effect (2:02:49) - Nature vs nuture (2:29:42) - Testing artificial intelligence (2:41:46) - Advice (2:45:53) - Mortality
Transcript
Discussion (0)
The following is a conversation with Richard Hire on a science of human intelligence.
This is a highly controversial topic, but a critically important one for understanding
the human mind.
I hope you will join me in not trying away from difficult topics like this, and instead,
let us try to navigate it with empathy, rigor, and grace.
If you're watching this on video now, I should mention that I'm
recording this introduction in an undisclosed location somewhere in the world. I'm safe
and happy and life is beautiful.
And now a quick few second mention of each sponsor. Check them out in the description. It's
the best way to support this podcast. We've got calm for meditation, linoed for cloud computing, buy optimizers for health,
simply safe for security, and masterclass for inspiration, choose wisely my friends.
And now, until the full ad reads, as always, no ads in the middle, I try to make this interesting,
but if you skip them, please still check out our sponsors. I enjoy their stuff, maybe you will too.
I enjoy their stuff, maybe you will too. This show is brought to you by Calm, a meditation and mental wellness app, over 100 million people
around the world use Calm to take care of their minds.
I'm a big believer, I'm a big fan of meditation in general, and there's all kinds of ways to
achieve that state of mind, where it's complete focus, where there's focus on the breath,
focus on a singular idea, or focus of just the emptiness of space. I do that through running, through walking,
through deep work sessions, I'm going to have to focus on a particular idea. Herman has
said talked about that, which is meditation is just not simply meditation. It's the ability
to just focus on one thing and all the great writers and the scholars are able to do that even if they don't call them meditation.
But if you want to get into it and do this is the mataclean rigorously guided meditation is a great way to do it and calm is my favorite app to get that done. you'll get 40% off a calm premium subscription, which includes hundreds of hours of programming,
check them out at calm.com slash flex.
This show is also brought to you by Linode,
Linux Virtual Machines.
It's an awesome compute infrastructure
that lets you develop, deploy, and scale,
whatever applications you build faster and easier.
This is both for small personal projects and huge systems. It's
lower cost than AWS. They got great customer service 24, 7, 365, however many days there are a year.
Like I said, I don't shout up about it. I love Linux. I love Linux in the cloud. I love
when the interface and the infrastructure of distributed computing is done really well
whether it's for host websites or doing any kinds of compute whether it's CPU GPU all
that kind of stuff. I love it when people do this well. And the interface on Linode is
just done incredibly well. It's user friendly, very accessible, very clear to monitor what
the heck is going on. I love it. Did I mention how much I love Linux?
I think of Linux as love in the cloud because Linux is love.
Visit linod.com slash legs for free credit.
This shows also brought to you by by optimizers that have a new magnesium supplement.
When I fast, I'm doing keto or carnivoresodium potassium and magnesium are essential. That's the what they call the electrolytes of those. Magnesium is the
trickiest to get right. That's why I use magnesium breakthrough for bi optimizers. Most
of the other supplements contain only one or two forms of magnesium like glycinate or
citrate. I've actually had a long discussion slash debate,
slash learning session with Andrew Huberman,
who may be the world's greatest expert in magnesium.
It was very eye opening.
Anyway, there's not just glycinate and citrate,
there's seven other kinds that the body can benefit from.
So this particular bio-optimizer supplement gets all of them. So you're covered.
And that makes it super easy so you don't have to think about it. Like I said, the difference between
an awesomely clear and focused mind on keto or when you're fasting for 16 or 24 hours or
multiple days, 48, 72 hours. The difference between feeling crappy and feeling
amazing is electrolytes and magnesium for me, like when I fasted for 72 hours, that was
the key. I wasn't feeling that great when that was lower than it needed to be when it
got higher. I felt great. Anyway, get a special discount at magbreakthrough.com slash Lex.
This show is also brought to you by SimplySafe, a home security company designed to be simple
and effective.
It takes 30 minutes to set up.
You can customize the system to your needs.
I have it set up in my place and I love it.
It is, I guess, the first layer of security in my place. I take my physical and cyber security
extremely, extremely seriously.
And part of doing a good job of securing
your physical location is having systems
that are actually really easy to set up,
a really easy to monitor, really easy to debug
if anything's going wrong.
And that's exactly what SIPLOSafe does for any consumer. You don't even need professional installation.
I think they offer professional installation, but you can set it up yourself.
It's super easy. From a robotics perspective, it's cool to turn your home into a
smart robot type of device that's sensing the world around it through cameras,
the motion sensors, all that kind of stuff. Go to simplisave.com slashlex to customize your system and claim your free indoor security
camera plus 20% off with interactive monitoring, which you should definitely sign up for.
This show is also brought to you by Masterclass.
$180 a year gets you in all access paths to watch courses from the best people in the world in their respective disciplines
the list is
freaking ridiculous Chris Hadfield, Neil the Grass Tyson will write Carlos Santana, Gareth Kasparov, Daniel Nagrano,
Neil Gaiman, Barnes-Corsezi, Tony Hawk, Jane Goodall, I really gotta talk to Jane Goodall at some point here soon
she's an incredible incredible woman
And the list keeps going Andrew Huberman just texted me and he should definitely have a masterclass because that guy actually every single episode of his podcast is
Is a masterclass of its own this whole
Series of adres is all about how awesome Andrew Huberman is which he is
of batteries is all about how awesome Andrew Huberman is, which he is.
Anyway, I do believe that the way to learn about a topic, there's two ways. One is you do hands-on training.
So you follow tutorials that are hands-on and you build something, something you care about.
Or you listen or get as close as possible to get the wisdom from the best people in the
world that are doing that kind of thing.
There's no replacement for that. get as close as as possible to get the wisdom from the best people in the world that are doing that kind of thing.
There's no replacement for that.
Just in life in general, whatever you want to do in life, make sure you figure out who's the best person in the world at that
and find a way to work with them, even if it's like intern or make coffee for them, all that kind of stuff.
Masterclass makes it super easy for you that you don't have to make coffee for them, all that kind of stuff. Masterclass makes it super easy for you
that you don't have to make coffee for anybody. You just pay $180 a year and you get to
listen to these experts. I highly recommend you check it out to get unlimited access to every
masterclass and get 15% off an annual membership at masterclass.com slash Lex. This is a Lex
Friedman podcast. To support it, please check out our sponsors in the description.
And now, dear friends, his Richard Hire.
What are the measures of human intelligence? How do we measure it?
Everybody has an idea of what they mean by intelligence.
In the vernacular, what I mean by intelligence is just being smart.
How well you reason, how well you figure things out.
What you do when you don't know what to do.
Those are just kind of everyday common sense definitions of how people use the word intelligence.
If you want to do research on intelligence, measuring something that you can study scientifically is a little trickier. And what almost all researchers who study intelligence use
is the concept called the G-Factor, general intelligence.
And that is what is common.
That is a mental ability that is common to virtually all tests
of mental abilities.
What's the origin of the term G-factor,
by the way, it's such a funny word,
for such a fundamental human thing?
The general factor, I really started with Charles Spirman.
And he noticed, this is like, boy, more than 100 years ago,
he noticed that when you tested people with different tests,
He noticed that when you tested people with different tests, all the tests were correlated positively.
So he was looking at student exams and things.
And he invented the correlation coefficient, essentially.
And when he used it to look at student performance on various topics, he found that all the scores were correlated with each other, and they were all positive correlations.
So he inferred from this that there must be some common factor that was irrespective of the content of the test. And positive correlation means if you do well on the first test, you're likely to do well
on the second test.
And presumably that holds for tests across even disciplines.
So not within subject, but across subjects.
So that's where the general comes in.
Something about general intelligence.
And when you were talking about measuring intelligence
and trying to figure out something difficult about this world
and how to solve the puzzles of this world,
that means generally speaking.
Not some specific test, but across all tests.
Absolutely right.
And people get hung up on this because they say,
well, what about the ability to do X? Isn't that independent? And they said,
I know somebody who's very good at this, but not so good at this. There's other thing. And so
there are a lot of examples like that, but it's a general tendency. So exceptions really don't disprove. You know, your everyday experience is not the same
as what the data actually show.
And your everyday experience, when you say,
oh, I know someone who's good at X,
but not so good at Y,
that doesn't contradict the statement of about it.
He's not so good, but he's not the opposite.
He's not a negative correlation.
Okay, so we're not our anecdotal data. I know guys really good at solving some kind of visual thing.
That's not sufficient for us to understand actually the depths of that person's intelligence.
So how this idea of G-factor, how much evidence is there, how strong, you
know, given across the decades that this idea has been around, how much has it been held
up, that there is a universal sort of horsepower of intelligence that's underneath all of
it, all the different tests we do to try to get to
this thing in the depths of the human mind that's a universal stable measure of a person's intelligence.
You use the couple of words in there, stable and we have to be precise with words.
Well, we can get away with being poetic. We can. There's a lot about research in general,
not just intelligence research, that is poetic.
Science has a panetic aspect to it.
And good scientists are very intuitive.
They're not just, hey, these are the numbers.
You have to kind of step back and see the big picture.
When it comes to intelligence research, you asked how well has this general
concept held up? And I think I can say without fear of being empirically contradicted, that
it is the most replicated finding in all of psychology. Now some cynics may say, well,
big deal, psychology, we all know there's a replication crisis in psychology, and a lot of this stuff doesn't
replicate.
That's all true.
There is no replication crisis when it comes to studying the existence of this general
factor.
Let me tell you some things about it.
It looks like it's universal in that you find it in all cultures.
The way you find it, step back one step,
the way you find it is to give a battery of mental tests.
What battery you choose, take a battery of any mental tests
you want, give it to a large number of diverse people.
And you will be able to extract statistically
the commonality among all those tests.
It's done by a technique called factor analysis.
You people think that this may be a statistical artifact of some kind.
It is not a statistical artifact.
What is factor analysis?
Factor analysis is a way of looking at a big set of data and look at the correlation
among the different test scores and then find empirically the clusters of scores that go
together.
And there are different factors.
So if you have a bunch of mental tests, there may be a verbal factor.
They may be a numerical factor.
There may be a visual spatial factor, but those factors have
variants in common with each other, and that is the common, that's what's common among all the
tests, and that's what gets labeled the G factor. So if you give a diverse battery of mental tests,
and you extract a G factor from it, That factor usually accounts for around half of the variance.
It's the single biggest factor, but it's not the only factor,
but it is the most reliable, it is the most stable,
and it seems to be very much influenced by genetics.
It's very hard to change the G factor with
training or drugs or anything else. You don't know how to increase the G factor.
Okay, you said a lot of really interesting things there. So first, I mean, just so
get people used to it in case they're not familiar with this idea. G-factor is what we mean. So often there's this term used IQ,
which is the way IQ is used,
they really mean G-factor in regular conversation.
The way, because what we mean by IQ,
we mean intelligence,
and what we mean by intelligence,
we mean general intelligence,
and general intelligence and general
intelligence in the human mind from a psychology from a serious rigorous scientific perspective
actually means g factor. So g factor equals intelligence just in this conversation to define terms.
Okay, so so there's this stable thing called g factor. You said it. Now, factor, you said factor many times means a measure
that's a potential could be reduced to a single number across the different factors you
mentioned. And what you said it accounts for half, half ish, accounts for half ish of what of variance across the different set of tests.
So if you do for some reason, well on some set of tests, what does that mean?
So that means there's some unique capabilities outside of the G factor that might account
for that.
And what are those?
What else is there besides the raw horsepower, the engine inside your mind that generates intelligence?
There are test taking skills.
There are specific abilities.
Someone might be particularly good at mathematical things, mathematical concepts,
even simple arithmetic people, some people are much better than others.
You might know people who can memorize,
and short-term memory is another component of this.
Short-term memory is one of the cognitive processes that's most highly correlated with
the G factor. So all those things like memory, test, test taking skills account for variability
across the test performances. But you so you can, you can run, but you can't hide from the thing that God
gave you, the genetics.
So that G factor science says that G factor is there.
Each one of us have, each one of us has a G factor.
Oh boy.
Some have more than others.
I'm getting uncomfortable already.
Well, IQ is a score.
others. I'm getting uncomfortable already. Well, IQ is a score. And IQs, an IQ score is a very good estimate of the G factor. You can't measure G directly. There's no direct measure. You
estimate it from these statistical techniques. But an IQ score is a good estimate. Why? Because
a standard IQ test is a battery of different mental abilities.
You combine it into one score and that score is highly correlated with the G factor,
even if you get better scores on some subtests than others. Because again, it's what's common
to all these mental abilities. So, a good IQ test. I'll ask you mental abilities. So a good IQ test.
And I'll ask you about that, but a good IQ test
tries to compress down that battery of tests.
Like, tries to get a nice battery,
a nice selection of variable tests into one test.
And so in that way, it sneaks up to the G-File.
And that's another interesting thing about G-Factor.
Now, you give, first of all, you have a great book on the neuroscience of intelligence
you have a great course, which is when I first learned a great teacher.
Let me just say, thank you.
Your course at the teaching company, I hope I'm saying that correctly.
The intelligent brain. The intelligent brain.
The intelligent brain is when I first heard about this G factor, this mysterious thing that
lurks in the darkness that we cannot quite shine a light on, we're trying to sneak up on.
So the fact that there's this measurable measure of intelligence, we can't measure it directly.
But we can come up with a battery
test or one test that includes a battery of variable type of questions that can reliably
or attempt to estimate in a stable way that G factor. That's a fascinating idea. So for
me as an AI person, it's fascinating. It's fascinating, there's something stable like that
about the human mind, especially if it's grounded
in genetics.
It's both fascinating that as a researcher of the human mind,
and all the human psychological, sociological,
ethical questions to start arising.
It makes me uncomfortable, but truth can be uncomfortable.
I get that a lot about being uncomfortable
talking about this.
Let me go back and just say one more empirical thing.
It doesn't matter which battery of tests you use.
So there are countless tests.
You can take any 12 of them at random, extract a G factor, and another 12 at random, and extract
a G factor.
And those G factors will be highly correlated, like over 0.9 with each other.
That's very...
So it is ubiquitous.
It doesn't depend on the content of the test
is what I'm trying to say.
It is general among all those tests of mental ability
and tests of mental...
You know, mental abilities include things like
G's playing poker.
Your skill at poker is not unrelated to G. Your skill at anything that requires reasoning
and thinking, anything, spelling, arithmetic, more complex things. This concept is ubiquitous.
And when you do batteries of test in different cultures you get the same thing. So this says something interesting
about the human mind that as a computer is designed to be general. So that means you can...
So it's not it's not easily made specialized. Meaning if you're going to be good at one thing,
If you're going to be good at one thing, Miyamoto Masashi has this quote, is an ancient warrior, famous for the book of five rings in the martial arts world.
And the quote goes, if you know the way broadly, you will see it in everything.
Meaning if you do one thing, it's going to generalize to everything. And that's an interesting thing about the human mind.
So that's what the G-factor reveals. Okay, so what's the difference if you can elaborate a little
bit further between IQ and G-factor just because it's a source of confusion for people?
And IQ is a score. People use the word IQ to mean intelligence,
but IQ has a more technical meeting
for people who work in the field.
And it's an IQ score, a score on a test
that estimates the G factor.
And the G factor is what's common among
all these tests of mental ability.
So if you think about, it's not a Venn diagram, but I guess you could make a Venn diagram out of it, but the G factor
would be really at the core, what's common to everything. And what IQ scores do is they
allow a rank order of people on the score. And this is what makes people uncomfortable.
This is where there's a lot of controversy about whether IQ tests are biased toward any
one group or another. And a lot of the answers to these questions are very clear, but they
also have a technical aspect of it. It's not so easy to explain.
Well, we'll talk about the fascinating and the difficult things about all of it. It's not so easy to to explain. Well, we'll talk about the fascinating and the
difficult things about all of this. But so by the way, when you say rank order, that means you get
a number and that means one person you can now compare. Like you could say that this other person
is more intelligent than me. Well, what you can say is IQ scores are interpreted really as percentiles.
So that if you have an IQ of 140 and somebody else has 70, the metric is such that you cannot say
the person with an IQ of 140 is twice as smart as a person with an IQ of 70, that would require a ratio scale within
absolute zero. Now you may think you know people with zero indulgence, but in
fact there is no absolute zero on an IQ scale. It's relative to other people. So
relative to other people, somebody with an IQ score of 140 is in the upper less than
1% whereas somebody with an IQ of 70 is two standard deviations below the mean.
That's a different percentile.
So it's similar to like in chess, you have an elo rating that's designed to rank order people.
So you can't say it's twice.
One person, if you're ilo ratings twice,
another person, I don't think your twice is good at chess.
It's not stable in that way,
but because it's very difficult to do these kinds of comparisons.
But so what can we say about the number itself?
Is that stable across tests and so on and so on?
There are a number of statistical properties of any test,
they're called psychometric properties.
You have validity, you have reliability, reliability,
there are many different kinds of reliability.
They all essentially measure stability.
And IQ tests are stable within an individual. kinds of reliability, they all essentially measure stability.
And IQ tests are stable within an individual.
There are some longitudinal studies where children were measured at age 11.
And again, when they were 70 years old and the two IQ scores are highly correlated with
each other, this comes from a fascinating study from Scotland. In the 1930s, some researchers
decided to get an IQ test on every single child age 11 in the whole country. And they did. And
those records were discovered in an old storeroom at the University of Edinburgh by a friend of mine, Ian Dairy,
who found the records, digitized them, and has done a lot of research on the people who
are still alive today from that original study, including brain imaging research, by the
way. It's a fascinating group of people who are studied.
Not to get ahead of the story,
but one of the most interesting things they found
is a very strong relationship
between IQ measured at age 11 and mortality.
So that in the 70 years later, they looked at the survival rates and they could
get death records from everybody.
And Scotland has universal healthcare for everybody.
And it turned out if you divide people by their age 11 IQ score into quartiles and then look at how many people are alive 70 years later.
I know this is in the book, I have the graph in the book, but there are essentially twice as
many people alive in the highest IQ quartile than in the lowest IQ quartile.
It's true in many and women.
Interesting. So it makes a big difference. Now why this is the case
is not so clear since everyone had access to health care. Well, there's a lot and we'll talk about
it, you know, just the the senses you used now could be explained by nature or nurture. We don't know.
Now there's a lot of science that starts to then dig in
and investigate that question.
Well, let me link on the IQ test.
How are the test design, IQ test design?
How do they work?
Maybe some examples for people who are not aware.
What makes a good IQ test question
that sneaks up on this G factor measure?
Well, your question is interesting because you want me to give examples of items that make good items.
And what makes a good item is not so much its content, but its empirical relationship to the total score that turns out to be valid by other means. Yeah, so for example
Let me give you an odd example from personality testing nice
So there's a personality test called the Minnesota Multiphasic personality inventory
MMPI been around for decades. I've heard about this test recently because of the Johnny Depp and Amber heard trial.
I don't know if you've been paying attention to that, but they have psychologist on the
stand and they were talking, apparently those psychologists did, again, I'm learning so
much from this trial, they have, different, a battery of tests to diagnose personality
disorders.
Apparently, there's that systematic way of doing so.
And the Minnesota one is one of the ones that there's the most science on.
There's a lot of great papers, which were all continuously cited on the stand, which
is fascinating to watch.
Sorry. A little bit of a tangent.
It's okay, I mean, this is interesting
because you're right, it's been around for decades.
There's a lot of scientific research
on the psychometric properties of the test,
including what it predicts with respect
to different categories of personality disorder.
But what I want to mention is the content
of the items on that test. All of the items
are essentially true false items. True or false, I prefer a shower to a bath. True or false,
I think Lincoln was a better president than Washington. What does that have to do? And the point is the content of these items,
nobody knows why these items in aggregate predict anything, but empirically they do. It's a
technique of choosing items for a test that is called dust bowl empiricism that the content doesn't matter, but for some reason,
when you get a criterion group of people with this disorder and you compare them to people
without that disorder, these are the items that distinguish irrespective of content.
It's a hard concept to grasp? Well, for sure, it's fascinating. But I
I consider myself part psychologist, because I love human robot interaction. And
that's a problem. Half of that problem is a psychology problem, because there's a
human. So designing these tests to get at the questions is the fascinating part.
Like, how do you get to, like, what is dust bowl in pericism referred to?
Does it refer to the final result?
Yeah.
So it's the test is dust bowl in pericism.
But how do you arrive at the battery of questions?
I presume
One of the things now again, I'm going to the excellent testimony in that trial
the explaining because they also the explain the tests
That a bunch of the questions are kind of
Make you forget that you're taking a test like it makes it very difficult for you to somehow figure out what you're supposed to answer.
Yes, it's called social desirability.
But we're getting a little far afield
because I only wanted to give that example
of dust bowl empiricism.
When we talk about the items on an IQ test,
many of those items in the dust bowl
empiricism method have no face validity. In other words, they don't look like
they measure anything. Yes. Whereas most intelligence tests, the items
actually look like they're measuring some mental ability. So here's here's one
of the. So you were bringing that up as an example
as what it is not.
Yes.
Got it.
Okay.
So I don't want to go too far afield on it.
A too far afield is actually one of the names of this podcast.
So I should mention that.
Far afield, yeah.
Far afield.
Yeah, so anyway, sorry.
So they feel the questions look like they pass the face validity test.
And some more than others.
So for example, let me give you a couple of things here.
If I, one of the subtests on a standard IQ test is general information.
Let me just think a little bit because I don't want to give you the actual item. But if I said how far is it between Washington, DC and Miami, Florida within 500 miles plus
or minus, you know, it's not a fact.
Most people memorize, but you know, something about geography, you say, well, I flew there
once, I know planes fly 500 miles.
You can kind of make an estimate.
But it also seems like it would be very cultural.
So there's that kind of general information.
Then there's vocabulary test.
What does
ragata mean? And I choose that word because that word was removed from the IQ test because people complained that disadvantaged people would not know that word just from their everyday
life. Okay. Here's another example from a different kind of subtest on what's
regatta by the way regatta is a I think I'm just a
saying competition a competition with votes not necessarily selling but
the competition yep yep I'm probably disadvantaged in that way okay
excellence so that was removed okay you're you're saying. Okay. So,
now here's another subtest. I'm going to repeat a string of numbers. And when I'm done, I want
you to repeat them back to me. Ready? Okay. Seven, four, two, eight, one, six. That's way too many.
Seven, four, two, eight, 1, 6. Okay.
You get the idea.
Now the actual test starts with a smaller number, you know, like two numbers, and then it
is people get it right.
You keep going, adding to the string of numbers until they can't do it anymore.
Okay.
But now try this.
I'm going to say some numbers, and when I'm done done I want you to repeat them to me backwards.
I quit.
Okay.
Now, so I gave you some examples of the kind of items on an IQ test.
General information.
I can't even remember all.
General information vocabulary, digit span, forward and digit span backward. Well, you said I can't even remember
them. That's a good question for me. What does memory have to do with you?
Let's hold that. All right. Let's just talk about these examples. Now, some of Some of those items seem very cultural,
and others seem less cultural.
Which ones do you think scores on which subtest are most highly correlated with the G factor?
Well, the D2-Advanser is less cultural.
Well, it turns out vocabulary is highly correlated. And it turns out that digit span backwards is highly correlated. How do you figure? Now you have decades of research
to answer the question, how do you figure? Right. So now there's like good research that gives you intuition about what kind of questions
get at it. Just like there's something I've done, I've actually used for research just to
make a autonomous vehicle like whether humans are paying attention, there's a body of literature that does like end back tests, for example, we have to put workload on the brain to do
recall memory recall, and that helps you kind of put some work onto the brain while the
person is doing some other tasks and does some interesting research with that. But that's loading the memory. So there's like research around
stably what that means about the human mind. And here you're saying, recall backwards is
a good protector transformation. Yeah, so you have to do some, like you have to load that into your brain and not just
remember it, but do something with it.
Right.
Here's another example of a different kind of test called the Hick Paradigm, and it's
not verbal at all.
It's a little box, and there are a series of lights arranged in a semi-circle at the top of the box, and then there's a home
button that you press. And when one of the lights goes on, there's a button next
to each of those lights. You take your finger off the home button and you just press
the button next to the light that goes on. And so it's a very simple reaction time.
Like goes on as quick as you can, you press the button and you get a reaction time.
From the moment you lift your finger off the button, when you press the button
with where the light is, that reaction time doesn't really correlate with IQ very much.
But if you change the instructions
and you say three lights are gonna come on simultaneously,
I want you to press the button next to the light
that's furthest from the other two.
So maybe lights one and two go on and light six goes on.
Simultaneously, you take your finger off
and you would press the button by light six goes on simultaneously you take your finger off and you would press the button
by light six. That's that reaction time to a more complex task. It's not really hard. Almost
everybody gets it all right, but your reaction time to that is highly correlated with the G factor.
This is fascinating. So reaction time says the temporal aspect to this. So what role does
it have? Speed of processing. It's the speed of processing.
Is this also true for ones that take longer like 5, 10, 30 seconds?
It's time part of the measure with some pieces. Yes.
And that is why some of the best IQ tests have a time limit
because if you have no time limit, people can do better. Yeah, but it doesn't, it doesn't
distinguish among people that well. So that adding the time element is important. So speed of
information processing and reaction time is a measure of speed of information processing turns out to be related to the G factor.
But the G factor only accounts for maybe half or some amount on the test performance. For example, I get pretty bad test anxiety. Like I was never, I mean, I just don't enjoy tests. I enjoy going back
into my cave and working it. Like I've always enjoyed homework way more than tests.
No matter how hard the homework is because I can go back to the cave and hide away and
think deeply. There's something about being watched and having a time limit that really
makes me anxious and
I could just see the mind not operating optimally at all.
But you're saying underneath there, there's still a g-factor, there's still a question.
There's no question.
Boy.
And if you get anxious taking a test, many people say, oh, I didn't do well because I'm
anxious.
Yeah.
You know, I hear that a lot.
Yeah.
Well, fine, if you're really anxious during the test, the score will be a bad estimate
of your G factor.
It doesn't mean the G factor isn't there.
That's right.
And by the way, standardized tests like the SAT, they're essentially intelligence tests.
They are highly G-loaded.
Now, the people who make the SAT don't want to mention that.
They have enough trouble justifying standardized testing, but the call it an intelligence test
is really beyond the pale.
But in fact, it's so highly correlated because it's a reasoning test.
SAT is a reasoning test, a verbal reasoning,
mathematical reasoning.
And if it's a reasoning test,
it has to be related to G.
But if people go in and take a standardized test,
whether it's an IQ test or the SAT,
and they happen to be sick that day with 102 fever,
the score is not going to be a good estimate of their
G. If they retake the test when they're not anxious or less anxious or don't have a
fever, the score will go up and that will be a better estimate. But you can't say their
G factor increased between the two tests.
Well, it's interesting.
So the question is, how wide of a battery of tests
is required to estimate the G factor well?
Because I'll give you as my personal example.
I took SAT and I think it was called the ACT
where I was to also, I took SAT many times.
Every single time I got a perfect math
and verbal, the time limit on the verbal
made me very anxious.
I did not, I mean, part of it, I didn't speak English very well, but honestly, it was like
you're supposed to remember stuff and like, I was so anxious and like, as I'm reading,
I'm sweating, I can't, you know that like, that feeling you have when you're reading a book and you just read a
page and you know nothing about what you've read because you zoned out, that's the same
feeling of like, I can't, I have to, you're like, nope, read and understand and that anxiety
is like, and you start seeing like the typography versus the content of the words like that was I don't it's interesting because I know that what they're measuring I could see being correlated with something but that anxiety or some aspect of the performance, sure plays a factor.
And I wonder how you sneak up in a stable way.
I mean, this is a broader discussion,
but that's like standardized testing,
how you sneak up, how you get at the fact
that I'm super anxious and still nevertheless
measure some aspect of my intelligence.
I wonder, I don't know, I don't know if you can say to that, that time limit sure is a pain. Well, let me say this, there are two ways to approach
the very real problem that you say that some people just get anxious or not good test takers.
By the way, part of testing is you know the answer, you can figure out the answer, or you can't.
Right. If you don't know the answer, there are many reasons you don't know the answer
at that particular moment. You may have learned it once and forgotten it. You may, it may be on the tip of your tongue
and you just can't get it because you're anxious about the time limit. You may never have learned it. You may have been exposed to it, but it was too complicated and you couldn't learn it.
I mean, there are all kinds of reasons here. But for an individual to interpret your scores
as an individual, whoever is interpreting the score has to take into account various things that would affect your individual score
And that's why decisions about college admission or anything else where tests are used are hardly ever the only
criterion
To make a decision and I think people are college admissions letting go of that very much.
Oh, yes.
But what does that even mean?
Because is it possible to design standardized tests that do get that are useful to college
admissions?
Well, they already exist.
The SAT is highly correlated with many aspects of success at college.
Here's the problem.
So maybe you could speak to this.
The correlation across the population versus individuals.
So our criminal justice system is designed to make sure,
well, it's still there's tragic cases where innocent people go go to jail, but you try
to avoid that.
And the same way with testing, it just it would suck for an SAT to misgenius.
Yes.
And it's possible, but it's statistically unlikely. So it really comes down to do which piece of information
maximizes your decision-making ability.
So if you just use high school grades, it's okay,
but you will miss some people who just don't do well
in high school, but who are actually pretty smart,
smart enough to be bored, silly in high school,
and they don't care, and their high school GPA isn't that good.
So you will miss them.
In the same sense that somebody who could be very able
and ready for college just doesn't do well on their SAT.
This is why you make decisions with taking in a variety of information.
The other thing I wanted to say, I talked about when you make a decision for an individual. Statistically, for groups, there are many people
who have a disparity between their math score and their verbal score.
That disparity, or the other way around, that disparity is called tilt.
The score is tilted one way or the other.
And that tilt has been studied empirically to see what that predicts. And in fact,
you can't make predictions about college success based on tilt. And mathematics is a good
example. There are many people, especially non-native speakers of English who come to this
country, take the SATs, do very well on the math and not so well on the verbal.
Well, if they're applying to a math program, the professors there who are making the decision or
the admissions officers, don't wait so much the score on verbal, especially if it's a non-native
speaker. Well, so yeah, you have to try to in the admission process, bring in the context.
But non-native isn't really the problem. I mean, that was part of the problem for me.
But it's the the anxiety was, which it's interesting. It's interesting.
Boy, reducing yourself down to numbers, but it's still true.
It's still the truth.
Well, it's a painful, that same anxiety that led me to be to struggle with the SAT, verbal
tests is still within me in always life.
So maybe that's not anxiety. Maybe that's something, you know,
like personality is also pretty stable. Personality is stable. Personality does impact the way
you navigate life. Yeah. There's no question. Yeah. And we should say that the G-Factor Intelligence is not just the
boss some kind of number on a paper. It's also has to do with how you navigate life, how
easy life is for you in this very complicated world. So personalities all tied into that and some
and some some deep fundamental way.
But now you've hit the key point about why we even want to study intelligence.
And personality, I think, to a lesser extent, but that's my interest.
I was more on intelligence.
I went to graduate school and wanted to study personality,
but that's kind of another story how I got kind of shifted from personality research
over to intelligence research
Because it's not just a number intelligence. It's not just an IQ score
It's not just an SAT score. It's what those numbers reflect
About your ability to navigate every day life
It has been said that life is one long intelligence test. And who can't
relate to that? And if you doubt, see, another problem here is a lot of critics of
intelligence research and intelligence testing tend to be academics who buy in large are pretty smart people.
And pretty smart people buy in large have enormous difficulty understanding what the world
is like for people with IQs of 80 or 75.
It is a completely different everyday experience. Even at a school course of 85, 90, you know, there's a popular television
program, Judge Judy, with Judge Judy deals with every day people with every day problems
and you can see the full range of problem-solving ability demonstrated there. And sometimes she does it for laughs, but it really isn't funny because
people who are, there are people who are very limited in their life,
navigation, let alone success by having, by not having good reasoning skills, which cannot be taught.
We know this, by the way, because there are many efforts.
You know, the United States military,
which excels at training people.
I mean, I don't know that there's a better organization
in the world for training diverse people.
And they won't take people with IQs under,
I think 83 is the cut off,
because they have found you, they
are unable to train people with lower IQs to do jobs in the military.
So, one of the things that G factor has to do with is learning?
Absolutely.
Some people learn faster than others.
Some people learn more than others.
Now, faster, by the way, is not necessarily better, as long as you get to the same place
eventually.
But you know, there are professional schools that want students who can learn the fastest
because they can learn more, or learn deeper, or all kinds of ideas about why you select people with the highest
scores. And there's nothing funnier, by the way, to listen to a bunch of academics
complain about the concept of intelligence and intelligence testing. And then you go
to a faculty meeting where they're discussing who to hire among the applicants. And all they
talk about is how smart the person is.
We'll get to that.
We'll sneak up to that in different ways, but there's something about reducing a person
to a number that in part is grounded to the person's genetics that makes people very
uncomfortable.
But nobody does that.
Nobody in the field actually does that. That is a worry that is a worry like,
well, I don't wanna call it a conspiracy theory.
I mean, it's a legitimate worry,
but it just doesn't happen.
Now, I had a professor in graduate school
who was the only person I ever knew who
considered the students only by their test scores.
Yes.
And later in his life, he kind of backed off that.
But
Well, let me ask you this. So we'll jump around. I'll come back to a book.
I tend to, I've had like political discussions with
people and actually my friend Michael Malis, he's an anarchist, I disagree with him on
basically everything except the fact that love is a beautiful thing in this world.
And he says this test about left versus right,
whatever it doesn't matter what the test is,
but he believes the question is,
do you believe that some people are better than others?
The question is ambiguous.
Do you believe some people are better than others? And to me, sort of the immediate
answer is no. It's a poetic question. It's ambiguous question, right? Like, you know,
people want to maybe the temptation to ask better at what, better at like sports, so
on. No. To me, I stand with the sort of
the Fahni documents of this country,
which is all men are created equal.
There's a basic humanity.
And there's something about tests of intelligence,
just knowing that some people are different,
like the science of intelligence that shows that
some people are different, like the science of intelligence that shows that some people are genetically
in some stable way across a lifetime have a greater intelligence than others,
makes people feel like some people are better than others and that makes them very uncomfortable.
And I maybe you can speak to that.
Like, the fact that some people are more intelligent
than others in a way that's,
cannot be compensated through education,
through anything you do in life.
What do we do with that?
Okay, there's a lot there.
We haven't really talked about the genetics of it yet, but you are correct in that it is
my interpretation of the data that genetics has a very important influence on the G factor.
And this is controversial, and we can talk about it. But if you think that genetics, that genes are deterministic, are always deterministic,
that leads to kind of the worry that you expressed.
But we know now, in the 21st century, that many genes are not deterministic, that are
probabilistic, meaning that their gene expression
can be influenced.
Now, whether they're influenced only
by other biological variables or other genetic variables
or environmental or cultural variables,
that's where the controversy comes in.
And we can discuss that in more detail if you like. But to go to the
question about better people better, there's zero evidence that smart people are better
with respect to important aspects of life, like honesty, even like ability. I'm sure you know many very intelligent people who are not terribly likeable or terribly kind or terribly honest.
Is there something you be said? So one of the things I've recently read for the second time, I guess that's what the word reread means.
The rise and fall of the third Reich, which is I think the best telling of the rise and
fall of Hitler.
And one of the interesting things about the people that, how should I say it?
Justified or maybe propped up the ideas that Hitler put forward
is the fact that they were extremely intelligent.
They were in the intellectual class.
They were like, it was obvious that they thought very deeply and rationally about the world. So what I would like to say is one of the things that shows to me is some of the worst atrocities
in the history of humanity have been committed by very intelligent people.
So that means that intelligence doesn't make you a good person.
I wonder if there's a G factor for intelligence.
I wonder if there's a G factor for goodness.
You know, they need to get an evil.
Of course, that's probably harder to measure because that's just subjective thing.
What it means to be good. And even the idea of evil is
deeply uncomfortable thing, because how do we know?
By this independent, whatever it is, it's independent of intelligence. So I agree with you about that.
But let me say this, I have also asserted my belief that more intelligence is better than less.
It doesn't mean more intelligent people are better people,
but all things being equal would you like to be smarter or less smart?
So if I had a pill, I have two pills. I said, this one will make you smarter, this one will make you dumber.
Which one would you like?
Are there any circumstances under which you would choose to be dumber? Well, let me ask you this
That's a very nuanced and interesting question
You know, there's been books written about this right?
Now we'll return to the hard questions the interesting questions, but let me ask about human happiness
this intelligence Lead to happiness. No.
No.
So, okay, so back to the pill then.
So why, one would you take the pill?
So you said IQ 80, 90, 100, 110,
you start going to the quartiles and is it obvious? Isn't there diminishing
returns and then it starts becoming negative?
This is an empirical question. And so that I have advocated in many forums,
more research on enhancing the G factor.
Right now, there's, there have been many claims
about enhancing intelligence.
With you mentioned the endback training,
it was a big deal a few years ago, it doesn't work.
Data is very clear, it does not work.
You know,
or doing like memory tests, like training and so on.
Yeah, it may give you a better memory in the short run, but it doesn't impact your G-factor.
It was very popular a couple of decades ago that the idea that listening to Mozart could
make you more intelligent. There was a paper
published on this with somebody I knew published this paper. Intelligence researchers never
believed it for a second. There have been hundreds of studies, all the meta-analyses, all the
summaries and so on. There's nothing to it. Nothing to it at all. But wouldn't it be something? Wouldn't it be world-shaking if you could
take the normal distribution of intelligence, which we haven't really talked about yet, but IQ
scores and the G-factors thought to be a normal distribution, and shift it to the right so that everybody is smarter.
Even a half a standard deviation would be world-shaking because there are many social problems,
many, many social problems that are exacerbated by people with lower ability to reason stuff out and navigate every day life.
So I wonder if there's a threshold. So maybe I would push back and say universal shifting
of the normal distribution may not be the optimal way of shifting. Maybe it's better to whatever the asymmetric
kind of distributions is like really pushing the lower up versus trying to make the people
at the average more intelligent.
So you're saying that if in fact there was some way to increase G, let's just call it
metaphorically a pill, an IQ pill. We should
only give it to people at the lower end. No, it's just intuitively, I can see that life becomes
easier at the lower end. Yes. If it's increased, it becomes less and less, it is a empirical scientific
question, but it becomes less and less obvious to me
that more intelligence is better.
At the high end, it's not because it would make life easier, but it would make whatever
problems you're working on more solvable. And if you are working on artificial intelligence, there's a tremendous potential
for that to improve society. I understand. So at that whatever problems you're working on,
yes, but there's also the problem of the human condition. There's love, there's fear, and all of
those beautiful things that sometimes if you're
good at solving problems, you're going to create more problems for yourself.
It's, I'm not exactly sure.
So ignorance is bliss is a thing.
So there might be a place, there might be a sweet spot of intelligence, given your environment,
given your personality, all of those kinds of things.
And that becomes less beautifully complicated, the more and more intelligent you become.
But that's a, that's a, that's a question for literature enough for science, perhaps.
Well, imagine this, imagine there wasn't IQ pill.
Yeah.
And it was developed by a private company.
And they are willing to sell it to you.
And whatever price they put on it, you are willing to sell it to you. And whatever price they put on it,
you are willing to pay it because you would like to be smarter.
But just before they give you a pill,
they give you a disclaimer for a design.
Yes.
Don't hold us...
You understand that this pill has no guarantee
that your life is going to be better,
and in fact, it could be worse.
Well, yes, that's how lawyers work, but I would love for science to answer the question
to try to protect if your life is going to be better or worse when you become more less intelligent.
It's a fascinating question about what is the sweet spot for the human condition?
Some of the things we see as bugs might be actually features may be crucial to our
Overall happiness is our limitations might lead to more happiness than less
But again more intelligence is better at the low end. That's, that's something that's less arguable and and and and fascinating if possible to increase. But you know there's virtually no
research that's based on a neuroscience approach to solving that problem. All
the solutions that have been proposed to solve that problem or do a mealier rate, that problem are essentially based on the blank slate assumption
that enriching the environment, removing barriers, all good things, by the way. I'm not against
any of those things, but there's no empirical evidence that they're going to improve the general
reasoning ability or make people more employable.
Have you read flowers of Aragonon?
Yes.
That's to the question of intelligence and happiness.
There are many profound aspects of that story.
It was a film that was very good.
The film was called Charlie for the younger people who are listening to this,
you might be able to stream it on Netflix or something, but it was a story about
a person with very low IQ who underwent a surgical procedure in the brain and he slowly became a genius. And the tragedy of the story is the effect was temporary.
It's a fascinating story, really.
That goes in contrast to the basic human experience that each of us individually have,
but it raises the question of the full range of people who might be able to be given different levels of intelligence.
You've mentioned the normal distribution. So let's talk about it. There's a book called
the Belkurve written in 1994 written by psychologist Richard Herndstein and political scientist Charles Murray.
and political scientist Charles Murray. Why was this book so controversial?
This is a fascinating book.
I know Charles Murray.
I've had many conversations with him.
Yeah, what is the book about?
The book is about the importance of intelligence in everyday life.
That's what the book is about.
It's an empirical book.
It has statistical analyses of very large databases that show that essentially IQ scores
or they're equivalent are correlated to all kinds of social problems and social benefits. And that in itself is not where the controversy
about that book came. The controversy was about one chapter in that book. And that is
a chapter about the average difference in mean scores between black Americans and white
Americans. And these are the terms that
were used in the book at the time and are still used to some extent. And historically, or really for It has been observed that disadvantaged groups score on average lower than Caucasians on
academic tests, tests of mental ability, and especially on IQ tests.
And the difference is about a standard deviation, which is about 15 points, which is a substantial difference.
In the book,
Herndtstein and Murray in this one chapter assert clearly and unambiguously that whether this
average difference is due to genetics or not, they are agnostic. They don't know. Moreover,
they assert they don't care because you wouldn't treat anybody differently knowing that if there
was a genetic component or not, because that's a group average finding. Every individual has to be
treated as an individual. You can't make any assumption about what
that person's intellectual ability might be from the fact of an average group difference.
They're very clear about this. Nonetheless, people took away, I'm going to choose my words carefully,
because I have a feeling that many critics didn't actually read the book.
They took away that Herndtstein and Murray were saying that blacks are genetically inferior.
That was the take-home message. And if they weren't saying it, they were implying it because they had a chapter that discussed this empirical observation of a difference. And isn't this horrible?
And so the reaction to that book was incendiary. What do we know about from that book and the research beyond about race differences and intelligence.
It's still the most incendiary topic in psychology. Nothing has changed that.
Anybody who even discusses it is easily called a racist, just for discussing it.
It's become fashionable to find racism in any discussion like this. It's
unfortunate. The short answer to your question is there's been very little actual research on this
topic since 19... since the Belkhar... since the Belkary, even before, this really became incendiary in 1969 with an
article published by an educational psychologist named Arthur Jensen. Let's just take a minute and
go back to that to see the bell curve in a little bit more historical perspective. Arthur Johnson was a educational psychologist
at UC Berkeley.
I knew him as well.
And in 1969 or 1968,
the Harvard Educational Review asked him
to do a review article on the early childhood education
programs that were designed to raise the IQs of minority students. This was before the federally funded Head Start program.
Head Start had not really gotten underway at the time Jensen undertook his review of what were a number of demonstration programs.
And these demonstration programs were for young children who were on kindergarten age,
and they were specially designed to be cognitively stimulating to provide lunches, do all the things that people thought would minimize this average gap of
intelligence tests. There was a strong belief among virtually all psychologists that
the cause of the gap was unequal opportunity due to racism, do all, you know, all negative things in the society. And if you could compensate
for this, the gap would go away. So early childhood education back then was called literally
compensatory education. Jensen looked at these programs. He was an empirical guy. He understood psychometrics. And he wrote a, it was over a hundred page article
detailing these programs and the flaws
in their research design.
Some of the programs reported IQ gains
on average five points,
but a few reported 10, 20 and even 30 point gains.
One was called the miracle in Milwaukee. That
investigator went to jail, ultimately, for fabricating data. But the point is that Jensen
wrote an article that said, look, the opening sentence of his article is classic. The opening
sentence is, I may not quoted exactly right, but but it's we have tried compensatory education and it has failed.
And he showed that these games were essentially nothing. You couldn't really document empirically
any gains at all from these really earnest efforts to increase IQ. But he went a step further, a fateful step further.
He said, not only have these efforts failed,
but because they have had essentially no impact,
we have to re-examine our assumption
that these differences are caused by environmental things
that we can address with education.
We need to consider a genetic influence, whether there's a genetic influence on this group
difference.
So you said that this is one of the more controversial works.
I think it's the most infamous paper in all of psychology.
I would go on to say, because in 1969, the genetic data was very skimpy on this question.
Skimpy and controversial, it's always been controversial, but it was even skimpy and
controversial.
It's kind of a long story that I go into a little bit in more detail in the book Neuroscience
of Intelligence.
But to say he was vilified as an understatement, I mean, he couldn't talk at the American Psychological
Association without bomb threats clearing the lecture hall.
Campus security watched him all the time.
They opened his mail.
He had to retreat to a different address. This was one of the earliest kinds, this is before the internet
and kind of internet is social media mobs, but it was that intense. And I have written
that overnight after the publication of this article, all intelligence research became radioactive.
Nobody wanted to talk about it.
And then it didn't, nobody was doing more research.
And then the bell curve came along.
And the Jensen controversy was dying down.
I have stories that Jensen told me about
his interaction with the Nixon White House on this issue. I mean, it was, this was like
a really big deal. It was some unbelievable stories, but, you know, he told me this,
so I kind of believe these stories. Nonetheless, 25 years later, 25 years later. 25 years later. All the silence basically saying, you know, nobody wants
to do this kind of research. There's so much pressure, so much attack against this kind
of research. And here's sort of a bold, stupid, crazy people that decide to dive right back
in. I wonder how much discussion that was,
do we include this chapter or not? Murray has said they discussed it and they felt
they should include it and they were very careful in the way they wrote it, which did them no good.
So as a matter of fact, when the bell Curve came out, it was so controversial.
I got a call from a television show called Nightline, it was with a broadcaster called
Ted Coppel, who had this evening show, I think it was on late at night, talked about news,
it was a straight up news thing. And a producer called an astrophil,
if I would be on it, to talk about the bell curve.
And I said, you know, she asked me what I thought
about the bell curve as a book.
And I said, look, it's a very good book.
It talks about the role of intelligence in society.
And she said, no, no, what do you think
about the chapter on race? That's what we want you to talk about.
I remember this conversation. I said, well, she said, what would you say if you were on TV? And I said, well, what I would say is that it's not at all clear if there's any genetic component to intelligence, any
differences, but if there were a strong genetic component, that would be a good thing.
And you know, a complete silence on the other end of the phone.
And she said, well, what do you mean?
And I said, well, if it's the more genetic any difference is,
the more it's biological.
And if it's biological, we can figure out how to fix it.
I see. That's interesting.
She said, would you say that on television?
Yes.
I said, no.
And so that was the end of that. So that's for more like
biology is
within the reach of science and the environment is a public policy, social and all those kinds of
things. It's it from your perspective, whichever one,
you think is more amenable to solutions in the short term is the one that excites you.
But you're saying that it's good. The truth of genetic differences, no matter what, between groups, is a painful, harmful, potentially,
potentially dangerous thing.
So let me ask you to this question.
Well, there's Bell Curve or any research on race differences.
Can that be used to increase the amount of racism in the world?
Can that be used to increase the amount of hate in the world? Do you think about this kind of stuff?
I have thought about this a lot, not as a scientist, but as a person.
And my sense is there is such enormous reservoirs of hate and racism that have nothing to do with scientific knowledge of veto power over what scientists study. If you think that the difference is, and by the way, virtually no one disagrees that there
are differences in scores, it's all about what causes them and how to fix it.
So if you think this is a cultural problem, then you must ask the problem, what do you want to change anything
about the culture? Or are you okay with the culture because you don't feel it's appropriate
to change a person's culture? So are you okay with that? And the fact that that may lead
to disadvantages in school achievement? It's a question. If you think it's environmental, what are the environmental parameters that can be fixed?
I'll tell you one, lead from gasoline in the atmosphere, lead in paint, lead in water.
That's an environmental toxin that society has the means to eliminate, and they should.
Yeah, just to sort of try and define some insights
and conclusion to this very difficult topic,
is there been research environment versus genetics,
nature versus nurture on this question of race differences?
There is not, no one wants to do this research.
It's, first of all, it's hard research to do.
Second of all, it's a minefield.
No one wants to spend their career on it.
Tenured people don't want to do it, let alone students.
The way I talk about it, well, before I tell you the way, I talk about it.
I want to say one more thing about Jensen.
He was once asked by a journalist straight out, are you a racist?
His answer was very interesting.
His answer was, I've thought about that a lot, and I've concluded it doesn't matter.
Now I know what he meant by this.
The guts to say that, wow.
He was a very unusual person.
I think he had a touch of Asperger syndrome to tell you the truth, because I saw him in
many circumstances.
He would be canceled on Twitter immediately with essence.
Yeah, but what he meant was he had a hypothesis.
And with respect to group differences, he called it the default hypothesis.
He said, whatever factors affect individual intelligence
are likely the same factors that affect group differences.
It was the default, but it was a hypothesis.
It should be tested.
And if it turned out empirical tests
didn't support the hypothesis,
he was happy to move on to something else
he was absolutely committed to that scientific ideal that that it's an empirical question
we should look at it and let's see what happens. The scientific method cannot be racist from his perspective it doesn't matter matter what the scientists, if they, if they
follow the scientific method, it doesn't matter what they believe.
And if they are biased and they consciously or unconsciously bias the data, other people
will come along to replicate it, they will fail.
And the process over time will work.
So let me push back on this idea, because psychology to me is full of gray areas.
And what I've observed about psychology, even replication crisis aside, is that
something about the media, something about journalism,
something about the virality of ideas in the public sphere.
They misinterpret, they take up things from studies, willfully or from ignorance misinterpret
findings and tell narratives around that.
I personally believe for me, I'm not saying that broadly about size, but for me, it's
my responsibility to anticipate the ways in which findings will be misinterpreted.
So I've had, I thought about this a lot because I published papers on semi-autonomous vehicles and
those you know cars people dying cars
There's people that've written me letters saying emails
Nobody writes letters. I wish they did
That I've blood in my hands because of things that I would say positive or negative this consequences. In the same way,
when you're a research of intelligence, I'm sure you might get emails or at least people might
believe that finding your study is going to be used by a large number of people to increase the
amount of hate in the world. I think there's some responsibility on scientists, but for me, I think there's a great responsibility
to anticipate the ways things will be misinterpreted.
And there, you have to, first of all, decide whether you want to say a thing at all, do the
study at all, publish the study at all, and to the words with which you explain it.
I find this on Twitter a lot, actually, which is when I write a tweet, and I'm usually just
doing so innocently, I'll write it, it takes me like five seconds to write it, or whatever,
thirty seconds to write it, and I'll think, like close my eyes open and try to see how will the world interpret this? Like what are
the ways in which this will be misinterpreted? And I'll sometimes adjust that tweet to see
like, yeah, so in my mind, it's clear, but that's it because it's my mind from which this tweet came. We have to think in a fresh mind that sees this
and it's spread across a large number of other minds.
How will the interpretation morph?
I mean, for a tweet, it's a silly thing.
It doesn't matter.
But for a scientific paper and study and finding,
I think it matters. So I don't know. Well, I don't know what
you're thoughts about on that because maybe for Justin, the data is there. What do you
want me to do? This is a scientific process has been carried out. If you think the data
was polluted by bias, do other studies that reveal the bias, but the data is there. I'm not a poet,
I'm not a literary writer, like, what do you want me to do? I'm just presenting you the data.
What do you think on that spectrum? What's the role of a scientist?
The reason I do podcasts, the reason I write books for the public is to explain what I think the
data mean and what I think the data don't mean. I don't do very much on Twitter other than to
retweet references to papers. I don't think it's my role to explain these because they're complicated, they're nuanced. But when you decide not to
do a scientific study because you're, or not to publish a result, because you're afraid
the result, could be, could be harmful or insensitive. That's not an unreasonable thought, and people will make different conclusions and decisions
about that.
I wrote about this.
I wrote, I'm the editor of a journal called Intelligence, which publishes scientific papers.
Sometimes we publish papers on group differences.
Those papers sometimes are controversial.
These papers are written for a scientific audience.
They're not written for the Twitter audience.
I don't promote them very much on Twitter.
But in a scientific paper, you have now Jews, your words carefully also because those papers are picked up by non-scientists
by writers of various kinds and you have to be available to discuss
what you're saying and what you're not saying
sometimes
You are successful at having a good conversation like we are today that doesn't start out
pre-jorative. Other times I have been asked to participate in debates where my
role would be to justify race science. Well you can see you start out, you know,
and that was a BBC request that I had that I received. I have so much. It's a love hate relationship, mostly hate with these shallow
journalism organizations. So they would want to use you as a kind of
inner debate setting to communicate as to like, there is raised differences between
groups and make that into debate.
Yes.
And put you in a role of justifying
racism.
Justifying what they're asking me to do.
Of course, it's like educating about this field of the science of intelligence.
Yeah.
I want to say one more thing before we get off the the the normal distribution.
You also ask me what is the science after the bell curve?
And the short answer is there's not much new work, but whatever work there is supports
the idea that there still are group differences.
It's arguable whether those differences have diminished at all or not.
And there is still a major problem in underperformance for school achievement.
For many disadvantaged and minority students, and there is so far is no way to fix it.
What do we do with this information?
Is this now a task?
Now we'll talk about the future on the neuroscience and the biology side,
but in terms of this information as a society in the public policy, in the political space,
in the social space, what do we do with this information? I've thought a lot about this.
The first step is to have people interested in policy understand what the data actually show to pay attention to
intelligence data. You can read policy papers about education and using your word
processor you can search for the word intelligence. You can search a 20,000
word document in a second and find out the word intelligence does not appear anywhere in most discussions
about what to do about achievement gaps.
I'm not talking about test gaps,
I'm talking about actual achievement gaps in schools
which everyone agrees is a problem.
The word intelligence doesn't appear among educators.
That's fascinating.
As a matter of fact, in California, there has been tremendous controversy about recent attempts
to revise the curriculum for math in high schools.
And we had a Stanford professor of education who was running this review, assert there's
no such thing as talent of a mathematical talent. And she wanted
to get rid of the advanced classes in math because, you know, not everyone could do that.
Now, of course, this has been very controversial. They've retreated somewhat. But the idea
that a university professor was in charge of this who believes that there's no talent that it doesn't exist.
This is rather shocking. Let alone the complete absence of intelligence data. By the way,
let me tell you something about what the intelligence data show. Let's take race out of it. Even though the origins of these studies were a long time ago,
I'm blocking on the name of the report. The Coleman report was a famous report about education,
and they measured all kinds of variables about schools, about teachers, and they looked at
schools about teachers, and they looked at academic achievement as an outcome. And they found
the most predictive variables of education outcome, where the variables the student brought
with him or her into the school, essentially their ability. And that when you combine the school and the teacher variables together, the
quality of the school, the funding of the school, the quality of the teachers, their education,
you put all the teacher and school variables together, it barely accounted for 10% of the
variants.
And this has been replicated now. You know, so the best research we have shows that school variables and teacher variables
together account for about 10% of student academic achievement.
Now, you want to have some policy on improving academic achievement.
How much money do you want to put into teacher education?
How much money do you want to put into the quality of the school administration?
You know who you can ask?
You can ask the Gates Foundation because they spend a tremendous amount of money doing
that.
And they, at the end of it, because they're measurement people, they want to know the
data, they found it had no impact at all.
And they've kind of pulled out of that kind of program.
So boy, let me ask you, this is me talking, but there's just the two of us.
Or just the two of us, but I'm gonna say some funny and ridiculous thing.
So you're surely not approving of it.
But there's a movie called Clerks.
I've seen it.
I've seen it.
There's a funny scene in there where a lovely couple are talking about the number of
previous sexual partners they had.
And the woman says that I believe she just had a handful,
like two or three or something like that, sexual partners,
but then she also mentioned that she was that called Flasia.
What's the scientific, but she went, you know,
gave a blowjob to 37 guys, I believe it is. And so that has to do with
the truth. So sometimes knowing the truth can get in the way of a successful relationship
of love of some of the human flourishing. And that seems to me that's at the core here that facing some kind of
truth that's not able to be changed is makes it difficult to sort of is limiting as opposed
to empowering. That's the concern. If you sort of test for intelligence and lay the data out,
it feels like you will give up on certain people.
You will sort of start bidding people,
it's like, well, this person is like,
let's focus on the average people,
or let's focus on the very intelligent people.
That's the concern.
And there's a kind of intuition
that if we just don't measure,
and we don't use that data,
that we retreat everybody equal
and give everybody equal opportunity.
If we have the data in front of us,
we're likely to misdistribute
the amount of sort of attention we allocate resources we allocate
To people that's that's probably the concern
It's a realistic concern
And it but I think it's a misplaced concern if you want to fix the problem
If you want to fix the problem you have to know what the problem. Yes
Now let me let me tell you this.
Let's go back to the bell curve.
Not the bell curve, but the normal distribution.
Yes.
16% of the population on average has an IQ under 85, which means they're very hard.
If you have an IQ under 85, it's very hard to find gainful employment at a salary that sustains you at least
minimally in modern life. Okay, not impossible, but it's very
difficult. 16% of the population of the United States is about
51 or 52 million people with IQs under 85. This is not a small issue.
14 million children have IQs under 85. Is this something we want to ignore?
Does this have any, what is the Venn diagram between, you know, when you have people with IQs under 85 and you have achievement in school or achievement in life?
There's a lot of overlap there.
This is why to go back to the IQ pill, if there were a way to shift that curve toward the higher end, that would have a big impact.
If I could maybe before we talk about the impact on life and so on, some of the criticisms
of the Bell curve, so Stephen J. Goode wrote that the Bell curve rests on four incorrect
assumptions.
It would be just interesting to get your thoughts
on the four assumptions, which are intelligence
must be reducible to a single number,
intelligence must be capable of rank ordering people
in the linear order, intelligence must be primarily
genetically based, and intelligence must be
essentially immutable.
Maybe not as criticisms, but as thoughts about intelligence.
Oh, yeah, we could spend a lot of time on him.
As soon as you got to school, yeah.
He wrote that in what, about 1985, 1984,
his views were overtly political, not scientific. He was a scientist, but his views on this were
overtly political, and I would encourage people listening to this if they really want to understand
his criticisms. They should just Google what he had to say, and Google the scientific reviews of his book, the mismeasure of
man, and they will take these statements apart.
They were wrong, not only were they wrong, but when he asserted in his first book that
there was no biological basis essentially to IQ. By the time the second edition came around,
there were studies of MRI,
MRIs of showing that brain size, brain volume
were correlated to IQ scores,
which he declined to put in his book.
So, I'm learning a lot today.
I didn't know, I didn't know the,
actually the extent of his work.
I was just using a few little snippets of criticism.
That's interesting.
So there's a battle here here at a book,
Miss Measure of Man,
that's missing a lot of the scientific grounding.
His book is highly popular in colleges today.
You can find it in any college or bookstore
under assigned reading.
It's highly popular.
The Miss Measure of Man.
Yes, highly influential. Can you speak to the Ms. Measure
Measure, man? I'm undereducated about this. So what is this the book basically
criticizing the ideas in the book? Yeah, yeah, where those four things came from.
And it is really a book that was really taken apart, point by point by a
number of people who actually understood
the data. And he didn't care. Yeah. He didn't care. He didn't.
It's a politically. Listen, because this is such a sensitive topic, like I said, I believe
the impact of the work as it is misinterpreted has to be considered because it's not just going
to be scientific discourse, it's going to be political discourse, there's going to be debates,
there's going to be politically motivated people that will use messages in each direction,
make something like the bulk of the enemy or the support for one's racist beliefs.
So I think you have to consider that, but it's not exactly on Nietzsche to anticipate Hitler.
So, or how his ideas will be misinterpreted and used for evil.
But there's a balance there.
So, I understand.
This is really interesting.
I didn't know.
Is there any criticism of the book you find compelling or interesting or
challenging to you from a scientific perspective?
There were factual criticisms about the nature of the statistics that were used, the statistical
analyses.
These were more technical criticisms.
And they were addressed by Murray in a couple of articles where he took all the criticisms
and spoke to them.
People listening to this podcast can certainly find all those online.
And it's very interesting.
But Murray went on to write some additional books, two in the last couple of years.
One about human diversity, where he goes through the data, refuting the idea that race is
only a social construct with no biological meaning. He discusses the
data. It's a very good discussion. You don't have to agree with it, but he presents data in
a cogent way. And he talks about the critics of that and he talks about their data in a
cogent, not personal way. It's a very informative discussion. A book is called Human Diversity.
He talks about race and he talks about gender, same thing, about sex differences.
And more recently, he's written what might be his final say on this, a book called Facing Reality.
Where he talks about this again.
So he can certainly defend himself.
He doesn't need me to do that.
But I would urge people who have heard about him and the bell curve and who think they
know what's in it, you are likely incorrect and you need to read it for yourself.
But it is scientifically, it's a serious subject, it's a difficult subject, ethically,
it's a difficult subject.
Everything you said here calmly and thoughtfully is difficult.
It's difficult for me to even consider that G-factor exists.
I don't mean from like that somehow G-factures inherently
racist or sexist or whatever.
It's just, it's difficult in the way that
concerning the fact that we die one day is difficult,
that we are limited by our biology.
It's difficult and it's, At least from an American perspective you would like to believe that everything is possible in this world
Well, that leads us to
what I think
We should do with this information
and
What I think we should do with this information is
unusual we should do with this information is unusual, because I think what we need to do is fund more
neuroscience research on the molecular biology of learning and memory, because one definition of
intelligence is based on how much you can learn and how much you can remember.
Yes.
And if you accept that definition of intelligence,
then there are molecular studies going on now,
and Nobel Prize is being won on molecular biology,
or molecular neurobiology of learning and memory.
Now, the step those researchers, those scientists
need to take when it comes to intelligence,
is to focus on the concept of individual differences.
Intelligence research has individual differences
as it's heart because it assumes that people differ on this variable
and those differences are meaningful and need understanding.
Cognitive psychologists who have morphed into molecular biologists studying learning and memory
hate the concept of individual differences
historically. Some now are coming around to it. I want to set next to a Nobel
Prize winner for his work on memory. And I asked him about individual
differences. And he said, don't go there, it'll set us back 50 years. But I said, don't you think they're the key though to understand,
you know, why can some people remember more than others?
He said, you don't want to go there.
I think the 21st century will be remembered by the technology and the science
that goes to individual differences.
Because we have now data, we have now the tools
that much, much better to start to measure, start to test
to me, not just on the sort of through test,
and I can test type of things, sort of outside the body
kind of things, but measuring all kinds of stuff
about the body.
So you had truly going to the molecular biology,
to the neurobiology, to the neuroscience.
But let me ask you about the life.
How does intelligence correlate with or lead to or has anything
to do with career success?
You've mentioned these kinds of things.
Is there any data?
You've had an excellent conversation with Jordan Peterson, for example.
Does any data on what intelligent means for success in life?
Success in life.
There is a tremendous amount of validity data that looked at intelligence test scores and various measures of life success.
Now of course, life success is a pretty broad topic
and not everybody agrees on what success means,
but there's general agreement on certain aspects of success that can be measured.
And including life expectancy, like you said, life expectancy. Now there's life success.
You know, life expectancy, I mean, that is such an interesting finding, but I, you know, I,
I, I, I, two scores are also correlated to things like income. Now, okay. So who thinks
income means you're successful. That's not the point. The point is that income is one empirical measure in this culture that says something about your level of success.
You can define success in ways that have nothing to do with income.
You can define success based on your evolutionary natural selection success. But for variables, and even that, by the way, is correlated to IQ in some studies.
So, however you want to define success, IQ is important. It's not the only determinant. People get
hung up on, well, what about personality?
What about so-called emotional intelligence? Yes, all those things matter. The thing that matters
empirically, the single thing that matters the most is your general ability, your general
mental intellectual ability, your reasoning ability, and the more complex your vocation, the more complex
your job, the more G matters. G doesn't matter in a lot of occupations, don't
require complex thinking. And there are occupations like that and G doesn't
matter. Within an occupation, the G might not matter so much, so that if you look at all the professors at MIT
and had a way to rank, order them, you know, there's a ceiling effect as what I'm saying,
that, you know, also when you get past a certain threshold, then there's
impact on wealth, for example, or career success. However, that's defined in each individual discipline,
but after certain point, it doesn't matter. Actually, it does matter in certain things. So, for
example, there is a very classic study that was started at Johns Hopkins when I was a graduate student there.
I actually worked on this study at the very beginning.
So the study of mathematically and scientifically precocious youth.
And they gave junior high school students, age 11 and 12, the standard SAT math exam.
And they found a very large number of students
scored very high on this exam.
I've not a large number.
I mean, they found many students when they cast the net
to all of Baltimore, they found a number of students
who scored as high on the SAT math
when they were 12 years old
as incoming Hopkins freshmen. And they said, Gene, now this is interesting. What shall we
do now? And on a case by case basis, they got some of those kids into their local community
college math programs. Many of those kids went on to be very successful.
And now there's a 50-year follow-up of those kids. And it turns out, if you... these kids were in
the top 1%. Okay, so everybody in the studies in the top 1% if you take that group that
Rarified group and divide them into quartiles
So that you have the top 25% of the top 1% and the bottom 25% of the top 1%
you can find
unmeasurable
And on measurable variables of success, the top quartile does better than the bottom quartile. In the top 1%, they have more patents, they have more publications, they have more tenure
at universities.
And this is based on, you're dividing them based on their score at age 12. I wonder how much interesting
Data is in the variability in the differences, so but that that's really that's a boy
That's very interesting, but it's also I don't know somehow painful. I don't know why it's so painful
That that's so that G-factor so determined of even in the nuanced
top percent. Well, this is interesting that you find that painful. Do you find it painful
that people with charisma are very successful, can be very successful in life, even though having
no other attributes other than their famous and people like them. Do you feel that painful?
Yes, if that charisma is untrainable.
So, one of the things, again, this is like I learned psychology from the Johnny Debt trial.
But one of the things the psychologist, the personality psychologist, he can maybe speak to this
because he had interest in this for time. Is she was saying that personality technically speaking is the thing that doesn't change
over a lifetime. It's the thing you're, I don't know if she was actually implying that
you're born with it.
Well, it's a trait. It's a trait that's's a trait that's relatively stable over time. I think that's
generally correct. So, to the degree your personality is stable over time, yes, that too is painful.
Because what's not painful is the thing, you know, if I'm fat and out of shape, I can exercise
fat, not a shape, I can exercise and you know become healthier in that way. If my diet is a giant mess and that's resulting in some kind of conditions that my body is
experiencing, I can fix that by having a better diet.
That's sort of my actions, my willed actions can make a change.
If charisma is part of the personality, that's the part of
the charisma that is part of the personality that is stable. Yeah, yeah, that's painful
too, because it's like, oh shit, I'm stuck with this. I'm stuck with this.
Well, I mean, and this pretty much generalizes to every aspect of your being. This is who
you are. You've got to deal with it.
And what it undermines, of course, is a realistic appreciation for this.
undermines the fairly recent idea prevalent in this country that if you work hard, you can be
anything you want to be, which has morphed from the original idea that if you work hard, you can be anything you want to be, which has morphed from the original idea that if you work hard, you can be successful.
Those are two different things.
And now we have, if you work hard, you can be anything you want to be.
This is completely unrealistic.
I'm sorry.
It just is.
Now you can work hard and be successful.
There's no question.
But you know what?
I could work very hard
and I am not going to be a successful theoretical physicist.
I'm just not.
That said, I mean, we should,
because we had this conversation already,
but it's good to repeat the fact that you're not going
to be a theoretical physicist.
It's not judgment on your basic humanity.
Returning again to the all men, which means men and women are created equal.
So again, some of the differences we're talking about in quote-unquote success, wealth, number of whether you want to know about prize or not,
that doesn't put a measure on your basic humanity and basic value and even goodness of
you as a human being. Because that, that your basic role in value and society is largely within
your control. It's some of these measures that we're talking about. It's good to remember
this. One question about the Flynn effect. What is it? Are humans getting smarter over the years, over the decades, over the centuries?
The flint effect is James Flynn, who passed away about a year ago, published a set of analyses
coming back a couple of decades. I mean, I first noticed this, that IQ scores when you looked over the
years seem to be drifting up.
Now this was not unknown to the people who make the test because they renorm the test periodically
and they have to ren them the test periodically because what
10 items correct
meant relative to other people
50 years ago is not the same as what 10 items mean relative today
People are getting more things correct
Now the scores have been drifting up about three points IQ scores have been drifting up about three points, IQ scores have been drifting up
about three points per decade.
This is not a personal effect.
This is a cohort effect.
It's not for an individual.
But the world, so what's the difference?
And this is presented in intelligence researchers with a great mystery.
Two questions. First, is it effect on the
50% of the variance that's the G factor or on the other 50% and there's evidence that it
is a G factor effect. And second, what on Earth causes this and doesn't this mean intelligence and g factor cannot be genetic because the scale
of natural selection is much, much longer
than a couple of decades ago.
And so it's been used to try to undermine the idea
that there can be a genetic influence on intelligence.
But certainly it can be, the flint effect can affect
the non-genetic aspects of intelligence,
because genes account for maybe 50% of the variance.
It could be as high as 80% for adults,
but let's just say 50% for discussion.
So the flint effect is still a mystery.
The still a mystery.
That's interesting.
Although the evidence is coming out,
I told you before I added a journal on intelligence
and we're doing a special issue in honor of James Flynn.
So I'm starting to see papers now
and really the latest research on this.
I think most people who specialize in this area, trying to understand the flint effect,
are coming to the view based on data that it has to do with advances in nutrition and
health care.
And there's also evidence that the effect is slowing down and possibly reversing.
Oh, boy.
So, how would nutrition, so the nutrition would still be connected to the G factor.
So nutrition as it relates to the G factor, so the biology, at least the intelligence.
Yes, that would be the claim.
The hypothesis being tested by the researchers.
Yes, and there's some evidence from infants that nutrition has made a difference.
So it's not an unreasonable connection, but does it negate the idea that there's a genetic influence,
not logically at all.
But it is very interesting so that if you take an IQ test today, but you take the score
and use the tables that were available in 1940, you're going to wind up with a much higher IQ number.
So are we really smarter than a couple of generations ago? No, but we might be able to solve
problems a little better and make use of our G because of things like sesame street and other curricula in school more people are going to to to school.
So there are a lot of factors here that disentangle it is fascinating. It's fascinating that there's not clear answers yet.
That as a population we're getting smarter we just just zoom out, that's what it looks like,
as a population we're getting smarter.
It's interesting to see what the effects of that are.
I mean, this raises the question.
We've mentioned it many times,
but haven't clearly addressed it,
which is nature versus nurture questions.
So how much of intelligence is nature?
How much of it is nurture?
How much of it is determined by genetics versus environment? All of it. All of it is nurture, how much of it is determined by genetics versus environment.
All of it.
All of it is genetics.
No, all of it is nature and nurture.
So yes, yes to, okay.
That's not a lot of variance.
Can you apportion to either?
Yeah.
Most of the people who work in this field say that that is a,
the framing of that, if the question is framed that way,
it can't be answered because nature and nurture
are not too independent influences.
They interact with each other.
And understanding those interactions is so complex that many behavioral geneticists say it is today
impossible and always will be impossible to disentangle that no matter what kind of
advances there are in DNA technology and genomic informatics. But they're still to push back on that.
That same intuition from behavioral geneticists
would lead me to believe that there cannot possibly be a stable G-factor
because it's super complex.
Many of them would assert that as a logical outcome.
But because I believe there is a stable G-Vactor from lots of sources of data,
not just one study, but lots of sources of data over decades, I am more amenable to the idea
that whatever interactions between genes and environment exist.
They can be explicated. They can be studied. And that information can be used as a basis for molecular biology of intelligence. And we'll do this exact question because it doesn't
this exact question, because doesn't the stability of the G factor give you at least a hint that there is a biological basis for intelligence?
Yes.
I think it's clear that the fact that an IQ score is correlated to things like thickness
of your cortex, that it's correlated to glucose metabolic
rate in your brain that identical twins reared apart are highly similar in their IQ scores.
These are all important observations that certainly more that indicate, not just suggests, but indicate
that there's a biological basis.
And does anyone believe intelligence has nothing to do with the brain?
I mean, it's so obvious.
Well, indirectly definitely has to do with it, but the question is, environment interacting with the brain, or is it the actual
raw hardware of the brain?
Well, some would say that the raw hardware of the brain, as it develops from conception
as it develops from conception through adulthood,
or at least through the childhood, that that so-called hardware that you are assuming
is mostly genetic, in fact,
is not as deterministic as you might think,
that it is probabilistic and what affects the probabilities
are things like
in-utering environment and other factors like that, including chance. That chance
affects the way the neurons are connecting during gestation. It's not, hey, it's pre-programmed.
So there is pushback on the concept that genes provide a blueprint that is a lot more
fluid.
Well, but also, yeah, so there's a lot, a lot, a lot happens in the first few months of
development.
So, in nine months, inside the mother's body, and in the, you know, the months, the few
months afterwards, there's a lot of fascinating stuff, like including chance and luck, like
you said, how things connect up.
And the question is afterwards,
the neuroplasticity of the brain,
how much adjustment there is relative to the environment,
how much that affects the G factor,
but that's where the whole conclusions
of the studies that we've been talking about
is that seems to have less and less and less of an effect
as pretty quickly.
Yes, and I do think there is more of a genetic,
by my view, and I'm not an expert on this.
I mean, genetics is a highly technical and complex subject.
I am not a geneticist, not a behavioral geneticist,
but my reading of this, my interpretation of this
is that there is a genetic blueprint more or less, and that has
a profound influence on your subsequent intellectual development, including the G factor.
And that's not to say things can't happen to, I mean, if you think of that genes provide a potential, fine, and then various variables
impact that potential.
And every parent of a newborn implicitly or explicitly wants to maximize that potential.
This is why you buy educational toys.
This is why you pay attention to organic baby food. This is why you do all these things because you want your baby to be as healthy and as smart as possible.
And every parent will say that.
Is there a case to be made?
Can you steal me on the case that genetics is a very tiny component of all of this and the environment is essential.
I don't think the data supports that genetics is a tiny component. I think the data support the idea
that the genetics is a very important and I don't say component, I say influence. Very important
influence and the environment is a lot less than people believe
Most people believe environment plays a big role. I'm not so sure
I guess what I'm asking you is can you see where or what you just said it might be wrong
Can you can you imagine a world and what kind of evidence would you need to see?
world. And what kind of evidence would you need to see? To say, you know what, the intuition, the study so far, like reversing the directions. So one of the cool things we have now, more
and more, is we're getting more and more data. And the rate of the data is escalating because
of the digital world. So when you start to look at a very large scale of data, both from a biology side
and a social side, we might be discovering some very counterintuitive things about society.
We might see the edge cases that reveal that if we actually scale those edge cases and they
become like the norm that will have a complete shift in our like you'll see G factor be able to be
modified throughout life in the teens and in the in in later life. So in any case you can make
or for your current intuitions are wrong. Yes, and it's a good question because I think everyone
should always be asked what evidence would change your mind.
It's certainly not only a fair question. It is really the key question for anybody working on any aspect of science.
I think that if environment
was very important,
we would have seen it clearly by now. It would have been obvious that
school interventions, compensatory education, early childhood education, all these things
that have been earnestly tried and well-funded, well-designed studies would show some effect
and they don't. They don't. What if the school, the way we've tried
school, compensatory school sucks, and we need to do
that?
Well, everybody said, that's what everybody said to Jensen.
He said, well, maybe these, maybe we need to start earlier.
Maybe we need not do pre-kindergarten, but pre-pre-kindergarten.
It's always an infinite, well, maybe we didn't get it right. But after decades of trying, 50 years, 50
or 60 years of trying, surely something would have worked to the point where you could actually
see a result and not need a probability level at .05 on some means. So that's why I, that's the kind of evidence
that would change my mind.
Population level interventions like schooling
that you would see like this actually has an effect.
Yes, and when you take adopted kids
and they grow up in another family
and you find out when those adopted kids are adults,
their IQ scores don't correlate with the IQ scores of their adoptive parents,
but they do correlate with their IQ scores of their biological parents,
whom they've never met. I mean, these are important. These are powerful observations.
And you would be convincing to you if the reverse was true.
Yes, that would be more.
Now, and there is some data on adoption
that indicates that the adopted children
are moving a little bit more toward their adoptive parents.
But it's, you know, it's, to me, the overwhelming,
the weight, I have this concept called the weight of evidence
where I don't interpret any one study too much.
The weight of evidence tells me genes are important.
But what does that mean?
What does it mean that genes are important
knowing that gene expression,
gene stone express themselves in a vacuum.
They express themselves in an environment.
So the environment has to have something to do with it, in a vacuum, they express themselves in an environment.
So the environment has to have something to do with it,
especially if the best genetic estimates of the amount
of variance are around 50 or even if it's as high as 80%,
it still leaves 20% of non-genetic.
Now maybe that was all luck.
Maybe that's all chance.
I could believe that.
I could easily believe that.
So, but I do think after 50 years of trying various interventions and nothing works, including
memory training, including listening to Mozart, including playing computer games, none of
that has shown any impact on intelligence
test scores. Is there data on the intelligence the IQ of parents as it relates to the children?
Yes, and there is some evidence, genetic evidence of kind of an interaction between the parents IQ and the environment,
that high IQ parents provide an enriched environment,
which then can impact the child in addition to the genes
that environment.
So there are all these interactions that, you know,
but it's not, you know, think about the number of books in a household.
This was a variable that's correlated with IQ. And it is. Yeah.
Well, well, why? Especially if the kid never reads any of the books. It's because more intelligent people
have more books in their house. And if you're more intelligent,
and there's a genetic component to that,
the child will get those genes or some of those genes,
as well as the environment,
but it's not the number of books in the house
that actually directly impacts the child.
So the two scenarios on this are you find that
and this was used to get rid of the SAT test. Oh, the SAT scores highly correlated with the
social economic status of the parents. So all you're really measuring is how rich the parents are.
Okay, well, why are the parents rich?
Okay, well, why are the parents rich? Yes.
And so you could, the opposite kind of,
syllogism is that people who are very bright make more money.
They can afford homes in better neighborhoods
so their kids get better schools.
Now, the kids grow up bright. Where in
that chain of events does that come from? Well, unless you have a genetically
informative research design where you look at siblings that have the same
biological parents and so on, you can't really disentangle all that. Most studies of social economic status and intelligence
do not have a genetically informed design.
So any conclusions they make about the causality
of the social economic status being the cause
of the IQ is a stretch.
And where you do find genetically informative designs, you find most of the
variants in your outcome measures are due to the genetic component. And sometimes the
SES adds a little, but the weight of evidence is it doesn't add very much variance to predict
what's going on beyond the genetic variance.
So when you actually look at it, and there aren't that many studies that have genetically
informed designs, but when you do see those, the genes seem to have an advantage. Sorry for the strange questions, but is there a connection between fertility or the number of kids that you have and G factor?
So, you know, the kind of conventional wisdom is people of maybe is it higher economic status or something like that or having fewer children.
I just loosely hear these kinds of things.
Is there data that you're aware of in one direction or another on this?
Well, strange questions always get strange answers.
Yes.
So do you have a strange answer for that? Well, the answer is, there were some studies that indicated the more children in a family,
the first born children would be more intelligent than the father, fifth or sixth.
It's not clear that those studies hold up over time. And of course, what you see also is that families where
there are multiple children, four, five, six, seven, you know, really big families. The social
economic status of those families, usually in the modern age is not that high.
Maybe it used to be the aristocracy, used to have a lot of kids, I'm not sure exactly.
But there have been reports of correlations between IQ and fertility,
but I'm not sure that the data are very strong, that the first born child is always the smartest.
It seems like there's some data to that, but I'm not current on that.
Well, how would that be explained?
That would be a nurture.
Well, it could be nurture.
It could be in uterine environment.
I mean, try to can't get it. And this is why this, you know, like many
areas of science, you said earlier that there are a lot of gray areas and no definitive answers.
This is not uncommon in science that the closer you look at a problem, the more questions
you get, not the fewer questions, because the universe is complicated.
And the idea that we have people on this planet who can study the first nanoseconds of
the Big Bang, That's pretty amazing.
And I've always said that if they can study the first nanoseconds of the big bang, we
can certainly figure out something about intelligence that allows that.
I'm not sure what's more complicated.
The human mind or the physics of the universe. It's unclear to me.
I think we over emphasize that.
That's a very humbling statement.
It may be it's very human-centric, egotistical statement that our mind is somehow super complicated,
but biology is a trick you want to unravel.
Consciousness.
What is that? I've always believed that consciousness and intelligence are the two
real fundamental problems of the human brain. And therefore, I think they must be related.
And problems like walk together, holding hands, kind of idea.
You may not know this, but I did some of the early research on anesthetic drugs with brain
imaging trying to answer the question, what part of the brain is the last to turn off when
someone loses consciousness?
And is that the first part of the brain to turn on when consciousness is regained?
And I was working with an anesthesiologist, and Mike Alkairr, who was really brilliant
at this, these were really the first studies of brain imaging using positron emission
tomography long before FMRI.
And you would inject a radioactive sugar that labeled the brain, and the harder the brain was working,
the more sugar it would take up,
and then you could make a picture of glucose use in the brain.
And he was amazing.
He managed to do this in normal volunteerism brought in
and anesthetized as if they were going into surgery.
He managed all the human subjects requirements on this research, and it was brilliant at
this.
What we did is we had these normal volunteers come in on three occasions. On one occasion, he gave them enough anesthetic
drug, so they were a little drowsy. And on another occasion, they came in and he fully anesthetized them.
know, he would say, you know, Mike, can you can you hear me? And the person would say, ah, yeah, you know, that's it. And then we would scan people under the and under no anesthetic
condition. So the same person. And we were looking to see if we could see the part of the
brain turn off. He subsequently tried to do this
with FABRI, which has a faster time resolution, and you could do it in real time as the person went
under and then regain consciousness where you couldn't do that with PET, you had to have three
drastic patients. And the results were absolutely fascinating. We did this with different anesthetic drugs and different drugs
Impacted different parts of the brain. So we were naturally looking for the common one and
Seemed to have something to do with the thalamus and
Consciousness this was actual data
unconsciousness
Real con actual consciousness. What part of the brain turns on?
What part of the brain turns off?
It's not so clear.
But maybe has something to do with the thalamus.
The sequence of events seem to have the thalamus in it.
Boy.
Now here's a question.
Are some people more conscious than others?
Are there individual differences in consciousness?
And I don't mean it in the psychedelic sense.
I don't mean it in the political consciousness sense.
I just mean it in everyday life.
To some people go through everyday life more conscious than others.
And are those the people we might actually label more intelligent?
So now, the other thing I was looking for is whether the parts of the brain others, and are those the people we might actually label more intelligent?
So now the other thing I was looking for is whether the parts of the brain we were
seeing in the anesthesia studies were the same parts of the brain we were seeing in
the intelligent studies. Now this is, you know, this was very complicated,
expensive research. We didn't really have funding to do this. We were trying to do
it on the fly.
I'm not sure anybody has pursued this. I'm retired now. He's going on to other things.
But I think it's an area of research that would be fascinating to see a lot more imaging studies
now of consciousness. I'm just not up on them. So, but basically the question is, which imaging, so newer imaging studies,
to see in high-resolution, spatial and temporal way, which part of the brain lights up
while when you're doing intelligence tasks, and which parts of the brain lights up when you're doing consciousness tasks,
and see the interplay between them, try to infer
a message challenge of neuroscience without understanding deeply, looking from the outside, try to
infer something about how the whole thing works. Well, imagine this. Here's a simple question.
Here's a simple question. Does it take more anesthetic drug to have a person lose consciousness if their IQ is 140 than a person with an IQ of 70?
That's an interesting way to study it. if the answer to that is, is this stable, yes,
that's very interesting.
So I tried to find out.
And I went to some anesthesiology textbooks about how you do dose.
And they dose by weight.
And what I also learned, this is a little bit off subject. Anesthesiologists are never sure if you how deep you are.
Yeah. And they usually tell by poking you with a needle. And if you don't jump, they tell the
surgeon to go ahead. I'm not sure that's literally true, but it's, well, it might be very difficult
to know precisely how deep you are.
It has to do with the same kind of measurements
that you were doing with the consciousness,
with the, it's difficult to know.
So I don't lose my train of thought.
I couldn't find in the textbooks anything
about dosing by intelligence.
I asked my friend, the anesthesiologist,
he said, no, he doesn't know.
I said, can we do a chart review and look at people using their years of education as a proxy for IQ?
Because if someone's gone to graduate school, that tells you something.
You can make some inference as opposed to someone who didn't graduate high school.
Can we do a chart review? he says, no, they never really
put down the exact dose. And no, he said, no. So to this day, the simple question, does
it take more anesthetic drug to put someone under if they have a high IQ or less or less?
It could go either way, because by the way, our early PET scans studies on intelligence
found the unexpected result of an inverse correlation
between glucose metabolic rate and intelligence.
It wasn't how much a brain area lit up.
How much it lit up was negatively correlated to how well they did on the test, which led to the
brain efficiency hypothesis, which is still being studied today. And there's more and more evidence
that the efficiency of brain information processing is more related to intelligence than
related to intelligence, then, then, then, then just more activity.
Yeah. And it would be interesting. Again, this is the total hypothesis of how much in the relationship between intelligence and consciousness, it's not obvious that those two, if there's correlation, that would be, they could be inversely correlated.
Wouldn't that be funny? If you
the
The consciousness factor the C factor plus the G factor equals one
It's a nice trade-off you get you get you get a trade-off. How deeply you experience the world versus how
Deeply you're able to a through the world. What a great hypothesis. Certainly somebody listening to this can
do this study. Even if it's the aliens analyzing humans a few centuries from now. Let me ask
you from an AI perspective. I don't know how much you've thought about machines, but there's the famous touring
test of intelligence for machines, which is a beautiful, almost like a cute formulation
of intelligence that Alan Turing proposed, basically conversation being if you can fool a human to think that
a machine is a human that passes the test. I suppose you could do a similar thing for
humans. If I can fool you that I am intelligent, then that's a good test of intelligence, right?
Like you're talking to two people
and my, the test is saying who has a higher IQ?
And it's an interesting test,
because yeah, maybe charisma can be very useful there.
And you're only allowed to use conversation, which is the formulation of the joint test.
Anyway, all that to say is, what are good tests of intelligence for machines?
You know, what do you think it takes to achieve human level intelligence for machines?
Well, I have thought a little bit about this, but every time I think about these things,
I rapidly reach the limits of my knowledge and imagination.
So when Alexa first came out, and I think there was a competing one while there was Siri with Apple and Google had Alexa.
No, no, Amazon had Alexa.
Amazon had Alexa. Google has Google assistant.
Something.
So I proposed to one of my colleagues that he buy one of these, each, you know, one of
each, and then ask questions from the IQ test.
Yes.
But it became apparent that they all search the internet.
So they all can find answers to questions like how far is it between Washington and Miami
and repeat after me.
Now I don't know if you said to Alexa, I'm going to repeat these numbers backwards
to me. I don't know what would happen. I've never done it. But the so one answer to your question
is try get you're going to try it right now. Let's try it. No, that's right. No, that's
Siri. So it would actually probably go to Google search and it will be all confusing kind of stuff.
It would fail.
Well, then I guess there's a test that it would fail.
Well, but that's not, that has to do more with the, you know, the language of communication
versus the content.
So if you did Nike, you test a person who doesn't speak English and the
test was administered in English, that's not really the test
of well, let's think about the computers that beat the
jeopardy champions. Yeah, so that's so that's because I
happen to know how those are programmed is a very hard
coded and there's definitely a lack of intelligence there.
and there's a very hard coded and there's definitely a lack of intelligence there.
There's something like IQ tests.
There's a guy,
artificial intelligence researcher,
Francois Cholet, he's a Google,
he's one of the seminal people in machine learning.
He also has a fun side thing,
develop a Nike U-Test from machines.
I haven't heard that.
I'd like to know about that.
I'll actually email you this because you'd be very interesting for you.
It doesn't get much attention because people don't know what to do with it.
But it deserves a lot of attention, which is it basically does a pattern type of tests where you have to do one standard one
is you're given three things and you have to do a fourth one, that kind of thing.
You have to understand the pattern here.
And for that, it really simplifies to... to, so the interesting thing is he's trying not to achieve high IQ.
He's trying to achieve like pretty low bar for IQ.
Things that are kind of trivial for humans and they're actually really tough for machines,
which is seeing, playing with these concepts of symmetry of counting.
If I give you one object, two objects, three objects, the last one is four objects.
You can count them.
You can cluster objects together.
It's both visually and conceptually.
We could do all these things with our mind that would take for granted the objectness of things.
We can figure out what spatially is an object and isn't.
And we can play with those ideas.
And machines really struggle with that.
So he really clearly formulated these IQ tests.
I wonder what that would equate to for humans with IQ, but it'd be a very low IQ
But that's exactly the kind of formulation like okay, we want to be able to solve this
How do we solve this and he does as a challenge and nobody's been able to
It's similar to the Alexa prize, which is Amazon is hosting a conversation on challenge
Nobody's been able to do well on his.
But that's an interesting, those kinds of tests are interesting because we take for granted all the
the ability of the human mind to play with concepts and to formulate concepts out of novel things. So like
Things we've never seen before we were able to use that. I mean, that's I've talked to a few people that design IQ tests
sort of online they write IQ tests and
I was trying to get some questions from them and they spoke to the fact that we can't really share questions with you because
like first of all it's really hard work to come up with questions. It's really, really hard work.
It takes a lot of research but it also takes a lot of novelty generating. Your constant coming
up with really new things and part of the point is that you're not supposed to be public.
They're supposed to be new to you when you look at them.
It's interesting that the novelties fundamental to the hardness of the problem,
at least a part of what makes the problem hard is you've never seen it before.
Right. That's called fluid intelligence, as opposed to what's called crystallizing intelligence,
which is your knowledge of facts.
You know things, but can you use those things to solve a problem?
Those are two different things.
Do you think we'll be able to, because I don't want to miss opportunity to talk about this,
to spoke about the neurobiology, the molecular biology of intelligence.
Do you think one day we'll be able to modify the biology of the genetics of a person to
modify their intelligence, to increase their intelligence?
We started this conversation by talking about a pill you could take. Do you think that such a pill would exist?
Metaphorically. I do. And I am supremely confident that it's possible because I am supremely
ignorant of the complexities of neurobiology. And so I have written.
Ignorance is bliss. Well, I have written that the nightmares of neurobiologists, you know, understanding
the complexities, this cascade of events that happens at the synaptic level, that these
nightmares are what fuel some people to solve.
So some people, you have to be undaunted.
I mean, yeah, this is not easy. Look, we're still trying to figure out cancer. It was only
recently that they figured out why aspirin works. You know, these are not easy problems, but I also have the perspective of the history of science,
is the history of solving problems that are extraordinarily complex.
And seem impossible at the time.
And so, one of the things you look at at companies like Neuralink,
And so one of the things you look at at companies like Neuralink,
you have brain, computer interfaces, you start to delve into the human mind and start to talk about machines measuring but also sending signals to the human mind. You start to wonder what that
what impact that has on the G factor. Modifying in small ways or in large ways the functioning,
the G factor, modifying the small ways or in large ways, the functioning, the mechanical electrical chemical functioning of the brain.
I look at everything about the brain.
There are different levels of explanation.
On one hand, you have a behavioral level, but then you have brain circuitry and then you have neurons and then you have
dendrites and then you have synapses and then you have the neurotransmitters and the presynaptic
and the post synaptic terminals and then you have all the things that influence neurotransmitters.
And then you have the individual differences among people.
Yeah, it's complicated.
But 51 million people in the United States
have IQs under 85 and struggle with everyday life. Shouldn't
that motivate people to take a look at this?
Yeah.
The energy is, you know.
Yeah.
Now, but I just want to linger one more time that, we have to remember that the science of intelligence,
the measure of intelligence is only a part
of the human condition, the thing that makes life beautiful
and the creation of beautiful things in this world
is perhaps loosely correlated,
but is not dependent entirely on intelligence.
Absolutely. I certainly agree with that.
So for any more sort of listening, I'm still not convinced that sort of more intelligence is always better
if you want to create beauty in this world. I don't know.
Well, I didn't say more intelligence is always better if you want to create beauty.
I just said all things being equal more is better than less.
That's all I mean.
Yeah, but that's sort of that I just want to say because to me one of the things that makes life great is the opportunity to create beautiful
things. And so I just want to sort of empower people to do that no matter what some IQ
test says. At the population level, we do need to look at IQ test to help people. And
to also inspire us, yeah, to take on some of these extremely difficult scientific
questions.
Do you have advice for young people in high school, in college, whether they're thinking
about career or they're thinking about a life they can be proud of?
Is there advice you can give?
Whether they're in the, they want to pursue psychology or biology
or engineering, are there want to be artists and musicians and poets?
I can't advise anybody on that level of what your passion is.
But I can't say if you're interested in psychology, if you're interested in science and the science
around
the big questions of
consciousness and intelligence and
psychiatric illness
We haven't really talked about brain illnesses and what we might learn from
about brain illnesses and what we might learn from, you know, if you are trying to develop a drug to treat Alzheimer's disease, you are trying to develop a drug to
impact learning and memory, which are core to intelligence. So it could well be
that the so-called IQ pill will come from a pharmaceutical company trying to
develop a drug for Alzheimer's disease. Because that's exactly what you're trying to do, right?
Yeah. Well, what will that drug do in a college student that doesn't have Alzheimer's disease?
So I would encourage people who are interested in psychology, who are interested in science, to pursue a scientific
career and address the big questions.
And this, you end the most important thing I can tell you, if you're going to be in kind
of a research environment, is you've got to follow the data where the data take you. You can't
decide in advance where you want the data to go. And if the data take you to places that you don't
have the technical expertise to follow, like, you know, I would like to understand more about
molecular biology, but I'm not going to become a molecular biologist now, but I know people who are.
And my job is to get them interested
to take their expertise into this direction.
And that, it's not so easy, but...
And if the data takes you to a place
that's controversial, that's counterintuitive in this world,
no, I would say it's probably a good idea to still push forward boldly, but to communicate
the interpretation of the results with skill, with compassion, with a greater breadth of
understanding of humanity, not just the science of the impact of the results.
One famous psychologist wrote about this issue
that somehow a balance has to be found
between pursuing the science and communicating it
with respect to people's sensitivities,
the legitimate sensitivities, somehow. He didn't say how. Somehow. Somehow.
And this is a part of that sentence. Somehow and balance is left up to the interpretation
of the reader. Let me ask you. You said big questions, the biggest or one of the biggest.
We're already talked about consciousness and intelligence, one of the most fascinating, one of the biggest, we already talked about consciousness and intelligence, one of the
most fascinating, one of the biggest questions.
But let's talk about the why.
Why are we here?
What's the meaning of life?
Oh, I'm not going to tell you.
You know what we are going to tell me?
This is very...
I'm going to have to wait for your next book.
The meaning of life, you know. We do the best we can to get through the day.
And then there's just a finite number of the days.
Are you afraid of the finiteness of it?
You think about your dad.
I think about it more and more as I get older.
Yeah, I do.
And it's one of these human things.
It is finite. We all know it.
Most of us deny it.
And don't want to think about it.
Sometimes you think about it in terms of a state planning.
You try to do the rational thing.
Sometimes it makes you work harder,
because you know your time is more and more limited and you want to get things done.
I don't know where I am on that. It is just one of those things that's always in the back of my mind.
And I don't think that's uncommon. It was just like G-factor intelligence.
It's a hard truth that's there.
And sometimes you kind of walk past it
and you don't want to look at it,
but it's still there.
Yeah.
Yeah.
Yes, you can't escape it.
And think about the G-factor intelligence.
Is everybody knows this is true on a personal daily basis.
If even if you think back to when you were in school, you know who the smart kids were.
When you are on the phone talking to a customer service representative, that in response to
your detailed question is reading
a script back to you and you get furious at this.
Have you ever called this person a moron or wanted to call this person a moron?
You're not listening to me.
Everybody has had the experience of dealing with people who they think are not at their
level.
It's just common because that's the way human
beings are. That's the way life is. But we also have a poor estimation of our own intelligence,
we have a poor, and we're not always a great, our judgment of human character of other people is not as good as a battery of tests.
We there's there's there's that's where bias comes in.
That's where our history, our emotions, all of that comes in.
So, you know, people on the internet, you know, there's such a thing as the internet.
And people on the internet will call each other dumb all the time. That's the worry here is that we give up on people.
We put them in a bin just because of one interaction or some small number of interactions as if
that's it.
They're hopeless.
That's just in their genetics. But I think no matter what the science here says,
once again, that does not mean we should not have compassion for our fellow man.
That's exactly what the science does say. It's not opposite of what the science says.
Everything I know about psychology, everything I've learned about intelligence,
everything points to the inexorable conclusion that you have to treat people as individuals
respectfully and with compassion. Because through no fault of their own, some people are
not as capable as others. And you want to turn a blind eye to it,
you want to come up with with theories about why that might be true. Fine. I would like to fix
some of it as best I can. And everybody is deserving of love. Which is a good way to end it, I think.
of which, this is a good way to end it, I think.
I'm thinking warmed up here. I know.
I know you can go for another many hours,
but to respect your extremely valuable time,
this is an amazing conversation.
Thank you for the teaching company,
the lectures you've given with the neuroscience of intelligence,
just the work you're doing. It's a difficult
topic. It's a topic that's controversial and sensitive to people and to push forward
boldly. And in that nuanced way, just thank you for everything you do. And thank you
for asking the big questions of intelligence, of consciousness.
Well, thank you for asking me. I mean, there's nothing like good conversation on these topics. Thanks for listening to this conversation with Richard
Hire. To support this podcast please check out our sponsors in the description
and now let me leave you with some words from Albert Einstein. It is not that
I'm so smart but I stay with the questions much longer. Thank you for listening
and hope to see you next time.
you