Lex Fridman Podcast - Jeff Hawkins: Thousand Brains Theory of Intelligence
Episode Date: July 1, 2019Jeff Hawkins is the founder of Redwood Center for Theoretical Neuroscience in 2002 and Numenta in 2005. In his 2004 book titled On Intelligence, and in his research before and after, he and his team h...ave worked to reverse-engineer the neocortex and propose artificial intelligence architectures, approaches, and ideas that are inspired by the human brain. These ideas include Hierarchical Temporal Memory (HTM) from 2004 and The Thousand Brains Theory of Intelligence from 2017. If you would like to get more information about this podcast go to https://lexfridman.com/ai or connect with @lexfridman on Twitter, LinkedIn, Facebook, Medium, or YouTube where you can watch the video versions of these conversations.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
The following is a conversation with Jeff Hawkins.
He's the founder of the Redwood Center for Theoretical
Neuroscience in 2002 and New Menta in 2005.
In his 2004 book titled On Intelligence
and in the research before and after,
he and his team have worked to reverse engineer
the New York cortex and propose artificial intelligence
architectures, approaches, and ideas
that are inspired by the human brain.
These ideas include hierarchical top or memory, HTM from 2004, and New Work, the Thousand's
Brain's Theory of Intelligence from 2017, 18, and 19.
Jeff's ideas have been an inspiration to many who have looked for progress beyond the
current machine learning approaches, but they have also received criticism for lacking
a body of empirical evidence supporting
the models. This is always a challenge when seeking more than small incremental steps
forward in AI. Jeff is a brilliant mind, and many of the ideas he has developed and aggregated
from neuroscience are worth understanding and thinking about. There are limits to deep
learning as it is currently defined. Forward progress in AI is shrouded in mystery.
My hope is that conversations like this can help provide an inspiring spark for new ideas.
This is the Artificial Intelligence Podcast.
If you enjoy it, subscribe on YouTube, iTunes, or simply connect with me on Twitter at
Lex Friedman spelled F-R-I-D.
And now here's my conversation with Jeff Hawkins.
Are you more interested in understanding the human brain or in creating artificial systems
that have many of the same qualities but don't necessarily require that you actually
understand the underpinning workings of our mind?
So there's a clear answer to that question.
My primary interest is understanding the human brain.
No question about it. But I also firmly believe that we will not be able to create fully intelligent machines
until we understand how the human brain works.
So I don't see those as separate problems.
I think there's limits.
So what can be done with machine intelligence if you don't understand the principles
of which the brain works.
And so I actually believe that studying the brain is actually the fastest way to get to machine intelligence.
And within that, let me ask the impossible question,
how do you not define, but at least think about
what it means to be intelligent?
So I didn't try to answer that question first.
We said, let's just talk about how the brain works.
And let's figure out how to turn parts of the brain,
mostly the neocortex, but some other parts too.
The parts of the brain, most associated with intelligence.
And let's discover the principles by how they work.
Because intelligence isn't just like some mechanism, and
it's not just some capabilities.
It's like, OK, we don't even know where to begin on this stuff.
And so now that we've made a lot of progress on this,
after we've made a lot of progress
on how the Neocortex works,
and we can talk about that,
I now have a very good idea
what's gonna be required to make Intelligent Machines.
I can tell you today,
but you know, some of the things are gonna be necessary,
I believe, to create Intelligent Machines.
Well, so we'll get there.
We'll get to the Neocortex
and some of the theories of how the whole thing works.
And you're saying, as we understand more and more
about the Neocortex, about our own human mind,
we'll be able to start to more specifically define
what it means to be intelligence, not useful
to really talk about that until...
I don't know if it's not useful.
Look, there's a long history of AI, as you know.
And there's been different approaches taken to it.
And who knows, maybe they're all useful.
So the good old fashioned AI, the expert systems,
the current convolution neural networks,
they all have their utility.
They all have a value in the world.
But I would think almost everyone
agree that not everyone are really intelligent,
in a sort of a deep way that humans are.
And so, it's just the question of how do you get from where those systems were or are today,
to where a lot of people think we're going to go.
And there's a big, big gap there, a huge gap.
And I think the quickest way of bridging that gap is to figure out how the brain does that.
And then we can sit back and look and say,
oh, which of these principles that the brain works on
are necessary and which ones are not?
Cooler, we don't have to build this in
tells machines aren't gonna be built out of
organic living cells.
But there's a lot of stuff that goes on the brain
that's gonna be necessary.
So let me ask maybe, before we get into the fun details,
let me ask maybe a depressing or a difficult question.
Do you think it's possible that we will never
be able to understand how our brain works, that maybe there's
aspects to the human mind, like we ourselves
cannot introspectively get to the core, that there's a wall
you eventually hit?
Yeah. I don't believe that's the case. I have never believed that's the case. There's not
been a single thing we've ever, humans have ever put their minds to, so we said, oh, we reached
the wall, we can't go any further. It's just people keep saying that. People used to believe that
about life, you know, a long Vitaal, right? There's like, what's the difference in living matter
and non-living matter, something special you never understand?
We no longer think that.
So there's no historical evidence
that suggests this is the case,
and I just never even considered that's a possibility.
I would also say today,
we understand so much about the near cortex.
We've made tremendous progress in the last few years
that I no longer think of as an open question.
The answers are very clear to me and the pieces we know we don't know are clear to me,
but the framework is all there and it's like, oh, okay, we're going to be able to do this.
This is not a problem anymore. It just takes time and effort, but there's no mystery,
a big mystery anymore. So then let's get it into it for people like myself who are not
So then let's get it into it for people like myself who are not very well versed in the human brain, except my own.
Can you describe to me at the highest level what are the different parts of the human brain
and then zooming in on the neocortex, the parts of the neocortex and so on, a quick overview?
Yeah, sure.
A human brain, we can divide it roughly into two parts.
There's the old parts, lots of pieces,
and then there's the new part.
The new part is the neocortex.
It's new because it didn't exist before mammals.
The only mammals have a neocortex.
And in humans, it's in primates, it's very large.
In a human brain, the neocortex occupies about 78%
to 75%
of the volume of the brain.
It's huge.
And the old parts of the brain are,
there's lots of pieces there.
There's a spinal cord and there's the brain stem
and the cerebellum and the different parts
of the basic ganglia and so on.
In the old parts of the brain, you have autonomic regulation,
like breathing and heart rate,
you have basic behaviors.
So like walking and running are controlled
by the old parts of the brain.
All the emotional centers of the brain
are in the old part of the brain.
So when you feel anger or hungry lost with things like that,
those are all in the old parts of the brain.
And we associate with the neocortex,
all the things we think about as sort of high level perception
and cognitive functions, anything from seeing and hearing and touching things
to language, to mathematics and engineering and science and so on.
Those are all associated with the near cortex.
And they're certainly correlated.
Our abilities in those regards are correlated with the relative size of
our near cortex compared to other mammals.
So that's like the rough division, and you obviously can't understand the neocortex
completely isolated, but you can understand a lot of it with just a few interfaces,
to the all parts of the brain.
And so it gives you a system to study.
The other remarkable thing about the neocortex compared to the old parts of the brain
is the neocortex is extremely uniform. It's not visually or anatomically or it's very, it's like
I always like to say it's like the size of a dinner napkin about two and a half millimeters thick
and it looks remarkably the same everywhere. Everywhere you look in that two and a half millimeters
is this detailed architecture and it looks remarkably the same everywhere. Everywhere you look in that two and a half millimeters is this detailed architecture,
and it looks remarkably the same everywhere.
And that's a cross-species,
the mouse versus a cat, and a dog and a human.
Where if you look at the old parts of the brain,
there's lots of little pieces of specific things.
So it's like the old parts of a brain evolved,
like this is a part that controls heart rate,
and this is the part that controls this,
and this is this kind of thing, and that's this kind of thing.
And these evolve for eons a long, long time,
and they have their specific functions.
And all of a sudden, mammals come along,
and they got this thing called the Neocortex.
And it got large by just replicating
the same thing over and over and over again.
This is like, wow, this is incredible.
So all the evidence we have, and this
is an idea that was first articulated in a very
cogent and beautiful argument by a guy named Vernon Malkassel in 1978, I think it was,
that the Neo-Cortex all works on the same principle.
So language, hearing, touch, vision, engineering, all these things are basically
underlying, all built in the same computational substrate. They're really all the same problem.
So did low level of building blocks all look similar?
Yeah, and they're not even that low level. We're not talking about like neurons. We're
talking about this very complex circuit that exists throughout the near cortex is remarkably
similar. It is, it's like, yes, you see variations of it here and there, more of the cell,
less and less and less and less and less and, more of the cell, less and less, I'll, so on.
But what Moundcast argued was, it says,
you know, if you take a section on your cortex,
why is one of visual area and one is a auditory area,
or why is it, and his answer was,
it's because one is connected to eyes
and one is connected to ears.
Literally, you mean just it's most closest
in terms of the number of connections to the sensor?
Literally, literally, if you took the optic nerve
and attached to a different part of the cortex,
that part would become a visual region.
This experiment was actually done by Margonca Sir.
Oh, boy.
And in developing, I think it was lemur,
as I can't remember what it was, it's some animal.
And there's a lot of evidence to this.
You know, if you take a blind person, a person is born, blind at birth.
They're born with a visual neocortex.
It doesn't, may not get any input from the eyes because of some, I can general defect
or something.
And that region does stuff something else.
It picks up another task.
So, and it's, so it's this, it's this very complex
thing. It's not like, oh, they're all built on neurons. No, they're all built in this very complex
circuit and somehow that circuit underlies everything. And so this is the, it's called the
common-cortical algorithm, if you will. Some scientists just find it hard to believe, and they decide, I can't believe that's true,
but the evidence is overwhelming in this case.
And so a large part of what it means
to figure out how the brain creates intelligence
and what does intelligence in the brain
is to understand what that circuit does.
If you can figure out what that circuit does,
as amazing as it is, then you can understand
what all these other cognitive functions are.
So if you were to sort of put neuro cortex outside of your book on intelligence, you look,
if you wrote a giant poem, a textbook on the neuro cortex, and you look maybe a couple centuries from now,
how much of what we know now would still be accurate to centuries from now?
So how close are we in terms of understanding?
I have to speak for my own particular experience here.
So I run a small research lab here.
It's like, it's like,
any other research lab,
I'm the sort of the principal investigator,
there's actually two of us and there's a bunch of other people.
And this is what we do.
We started the near cortex
and we published our results and so on.
So about three years ago,
we had a real breakthrough in this field.
It's tremendous, but we've now published I think three papers on it.
And so I have a pretty good understanding of all the pieces and what we're missing.
I would say that almost all the empirical data we've collected about the brain, which is enormous.
If you don't know the neuroscience literature, it's just incredibly big.
And it's for the most part all correct.
It's facts and experimental results and measurements and all kinds of stuff.
But none of that has been really assimilated into a theoretical framework. It's data in the language of Thomas Kuhn, the historian, would be sort of a pre-paradigm
science.
Lots of data, but a way to fit it together.
I think almost all of that's correct.
There's going to be some mistakes in there.
And for the most part, there aren't really good, cogent theories about how to put it together.
It's not like we have two or three competing good theories,
which ones are right and which ones are wrong.
It's like, yeah, people just scratch in their heads,
probably means some people can be up and trying to figure out
what the whole thing does.
And in fact, there's very, very few labs that we do.
That focus really on theory and all this unassimulated data
and trying to explain it.
So it's not like we've got it wrong.
It's just that we haven't got it at all.
So it's really, I would say, pretty early days
in terms of understanding the fundamental theories,
forces of the way our mind works.
I don't think so.
I would have said that's true five years ago.
So as I said, we had some really big breakfasts on this recently and we started publishing
papers on this.
So, we'll get to that.
But, so, I don't think it's, I'm an optimist and from where I sit today, most people would
disagree with this, but from where I sit today, from what I know, it's not super early days
anymore.
We are, it's, you know, the way these things go is it's not a linear path, right?
You don't just start accumulating and get better and better,
but no, you can't, all this stuff you've collected,
none of it makes sense.
All these different things are just sort of around it.
And then you're gonna have some breaking points
where all of a sudden, oh my God, now we got it, right?
That's how it goes in science.
And I personally feel like we passed that little thing
about a couple of years ago.
All that big thing a couple of years ago.
So we can talk about that.
Time will tell if I'm right, but I feel very confident about it.
That's from London to say it on tape like this.
At least very optimistic.
So let's, before those few years ago, let's take, take us back to HTM, the Herarchical
Temporal Memory Theory, which you first proposed on intelligence.
It went through a few different generations. Can you describe what it is, how it
evolved through the three generations since you first put it on paper?
Yeah, so one of the things that neuroscientists just sort of missed for many, many
years, and especially people who were thinking about theory, was the nature of
time in the brain.
Brain's process, information through time, the information coming into the brain
is constantly changing.
The patterns from my speech right now,
if you're listening to it at normal speed,
would be changing on your ears
about every 10 milliseconds of cell you'd have a change.
Disconstant flow.
When you look at the world, your eyes are moving constantly,
three to five times a second, and the input's completely completely. If I were to touch something
like a coffee cup, as I move my fingers, the input's changed. So this idea that the brain
works on time-changing patterns is almost completely or was almost completely missing from a lot
of the basic theories, like fears of vision and so on. It's like, oh no, we're going to
put this image in front of you and flash it and say, what is it? A convolutional neural networks work that way
today, right? You know, classify this picture. But that's not what vision is like. Vision
is this sort of crazy time-based pattern that's going all over the place, and so is touch
and so is hearing. So the first part of a hierarchical temporal memory was the temporal part.
It's the same. You won't understand the brain, nor will you understand intelligent machines
unless you're dealing with time-based patterns.
The second thing was the memory component of it was,
is to say that we aren't just processing input.
We learn a model of the world.
And the memory stands for that model.
We have to, the point of the brain,
the point of the neocorticism, it learns a model of the world. We have to, the point of the brain, the point of the Neo-Cortez, it learns a model of the world.
We have to store things that are experiences
in a form that leads to a model of the world.
So we can move around the world,
we can pick things up and do things
and navigate and know how it's going on.
So that's what the memory referred to.
And many people just, they were thinking about
like certain processes without memory at all.
They're just like processing things.
And then finally, the hierarchical component
was a reflection to that the Neo-Coreject,
so though it's just uniform sheet of cells,
different parts of it project to other parts,
which project to other parts.
And there is a sort of rough hierarchy in terms of that.
So the hierarchical temple memory is just saying,
look, we should be thinking about the brain
as time-based,
model memory-based and hierarchical processing.
That was a placeholder for a bunch of components
that we would then plug into that.
We still believe all those things like just said,
but we now know so much more that I'm
stopping the user with high-rotumple memory yet
because it's
insufficient to capture the stuff we know.
So again, it's not incorrect, but I know no more and I would rather describe it more
accurately.
Yeah.
So you're basically, we could think of HTML as emphasizing that there's three aspects
of intelligence that are important to think about whatever the, whatever the eventual theory
converges to.
So in terms of time, how do you think of nature of time across different timescales?
You mentioned things changing, sensory and post-changing every 10-20 minutes.
What about every few minutes, every few months and years?
Well, if you think about a neuroscience problem, the brain problem. Neurons themselves can stay active for certain
periods of time. They can grow parts of the brain with this doctor for minutes.
You know, so you could hold a certain perception or activity for a certain
part of time, but not most of them don't last that long. And so if you think about
your thoughts are the activity neurons, if you're going to
want to involve something that happened a long time ago, even just this morning, for example,
the neurons haven't been active throughout that time. So you have to store that. So by
asking you, what did you have for breakfast today? That is memory. You built it into your
model of the world now. You remember that. And that memory is in the synapses, basically, in the formation of synapses. And so, you're sliding into what you know, used to different
time scales. There's time scales of which we are like understanding my language and moving
about and seeing things rapidly and over time. That's the time scales of activities and neurons.
But if you want to get in longer time scales, then it's more memory. And we have to invoke
those memories to say, oh, yes
Well, now I can remember what I had for breakfast because I stored that someplace. I may forget it tomorrow
But I'd stored for it for now. So this memory also need to have
the hierarchical
Aspect of reality is not just about concepts. It's also about time. Do you think of it that way?
Yeah, time is infused in everything. It's like you really can't separate it out. If I ask you what is
what is your you know, how's the brain learn a model of this coffee cup here? I have a coffee cup
and then I have to coffee up. I say well time is not an inherent property of this of this of the
model I have of this cup, whether it's a visual model or a tactile model.
I can sense it through time, but the model itself
doesn't have that much time.
If I asked you, if I said, well, what is the model of my cell phone?
My brain has learned a model of the cell phones.
If you have a smartphone like this.
And I said, well, this has time aspects to it.
I have expectations when I turn it on, what's going to happen,
and what the water, how long it's going to take
to do certain things.
If I bring up an app, what sequences.
And so I have an instant, it's like melodies in the world.
I have melody has a sense of time.
So many things in the world move and act,
and there's a sense of time related to them.
Some don't.
But most things do, actually.
So it's sort of infused throughout the models of the world.
You build a model world, you're learning the structure
of the objects in the world,
and you're also learning how those things change through time.
Okay, so it really is just a fourth dimension
that's infused deeply.
And you have to make sure that your models
of intelligence
incorporate it.
So like you mentioned, the state of neuroscience
is deeply empirical.
A lot of data collection.
It's, you know, that's where it is.
You mentioned Thomas Kuhn, right?
Yeah.
And then you're proposing a theory of intelligence,
and which is really the next step,
the really important step to take.
But why is HTML or what we'll talk about soon,
the right theory?
So is it backed by intuition?
Is it backed by evidence? Is it backed by evidence?
Is it backed by a mixture of both?
Is it kind of closer to or strength theories in physics?
Where this mathematical components
which show that, you know, what it seems that this,
it fits together too well for it not to be true.
Which is what we're strength theories.
Is that where your condescending?
It's a mixture of all those things, although definitely where we are right now, it's definitely
much more on the empirical side than let's say string theory.
The way this goes about, we're theorists, right?
So we look at all this data and we're trying to come up with some sort of model that's
some explained to basically, and there's unlike string theory, there's this vast more amounts of empirical data here
that I think than most physicists deal with.
And so our challenge is to sort through that
and figure out what kind of constructs would explain this.
And when we have an idea,
you come up with a theory of some sort,
you have lots of ways of testing it.
First of all, there are 100 years of underestimulated empirical data from neuroscience.
So we go back and repapers and we say, oh, did someone find this already?
We can predict x, y, and z, and maybe no one's even talked about it since 1972 or something,
but we go back and find that. And we say, oh, either it can support the theory
or it can invalidate the theory.
And we say, OK, we have to start over again.
Oh, no, it's support.
Let's keep going with that one.
So the way I kind of view it, when we do our work,
we look at all this empirical data.
And it's what I call it as a set of constraints.
We're not interested in something that's biologically inspired.
We're trying to figure out how the actual brain works.
So every piece of empirical data is a constraint on a theory.
If you have the correct theory, it needs to explain every pin, right?
So we have this huge number of constraints on the problem,
which initially makes it very, very difficult.
If you don't have any constraints, you can make up stuff all the day.
You can say, oh, here's an answer, how you can do this don't have any constraints, you can make up stuff all the day.
You can say, oh, here's an answer to how you can do this.
You can do that, you can do this.
But if you consider all biology, as a set of constraints,
all neuroscience, as a set of constraints,
and even if you're working in one little part
of the Neocortex, for example,
there are hundreds and hundreds of constraints.
These are empirical constraints
that it's very, very difficult initially
to come up with a theoretical framework for that.
But when you do, and it solves all those constraints at once, you have a high confidence that you
got something close to correct.
It's just mathematically almost impossible not to be.
So that's the curse and the advantage of what we have.
The curse is we have to solve, we have to meet all these constraints, which is really hard.
But when you do meet them, then you have a great confidence that you've discovered something.
In addition, then we work with scientific labs. So we'll say, oh, there's something we can't find,
we can predict something, but we can't find it anywhere in the literature. So we will then,
we have people we collaborated with, we'll say, sometimes they'll say,
you know what, I have some collected data,
which I didn't publish, but we can go back and look in it
and see if we can find that, which is much easier
than designing a new experiment.
You know, neuroscientist experiments take a long time,
years.
So although some people are doing that now too.
So, but between all of these things,
I think it's a reasonable, actually a very, very good
approach.
We are blessed with the fact that we can test our theories out the Yang Yang here because
there's so much on a similar data, and we can also falsify our theories, right, usually,
which we do often.
So, it's kind of reminiscent to whenever that was with Copernicus, you know, when you figure out that the Suns at the center,
the solar system was supposed to earth, the piece is just fallen to place.
Yeah, I think that's the general nature of the Ha moment.
Is in his Copernicus, it could be, you could say the same thing about Darwin.
You could say the same thing about, you know, about the double helix, that people have been working
on a problem for so long, and have all this data and they can't make sense of it.
But when the answer comes to you and everything falls into place, it's like, oh my gosh,
that's it.
That's got to be right.
I asked both Jim Watson and Francis Crick about this.
I asked them, you know, when you were working on
trying to discover the structure of the double helix,
and when you came up with the sort of,
the structure that ended up being correct,
but it was sort of a guess, you know,
it wasn't really verified yet.
I said, did you know that it was right?
And they both said, absolutely. did you know that it was right? And they both said absolutely.
We absolutely knew it was right. And it doesn't matter if other people didn't believe it or not.
We knew it was right. They'd get around the thing and agree with it eventually anyway.
And that's the kind of thing you hear a lot with scientists who really are studying a difficult problem.
And I feel that way too about our work. If you talk to Crick, Watson, about the problem you're trying to solve,
the of finding the DNA of the brain.
Yeah. In fact, Francis Crick was very interested in this in the latter part of his
life. And in fact, I got interested in brains by reading an essay he wrote in 1979,
call thinking about the Brain. And that
is when I decided I'm going to leave my profession of computers and engineering and become a neuroscientist,
just reading that one essay from Francis Crack. I got to meet him later in life. I had,
I got to, I spoke at the Salk Institute and he was in the audience and then I had a
tea with him afterwards. He was interested in different problems. He was, he was in the audience and then I had a two with him afterwards.
He was interested in a different problem.
He was focused on consciousness.
Yeah, and that's not a easy problem, right?
Well, I think it's the red herring and so we weren't really overlapping along there.
Jim Watson, who is still alive, is also interested in this problem. And he was, when he was director of the
Colesman Harbor Laboratories, he was really sort of
behind moving in the direction of neuroscience there.
And so he had a personal interest in this field.
And I have met with him numerous times.
And in fact, the last time was a little bit over a year ago,
I gave a talk at Cosmine Harbor Labs
about the progress we were making in our work. And it was a lot of fun because he said,
well, you wouldn't be coming here unless you had something important to say, so I'm going to go
to the tender talk. So he sat in the very front row. Next to him was the director of the lab, Bruce Stillman. So these guys are in the front row of this auditorium, right? So nobody sat in the very front row. Next one was the director of the lab, Bruce Stillman.
So these guys are in the front row of this auditorium, right?
So nobody else in the auditorium
wants to sit in the front row,
because Jim Watson is the director of the,
and I gave a talk and I had dinner with Jim afterwards.
But there's a great picture of my colleague,
Subitone Hadtick, where I'm up there
sort of like screaming the basics of this new framework we have.
And Jim Watson's on the edge of his chair.
He's literally on the edge of his chair,
like, intently staring up at the screen.
And when he discovered the structure of DNA,
the first public talk he gave was at Colesping Harbor Labs.
And there's a picture, there's a famous picture
of Jim Watson standing up the
I-board with an overrated thing pointing at something with the funding of Double Heel
X to this pointer.
And it actually looks a lot like the picture of me.
So, there was a sort of funny, there's the area I'm talking about the brain and there's
Jim Watson standing up, tentally I didn't, of course there was, you know, whatever, 60
years earlier he was standing, you know, pointing at the Double Heel X.
And it's one of the great discoveries in all of, you know, whatever, by all its science,
all science, DNA.
So, it's the funny that there's echoes of that in your presentation.
Do you think in terms of evolutionary timeline and history, the development of the New York
cortex was a big leap?
Or is it just a small step?
So like if we ran a whole thing over again from the from the
birth of life on earth, how likely would we develop the mechanism of the New
Acquired? Okay, well those are two separate questions. One was it a big leap and
one was how like it is. Okay, they're not necessarily related. They'd be correlated.
They'd be correlated. We don't really have enough data to make a judgment about
that. I would say definitely was a big leap and I can tell you why I think I don't think it was
just another incremental step.
I'll get that in a moment.
I don't really have any idea how likely it is.
If we look at evolution, we have one data point, which is Earth, right?
Life formed on Earth billions of years ago, whether it was introduced here or it created
here or someone introduced it, we don't really know, whether it was introduced here or it created here, or someone introduced
that we don't really know, but it was here early. It took a long, long time to get to multicellular
life. And then from multiteilular life, it took a long, long time to get the Zinger cortex.
And we've only had the New York cortex for a few hundred thousand years. So that's like nothing.
for a few hundred thousand years. So that's like nothing.
So is it likely?
Well, certainly isn't something that happened right away on Earth.
And there were multiple steps to get there.
So I would say it's probably not going to be something that would happen instantly
some other planets that might have life.
It might take several billion years on average.
Is it likely?
I don't know.
But you'd have to survive for several billion years to find out.
Probably. Is it a big leap? Yeah, I think it's, it is a qualitative difference in all other
evolutionary steps. I can try to describe that, if you didn't like. Sure, in which way.
Yeah, I can tell you how. Pretty much, let's start with a little preface. Many of the things that humans are able to do do not have obvious
survival advantages precedent.
You know, we create music. Is that, is there a really survival advantage to that?
Maybe, maybe not. What about mathematics? Is there a real survival advantage to mathematics?
You can stretch it.
You can try to figure these things out, right?
But mostly evolutionary history, everything
had immediate survival advantages to them.
So I'll tell you a story, which I like, may not be true.
But the story goes as follows.
Organisms have been evolving since the beginning of life here on Earth. Adding this sort of complexity onto that and the brain itself is evolved this way.
In fact, there's an older part to the brain that kind of just keeps calming on new things
and we keep adding capabilities.
And we got to the Neo Cortex.
Initially, it had a very clear survival advantage
in that it produced better vision
and better hearing and better touch,
and maybe you can say so on.
But what I think happens is that evolution just got,
it took a mechanism, and this is in our recent theory,
but it took a mechanism that evolved
a long time ago for navigating in the world, for knowing where you are.
These are the so-called grid cells and place cells
of an old part of the brain.
And it took that mechanism for building maps of the world
and knowing where you are in those maps
and how to navigate those maps.
And it turns it into a sort of a slim down idealized version
of it. And that idealized version of it.
And that idealized version could not apply to building maps of other things.
Maps of coffee cups and maps of phones, maps of, you know, maps of the concepts.
Yes, and not just almost exactly.
And so you, and it just started replicating this stuff, right?
You just think more and more and more.
So we went from being sort of dedicated purpose neural hardware to solve certain problems that are important to survival
to a general purpose neural hardware that could be applied to all problems. And now it's
escaped the orbit of survival. We are now able to apply it to things which we find enjoyment.
apply it to things which we find enjoyment,
but aren't really clearly
survival characteristics. And that seems only have happened in humans
to the large extent.
And so that's what's going on,
where we sort of have,
we've sort of escaped the gravity
of evolutionary pressure in some sense,
in the near cortex.
And it now does things that are really interesting,
discovering models of the universe,
which may not really help us.
Doesn't matter, how is it help us surviving,
knowing that there might be multiverses,
or there might be the age of the universe,
or how various stellar things occur.
It doesn't really help us survive at all.
But we enjoy it, and that's what happened.
Or at least not in the obvious way, perhaps it is required.
If you look at the entire universe and the evolution
in a way, it's required for us to do interplanetary travel
and therefore survive past our own son.
But you know, let's not get too...
But evolution works at one time frame.
It's survival.
If you think of survival of the phenotype, survival of the individual.
Right, exactly.
That way, you're talking about there is spanned whale beyond that.
So there's no genetic.
I'm not transferring any genetic traits to my children that are going to help them survive
better on Mars.
Right.
It's all the different mechanisms in this room.
Yeah. So let's get into the new, as you've mentioned,
this idea, I don't know if you have a nice name,
1000,
I'll be calling it the Thousand Brain Theory of Intelli.
I like it.
So can you talk about this idea of space of your concepts
and so on?
Yeah.
So can I just describe sort of the,
there's an underlying core discovery, which then everything
comes from that.
It's a very simple, this is really what happened.
We were deep into problems about understanding how we build models of stuff in the world
and how we make predictions about things.
And I was holding a coffee cup just like this in my hand.
And I had my finger was touching the side,
my index finger, and then I moved it to the top,
and I was gonna feel the rim at the top of the cup.
And I asked myself a very simple question.
I said, well, first of all, let's say,
I know that my brain predicts what it's gonna feel
before it touches it.
You can just think about it and imagine it.
And so we know that the brain's making predictions
all the time.
So the question is, what does it take to predict that?
And there's a very interesting answer.
First of all, it says, the brain has
to know it's touching a coffee cup.
It has to have a model of a coffee cup.
And needs to know where the finger currently
is on the cup relative to the cup.
Because when I make a movement, and you
just know where it's going to be on the cup,
after the movement is completed, relative to the cup. And then it can make a just know where it's going to be on the cup, after the movement is completed, relative to the cup,
and then it can make a prediction about what it's going to sense.
So this told me that the neocortex,
which is making this prediction,
needs to know that it's sensing, it's touching a cup,
and it needs to know the location
of my finger relative to that cup,
in a reference frame of the cup.
It doesn't matter where the cup is relative.
My body, it doesn't matter, it's orientation.
None of that matters. It's where my finger is relative to the cup, which tells me then that the
neocortex has a reference frame that's anchored to the cup, because otherwise I wouldn't be able to
say the location and I wouldn't be able to predict my new location. And then we quickly, very instantly,
instantly, you can say, well, every part of my skin could touch this cup and therefore every
part of my skin is making predictions and every part of my skin could touch this cup. And therefore, every part of my skin is making predictions. And every part of my skin must have a reference frame
that it's using to make predictions.
So the big idea is that, throughout the neocortex,
there are, everything is being stored
and referenced in reference frames.
You can think of them like XY, y, z reference frames,
but they're not like that.
We know a lot about the neural mechanisms for this,
but the brain thinks in reference frames.
And as an engineer, if you're an engineer,
this is not surprising.
You say, if I were under the build a CAD model
of the coffee cup, well, I would bring it up
in some CAD software and I would assign some reference
frame and say, this features at this location, and so on.
But the fact that this idea that this is occurring out in your cortex everywhere,
it was a novel idea. And then a zillion things fell into place after that. A zillion.
So now we think about the near cortex as processing information quite differently than we used to do it.
We used to think about the nearx as processing sensory data and extracting features
from that sensory data and then extracting features
from the features very much like a deep learning
network does today.
But that's not how the brain works at all.
The brain works by assigning everything,
every input, everything to reference frames.
And there are thousands, hundreds, thousands of them
active at once in your New York Courtax.
It's a surprising thing to think about, but once you've sort of internalized this, you
can understand that it explains almost all the mysteries we've had about this structure.
So one of the consequences of that is that every small part of the neocortex, so you
have a millimeter square, and there's 150,000 of those.
So it's about 150,000 square millimeters. If you take every little square
millimeter of the cortex, it's got some input coming into it and it's going to have reference frames
which assign that input to and each square millimeter can learn complete models of objects. So what
do I mean by that? If I'm touching the coffee cup, well, if I just touch it in one place, I can't
learn what this coffee cup is because I'm just feeling one part.
But if I move it around the cup and touch it in different areas, I can build up a complete
model of the cup because I'm now filling in that three-dimensional map, which is the coffee
cup.
I can say, oh, what am I feeling at all these different locations?
That's the basic idea.
It's more complicated than that.
But so through time, and we talked about time earlier, through time, even a single column
which is only looking at or a single part a single column, which is only looking at,
or a single part of the cortex,
which is only looking at a small part of the world,
can build up a complete model of an object.
And so if you think about the part of the brain,
which is getting input from all my fingers,
so there's spread across the top of your head here.
This is the somatosensory cortex.
There's columns associated,
all there are from areas of my skin.
And what we believe is happening is that all of them are building models of this cup,
every one of them, or things.
They're not all building, not every column or every part of the cortex builds models
of everything.
But they're all building models of something.
And so you have, it's so when I touch this cup with my hand, there are multiple models
of the cup being invoked. If I look at it with my eyes, there there are multiple models of the cup being invoked.
If I look at it with my eyes,
there are again many models of the cup being invoked
because each part of the visual system,
the brain doesn't process an image.
That's a misleading idea.
It's just like your fingers touching the cup,
so different parts of my retina are looking at
different parts of the cup.
And thousands and thousands of models of the cup
are being invoked at once.
And they're all voting with each other,
trying to figure out what's going on.
So that's why we call it the Thousand Brains Theory,
even intelligence because there isn't one model of the cup.
There are thousands of models to this cup.
There are thousands of models that you're phone
and about cameras and microphones and so on.
It's a distributed modeling system,
which is very different than what people have thought about it.
And so that's a really compelling and interesting idea.
I have two first questions.
So one, on the ensemble part of everything coming together,
you have these thousand brains.
How do you know which one has done the best job
of forming the...
Great question.
Let me try to explain.
There's a problem that's known in neuroscience called
the sense of fusion problem.
Yes.
And so the idea is something like,
oh, the image comes from the eye.
There's a picture on the retina.
And it gets projected to the neocortex.
Oh, by now, it's all sped out all over the place.
And it's kind of squirrely and distorted.
And pieces are all over the, you know, it doesn't look like a picture anymore.
When does it all come back together again, right?
Or you might say, well, yes, but I also have sounds or touches associated with the cup.
So I'm seeing the cup and touching the cup.
How do they get combined together again?
So it's called the sense of fusion problem.
As if all these disparate parts have to be brought together into one model someplace.
That's the wrong idea.
The right idea is that you get all these guys voting.
There's auditory models of the cup,
there's visual models of the cup,
there's tactile models of the cup. There are one, of the cup, there's tactile models of the cup.
There are one, the individual system, there might be ones that are more focused on black and white,
ones forging on color, it doesn't really matter.
There's just thousands and thousands of models of this cup, and they vote.
They don't actually come together in one spot.
Just literally think of it this way.
Imagine you have these columns, or like about the size of a little piece of spaghetti,
like a two and a half millimeters tall
and about a millimeter in my.
They're not physical, but you can think of them that way.
And each one's trying to guess what this thing is or touching.
Now, they can do a pretty good job
if they're allowed to move over a touch.
So I can reach my hand into a black box
and move my finger around an object
and if I touch enough space, it's like,
oh, okay, I don't know what it is.
But often, we don't do that.
Often I can just reach and grab something
with my hand all at once and I get it.
Or if I had to look through the world through a straw,
so I'm only invoking one little column,
I can only see a part of something
and I have to move the straw around.
But if I open my eyes, I see the whole thing at once.
So what we think is going on is all these little pieces,
beget if you have all these little columns in the cortex,
or all trying to guess what it is that they're sensing.
They'll do a better guess if they have time
and can move over time,
so if I move my eyes or move my fingers.
But if they don't, they have a poor guess.
It's a probabilistic guess of what they might be touching.
Now imagine they can post their probability
at the top of a little piece of spaghetti.
Each one of them says,
and it's not really a probability distribution.
It's more like a set of possibilities.
In the brain, it doesn't work as a probability of
distribution.
It works as more like what we call a union.
You could say, in one column says, I think it could be a
coffee cup, soda can, or a water bottle.
And another column says, I think it could be a coffee cup,
or telephone, or a camera, whatever, right?
And all these guys are saying what they think it might be.
And there's these long range connections
in certain layers in the cortex.
So there's some layers in some cell types in each column
send the projections across the brain.
And that's the voting occurs.
And so there's a simple associate of memory mechanism.
We've described this in a recent paper
and we've modeled this.
That says they can all quickly settle on the only or the one best answer for all of them. If there is
a single best answer, they all vote and say, yep, it's got to be the coffee cup. And at
that point, they all know it's a coffee cup. And at that point, everyone acts as if it's
the coffee cup. They, yep, we know it's a coffee, even though I've only seen one little
piece of this world, I know it's a coffee cup I'm touching or I'm seeing or whatever.
And so you can think of all these columns
are looking at different parts in different places,
different sensory input, different locations,
they're all different, but this layer that's doing the voting,
that's it's solidifies.
It's just like it crystallizes and says,
oh, we all know what we're doing.
And so you don't bring these models together in one model,
you just vote and there's a crystallization of the vote.
Great.
That's at least a compelling way to think about the way you form a model of the world.
Now you talk about a coffee cup.
Do you see this as far as I understand that you were proposing this as well, that this extends
to much more than coffee cups?
Yeah.
That does.
Or at least the physical world, it expands to the world of concepts.
Yeah, it does.
And well, the first, the prime of the face of every evidence for that is that the regions
of the neocortex that are associated with language or high level thought or mathematics
or things like that, they look like the regions of the neocortex that process vision
and hearing and touch.
They don't look any different.
Or they look only marginally different.
And so one would say, well,
if Vernon Mount Castle, who proposed it all,
all the parts of the Neocortex doing the same thing,
if he's right, then the parts that do
in language or mathematics or physics
are working on the same principle.
They must be working on the principle of reference frames.
So that's a little odd thought.
But of course, we had no prior idea how these things
happen.
So let's go with that.
And in our recent paper, we talked a little bit about that.
I've been working on it more since.
I have better ideas about it now.
I'm sitting here, I'm very confident
that that's what's happening.
And I can give you some examples to help you think about that. It's not we understand it completely,
but I understand it better than I've described it in any paper so far. But we did put that idea out
there. It's a good place to start, you know, and the evidence would suggest it's how it's happening.
And then we can start tackling that problem one piece at a time. Like, what does it mean to do
high level thought? What does it mean to do high level thought?
What does it mean to do language?
How would that fit into a reference frame framework?
Yeah, so there's a, I don't know if you could tell me
if there's a connection, but there's
an app called Anki that helps you remember
different concepts.
And they talk about like a memory palace that helps you
remember a completely random concepts by sort of trying
to put them in a physical space in your mind and putting them next to each other.
Well, the method of low-key.
Low-key, that.
For some reason that seems to work really well.
Yeah.
Now that's a very narrow kind of application of just remembering some facts.
But that's not, but that's a very, very telling one.
Okay, exactly.
Yeah, exactly.
So, this seems like you're describing a mechanism why this seems to work.
Yes. So basically the way what we think is going on is all things you know, all concepts,
all ideas, words, everything you know are stored in reference frames. And so if you want to
remember something, you have to basically navigate through a reference frame, the same way
a rat navigates to a mage, and the same way my finger navigates to this coffee cup.
You're moving through some space.
And so, if you have a random list of things you would ask to remember, by assigning them
to a reference frame, you already know very well to see your house.
Right?
Now, idea of the method of low-keyes, you can say, okay, in my lobby, I'm going to put this
thing.
And then the bedroom, I put this one, I go down the hall, I put this thing.
And then you want to recall those facts.
So to recall those things, you just walk mentally, you walk through your house.
You're mentally moving through a reference frame that you already had.
And that tells you, there's two things that's really important about that.
It tells us the brain prefers to store things in reference frames.
And the method of recalling things or thinking, if you will, is to move
mentally through those reference frames. You could move physically through
some reference frames, like I could physically move through the reference
frame of this coffee cup. I can also mentally move through the reference
frame of the coffee cup, imagining me touching it. But I can also mentally move my
house. And so now we can ask yourself, or are all concepts stored this way?
There was some recent research using human subjects in FMRI, and I'm going to apologize for
not knowing the name of the scientists who did this.
But what they did is they put humans in this FMRI machine, which is one of these imaging
machines, and they gave the humans tasks to think about birds.
So they had different types of birds, and birds that looked big and small,
and long legs, and long legs, things like that.
And what they could tell from the FMRI,
was a very clever experiment.
They could tell when humans were thinking about the birds,
that the knowledge of birds was arranged in a reference frame,
similar to the ones that are used when you navigate in a room.
That these are called grid cells and there are grid cells like patterns of activity in the neocortex when they do this. So that it's a very clever experiment, you know, and what it basically
says is that even when you're thinking about something abstract and you're not really thinking about
as a reference frame, it tells us the brain is actually using a reference frame. And it's using
the same neural mechanisms. These grid cells, are the basic same neural
mechanisms that we've proposed that grid cells which exist in the old part of the brain,
the antihonac cortex, that that mechanism is now similar mechanism is used throughout
the near cortex.
It's the same nature that preserved this interesting way of creating reference frames.
And so now they have empirical evidence that when you think about concepts
like birds that you're using reference frames that are built on grid cells. So that's similar
to the method of low chi, but in this case the birds are related so it makes they create
their own reference frame which is consistent with bird space. And when you think about
something you go through that, you can make the same example, let's take a mathematics.
All right, let's say you want to prove a conjecture.
What is a conjecture?
Conjecture is a statement you believe to be true,
but you haven't proven it.
And so it might be an equation.
I want to show that this is equal to that.
And you have some places you start with.
You say, well, I know this is true, and I know this is true.
And I think that maybe to get to the final proof,
I need to go through some intermediate results.
But I believe it's happening.
It's literally these equations where these points
are assigned to a reference frame, a mathematical reference
frame.
And when you do mathematical operations,
a simple one might be multiplied or divide,
but you might be able to pass, transform, or something else.
That is like a movement in the reference frame of the math.
And so you're literally trying to discover a path from one location to another location
in a space of mathematics.
And if you can get to these intermediate results, then you know your map is pretty good,
and you know you're using the right operations.
Much of what we think about is solving hard problems is designing the correct reference frame
for that
problem.
Figuring out how to organize the information and what behaviors I want to use in that space
to get me there.
Yeah, so if you dig in on an idea of this reference frame, whether it's the math, you start a set
of axioms to try to get to proving the conjecture.
Can you try to describe maybe taking a back, how you think of the reference
frame in that context?
Is it the reference frame that the axioms are happy in?
Is it the reference frame that might contain everything?
Is it a changing thing as you have many, many reference frames?
In fact, the way the theory, the thousand brain theory of intelligence says that every
single thing in the world has its own reference frame.
So every word has its own reference frame.
And we can talk about this, the mathematics workout, this is no problem for neurons to do
this.
But how many reference frames does the coffee cup have?
Well, it's on a table.
Let's say you ask how many reference frames could the column in my finger that's touching
the coffee cup hat because there are many, many,
copy, there are many, many models of the coffee cup. So the coffee, there is no one model
of the coffee cup. There are many models of the coffee cup and you can say, well, how many different
things can my finger learn? It's a question you want to ask. Imagine I say every concept, every idea,
everything you've ever know about that you can say, I know that thing. It has a reference frame
associated with it. And what we do when we build composite objects, we assign reference frames to point another
reference frame.
So my coffee cup has multiple components to it.
It's got a limb, it's got a cylinder, it's got a handle.
And those things have their own reference frames and they're assigned to a master reference
frame, which is called this cup.
And now I have this fundamental logo on it.
That's something that exists elsewhere in the world. It's its own thing. So it has its own reference frame, which is called this cup. And now I have this fundamental logo on it. Well, that's something that exists elsewhere in the world.
It's its own thing.
So it has its own reference frame.
So we need to have to say, how can I sign the
Neumente logo reference frame onto the cylinder or onto the
coffee cup.
So we talked about this in the paper that came out in
December of this last year.
The idea of how you can sign reference frames
to reference frames, how neurons could do this?
So my question is,
even though you mentioned reference frames a lot,
I almost feel it's really useful to dig into
how you think of what a reference frame is.
I mean, it was already helpful for me to understand
that you think of reference frames
as something there is a lot of.
Okay, so let's just say that we're gonna have
some neurons in the brain, not many actually,
10,000, 20,000 are gonna create
a whole bunch of reference frames.
What does it mean?
What is a reference frame?
First of all, these reference frames
are different than the ones you might have used to.
We know lots of reference frames.
For example, we know the Cartesian coordinates,
XYZ, that's a type of reference frame.
We know logitudent latitude, that that's a type of reference frame. We know, longitude and latitude,
that's a different type of reference frame.
If I look at a printed map,
it might have columns A through M and rows,
one through 20, that's a different type of reference frame.
It's kind of a Cartesian point reference frame.
The interesting thing about the reference frames in the brain,
and we know this because these have been established through neuroscience studying the antironic cortex.
So I'm not speculating here, okay?
This is known neuroscience in an old part of the brain.
The way these cells create reference frames, they have no origin.
So what it's more like you have a point, a point in some space, and you give it a particular
movement, you can then tell
what the next point should be. And you can then tell what the next point would be. And
so on. You can use this to calculate how to get from one point to another. So how do I
get from my house to my home, or how do I get my finger from the side of my cup to the
top of the cup. How do I get from the axioms to the architecture?
So it's a different type of reference for him,
and if you want, I can describe in more detail,
I can paint a picture how you might want to think about that.
It's really helpful to think it's something you can move through.
But is it helpful to think of it as spatial in some sense,
or is there something more important? No, it's definitely spatial. Is it helpful to think of it as spatial in some sense,
or is there something more important? No, it's definitely spatial.
It's spatial in a mathematical sense.
How many dimension, can it be crazy number of dimensions?
Well, that's an interesting question.
In the old part of the brain, the Antorhonic cortex,
they studied rats, and initially it looks like,
oh, this is just two-dimensional.
It's like the rat is in some box and a maze or whatever,
and they know where the rat is using these two-dimensional reference frames and know where it is in
the rat. The maze. We said, okay, what about bats? That's a mammal and they fly in three-dimensional
space. How do they do that? They seem to know where they are, right? So this is a current
area of active research and it seems like somehow the neurons in the inter-rionic cortex can learn three-dimensional space.
We just, two members of our team, along with Ilif Fett
from MIT, just released a paper this little, literally,
last week, it's on bioarchive, where they show that you can,
if you, the way these things work, and unless you want to,
I won't get into the detail, but grid cells can represent any end-dimensional space.
It's not inherently limited.
You can think of it this way.
If you had two-dimensional, the way it works
is you had a bunch of two-dimensional slices.
That's the way these things work.
There's a whole bunch of two-dimensional models.
And you can just, you can slice up any end-dimensional space
with two-dimensional projections.
So, and you could have one-dimensional model.
So, there's nothing inherent about the mathematics,
about the way the neurons do this,
which constrain the dimensionality of the space,
which I think was important.
So obviously, I have a three-dimensional map
of this cup, maybe it's even more than that, I don't know.
But it's clearly three-dimensional map of the cup.
I don't just have a projection of the cup.
But when I think about birds or when I think about mathematics,
perhaps it's more than three dimensions.
Who knows?
So in terms of each individual column building up
more and more information over time,
do you think that mechanism is well understood
in your mind, you've proposed a lot of architectures there. Is that a key piece or is it?
Is the big piece the thousand brain
Theory of intelligence the ensemble of it all well. I think they're both big
I mean clearly the concept as a theorist the concept is most exciting, right?
We have a little concept of concept
This is a totally new way of thinking about other New York characteristics work
so that is appealing.
It has all these ramifications.
And with that, as a framework for how the brain works, you can make all kinds of predictions
and solve all kinds of problems.
Now we're trying to work through many of these details right now.
Okay, how do the neurons actually do this?
Well, in terms of, if you think about grid cells and play cells in the old parts of the
brain, there's a lot that's known about them, but there's still some mysteries.
There's a lot of debate about exactly the details, how these work and what are the brain. There's a lot to snowing about them, but there's still some mysteries. There's a lot of debate about exactly the details, how these work, and what are the
soons. And we have that still that same level of detail, that same level of concern.
What we spend here most of our time doing is trying to make a very good list of the things
we don't understand yet. That's the key part here. What are the constraints? It's not
like, oh, this seems seems work for done. Now, it's like, okay, it kind of works, but these are other things we know what has to do,
and it's not doing those yet. I would say we're well on the way here. We're not done yet.
There's a lot of trickiness to this system, but the basic principles about how different layers
in the near cortex are doing much of this, we understand, but there's some fundamental parts that we don't understand
as well. So what would you say is one of the harder open problems or one of the ones that have been
bothering you. Oh, oh, yeah. Keeping you up at night the most. Oh, well, right now this is a
detailed thing that wouldn't apply to most people. Okay. But you want me to ask that question? Yeah,
please. We've talked about as if, oh, to predict what you're
going to send some of this coffee cup,
I need to know where my finger is going to be on the coffee cup.
That is true, but it's insufficient.
Think about my finger touches the edge of the coffee
cup.
My finger can touch it at different orientations.
I can rotate my finger around here.
And that doesn't change.
I can make that prediction and somehow,
so it's not just the location,
there's an orientation component of this as well.
This is known in the old part of the brain too,
there's things called head direction cells,
which way the rat is facing,
it's the thing kind of basic idea.
So if my finger were a rat,
in three dimensions, I have a three-dimensional orientation
and I have a three-dimensional location.
If I was a rat, I would have a, when I think of it as a two-dimensional location, a two-dimensional orientation, and I have a three-dimensional location. If I was a rat, I would have a,
when I think of it as a two-dimensional location,
a two-dimensional orientation,
or one-dimensional orientation,
like just which way is it facing?
So how the two components work together,
how does it, I combine orientation,
the orientation of my sensor,
as well as the location,
is a tricky problem.
And I think I've made progress on it.
So at a bigger version of that,
so perspective is super interesting,
but super specific.
Yeah, I warned you.
No, no, no, it's really good,
but there's a more general version of that.
Do you think context matters,
the fact that we are in a building in North America that
we in the day and age where we have mugs?
I mean, there's all this extra information that you bring to the table about everything
else in the room that's outside of just the coffee cup.
Of course, how does it get connected to you?
Yeah, and that is another really interesting question. I'm going to throw that under the
rubric or the name of attention problems. First of all, we have this model. I have many,
many models. And also the question, does it matter?
Well, it matters for certain things. Of course, it does. Maybe what we think about as a coffee
cup and another part of the world is viewed as something completely different. Or maybe
our logo, which is very benign in this part of the world, it means something very
different in another part of the world.
So, those things do matter.
I think the way to think about this the following, one way to think about it, is we have all
these models of the world, okay.
And we model everything.
And as I said earlier, I kind of snuck it in there.
Our models are actually, we build composite structure.
So every object is composed of other objects,
which are composed of other objects,
and they become members of other objects.
So this room is chairs and a table and a room and a wall,
and so on.
Now we can just arrange them in these things
in a certain way and go, oh, that's the element
that compensate them.
So what we do is when we go around the world, these things are certain way you go, oh, that's the nement the conference room.
And what we do is when we go around the world, and we experience the world,
by walking into a room, for example,
the first thing I was going to say,
oh, I'm in this room, do I recognize the room?
Then I can say, oh, look, there's a table here.
And by attending to the table,
I'm then assigning this table in a context of the room.
And I said, oh, on the table, there's a coffee cup.
Oh, and on the table, there's a logo.
And in the logo, there's the word, nomenta.
Oh, and look in the logo, there's the letter E.
And look at it as an unusual syrup.
And it doesn't actually, but I'm in a pretend case.
So the point is, your attention is cut
a drilling deep in and out of these nested structures.
And I can pop back up, and I can pop back up and I can pop back down,
I can pop back up and I can pop back down. So when I attend to the coffee cup, I haven't lost
the context of everything else, but it's sort of a nested structure.
So the attention filters the reference frame formation for that particular period of time.
Yes, it basically, at moment to moment, you attend the sub-components,
and then you can attend the sub-components,
the sub-components.
You can move up and down that time.
You can move up and down that we do that all the time.
You're not even, not that I'm aware of it.
I'm very conscious of it, but,
but most people don't even think about this.
You just walk in room and you don't say,
oh, I looked at the chair and I looked at the board
and looked at that word on the board
and I looked over here, what's going on?
Right.
So what percentage of your day,
are you deeply aware of this,
and what part can you actually relax
and just be Jeff?
Me personally, like my personal day.
Yeah.
Unfortunately, I'm afflicted with too much of the former.
I think.
Well, unfortunately, they are unfortunate.
Yeah, so you don't think it's useful?
Oh, I did useful, totally useful.
I think about this stuff almost all the time.
And one of my primary ways of thinking
is when I'm in sleep at night, I always wake up
in the middle of the night.
And then I stay awake for at least an hour with my eyes shut
in a sort of a half-sleep state, thinking about these things.
I come up with answers to problems very often
in that sort of half-sleeping state.
I think about on my bike ride, I think about on a walk.
I'm just constantly thinking about this.
I have to almost schedule time to not think about the stuff
because it's very, it's mentally taxing.
When you're thinking about the stuff,
are you thinking introspectively,
like almost taking a step outside of yourself
and trying to figure out what is your mind doing right now?
I do that all the time, but that's not all I do.
I'm constantly observing myself.
So as soon as I started thinking about grid cells,
for example, and getting into that,
I started saying, oh, well, grid cells
can have my place of sense in the world,
that's where you know where you are.
And it's interesting, you know,
we always have a sense of where we are,
unless we're lost.
And so I started at night when I got up
to go to the bathroom, I would start trying to do
a complete of my eyes closed all the time,
and I would test my sense of grissails.
I would walk five feet and say, okay, I think I'm here.
Am I really there?
What's my error?
And then I would count my error again,
and see how the errors accumulate.
So even something as simple as getting up
in the middle of the night to go to the bathroom,
I'm testing these theories out.
It's kind of fun, and the coffee cup is an example
of that too.
So I think I find that these sort of everyday
introspection are actually quite helpful.
It doesn't mean you can ignore the science.
I mean, I spend hours every day reading
Ridiculousy Complex Papers.
That's not nearly as much fun, but you have to sort of build up
those constraints and the knowledge about the field
and who's doing what and what exactly they think
is not being here. And then you can sit back and say, okay, let's try to piece this all together.
Let's come up with some, you know, I'm very in this group here, people, they know they do this,
I do this all this time. I come in with these introspective ideas and say, well,
they're never thought about this. Now, watch, well, this all do this together.
And it's helpful. It's not, as long as you do, you don't, if you all you did was
that, then you're just making up stuff, right? But if you're constraining it by the reality of
the neuroscience, then it's really helpful. So let's talk a little bit about deep learning.
And the success is in the applied space of neural networks, the ideas of training model on data, and these simple computational
units, artificial neurons that with back propagation, statistical ways of being able to generalize
from the training set onto data that's similar to that training set.
So where do you think are the limitations of those approaches? What do you think
are your strengths relative to your major efforts of constructing a theory of human intelligence?
Yeah. Well, I'm not an expert in this field. I'm somewhat knowledgeable. So I'm not.
I know it is in just your intuition. What are your? Well, I have a little bit more than
intuition, but I just want to say like, you know, one of the things that you ask me,
do I spend all my time thinking about
neuroscience, I do.
That's to the exclusion of thinking
about things like convolutional neural networks.
But I try to stay current.
So look, I think it's great.
The progress they've made.
It's fantastic.
And as I mentioned earlier,
it's very highly useful for many things.
The models that we have today
are actually derived from a lot of neuroscience principles.
They are distributed processing systems
and distributed memory systems
and that's how the brain works.
They use things that we might call them neurons,
but they're really not neurons at all.
So we can just, they're not really neurons.
So they're distributed process systems.
And nature of hierarchy that came also from neuroscience.
And so there's a lot of things
that the learning rules basically,
not back prop, but other heavy and tight-run.
I'll be curious to say they're not neurons at all.
Can you describe in which way?
I mean, some of it is obvious,
but I'd be curious if you have specific ways
in which you think are the biggest differences.
Yeah, we had a paper in 2016 called,
Why Neurons of Thousands of Synapses.
And if you read that paper, you'll know what I'm talking about here.
A real neuron in the brain is a cottonplex thing.
And let's just start with the synapses on it, which is a connection between neurons.
Real neurons can everywhere from 5 to 30,000 synapses on them.
The ones near the cell body, the ones that are close to the cell model, the cell body,
those are like the ones with people modeled in artificial neurons.
There is a few hundred of those, maybe they can affect the cell, they can make the cell
become active.
95% of the synapses can't do that.
They're too far away.
So if you act at one of those synapses, it just doesn't affect the cell body enough
to make any difference.
And you want them individually.
And you want to be individually, or even if you do all mass of them.
What we know, what real neurons do is the following. If you activate, or they, you know,
you get 10 to 20 of them, active at the same time, meaning they're all receiving an input at the
same time. And those 10 to 20 synapses or 40 synapses are within a very short distance on the dendrolic,
40 microns, a very small area. So if you activate a bunch of these right next to each other,
at some distant place, what happens is it creates what's called the dendritic spike.
And dendritic spike travels through the dendroins and can reach the soma or the cell body.
Now, when it gets there, it changes the voltage,
which is sort of like gonna make the cell fire,
but never enough to make the cell fire.
It's sort of what we call it, it says,
we depolarize the cell, you raise the voltage a little bit,
but not enough to do anything.
It's like, well, good as that,
and then it goes back down again.
So, we proposed a theory, which I'm very confident in, basics are, is that what's happening
there is those 95% of those synapses are recognizing dozens to hundreds of unique patterns.
They can write, you know, about 10, 20 synapses at a time, and they're acting like predictions.
So the neuron actually is a predictive engine on its own.
It can fire when it gets enough, they call it proximal input,
from those ones near the cell fire, but it can get ready to fire
from thousands to hundreds of patterns that are recognized
as from the other guys.
And the advantage of this to the neuron is that when it actually
does produce a spike in action potential, it does so slightly
sooner than it would have otherwise.
And so what could just slightly sooner?
Well, the slightly sooner part is that all the neurons in the excitatory neurons in the
brain are surrounded by these inhibitory neurons, and they're very fast, the inhibitory neurons,
these baskets all.
And if I get my spike out a little bit sooner than someone else, I inhibit all my neighbors
around me.
Right?
And what you end up with is a different representation.
You end up with a representation that matches your prediction. It's a sparsal representation,
meaning it's a fewer non-interactive, but it's much more specific. And so we showed how networks
of these neurons can do very sophisticated temporal prediction, basically. So this summarizes real neurons in the brain are time-based prediction engines
and there's no concept of this at all in artificial or what we call point neurons.
I don't think you can bail the brain without them.
I don't think you can build intelligence with them.
It's where large part of the time comes from.
These are predictive models and the time is, there's a prior and a prediction and an action
and it's inherent through every neuron in the neocortex.
So I would say that point neuron sort of model a piece of that and not very well with that
either.
But, you know, like for example synapses are very unreliable and you cannot assign any
precision to them.
So even one digit of precision is not possible.
So the way real neurons work is they don't add these,
they don't change these weights accurately like artificial neural networks do.
They basically form new synapses.
And so what you're trying to always do is detect the presence of some 10 to 20 active synapses at the same time as opposed and they're almost binary
It's like because you can't really represent anything much finer than that
So these are the kind of dish and I think that's actually another essential component because the brain works on sparse patterns
and all that all that mechanism is based on sparse patterns and I don't actually think you could build our real brains or
Machine intelligence without incorporating some of those ideas.
It's hard to even think about the complexity that emerges from the fact that the timing
of the firing matters in the brain, the fact that you form new synapses and everything
you just mentioned in the past.
You can't, but trust me, if you spend time on it, you can get your mind around it.
It's no longer a mystery to me. No, but, but sorry, as a function in a mathematical
way, it's, can you get it, start getting an intuition about what gets it excited, what
not, and what kind of representations. It's not as easy as, there are many other types
of neural networks that are more amenable to pure analysis.
You know, especially very simple networks.
You know, I have four neurons and they're doing this.
Can we describe them mathematically
what they're doing type of thing?
Even the complexity of convolutional neural networks today,
it's sort of a mystery that can't really describe
the whole system.
And so it's different.
My colleague, Subatana Amad,
he did a nice paper on this.
You can get all the stuff on our website if you're interested.
Talking about some of the mathematical properties
of sparse representations.
And so what we can do is we can show mathematically,
for example, why 10 to 20 synapses to recognize a pattern
is the correct number, is the right number you'd want to use.
And by the way, that matches biology.
We can show mathematically some of these concepts about the show why the brain is so robust
to noise and error and fall out and so on.
We can show that mathematically as well as empirically in simulations.
But the system can't be analyzed completely.
Any complex system can't. So that's
out of the realm. But there is mathematical benefits and intuitions that can be derived
from mathematics. And we try to do that as well. Most of our papers have a section about
that.
So I think it's refreshing and useful for me to be talking to you about deep neural networks because your intuition basically says that we can't achieve anything like intelligence
with artificial neural networks.
Well, not in the current form.
Not in the current form.
So what can you do with in the ultimate form?
Sure.
So let me dig into it and see what your thoughts are there a little bit.
So I'm not sure if you read this little blog post called bitter lesson by Rich Sutton
recently. He's a reinforcement learning pioneer. I'm not sure if you write this little blog post called bitter lesson by Rich Sutton recently recently
He's a reinforcement learning pioneer. I'm not sure if you're familiar with him
His basic idea is that all the stuff we've done in AI in the past 70 years. He's one of the old school guys
The biggest lesson learned is that all the tricky things we've done
biggest lesson learned is that all the tricky things we've done don't you know they benefit in a short term but in the long term what wins out is a simple general method that just
relies on Moore's law on computation getting faster and faster.
This is what he's saying.
This is what has worked up to now.
This is what has worked up to now.
Yeah.
That if you're trying to build a system, if we're talking about,
it's not concerned about intelligence, it's concerned about system that works in terms of
making predictions on applied narrow AI problems, right? That's what this discussion is about.
That you just try to go as general as possible and wait years or decades for the competition
to make it actually.
He's saying that as a criticism or is he saying this is a prescription and what we ought
to be doing?
Well, it's very difficult.
He's saying this is what has worked and yes, a prescription, but it's a difficult prescription
because it says all the fun things you guys are trying to do, we are trying to do.
He's part of the community.
Yeah.
He's saying it's only going to be short-term gains.
So this all leads up to a question,
I guess, on artificial neural networks
and maybe our own biological neural networks,
is, do you think if we just scale things up significantly?
So take these dumb artificial neurons,
the point neurons, I like that term.
If we just have a lot more of them, do you think some of the elements that we see in the
brain may start emerging?
No, I don't think so.
We can do bigger problems and of the same type.
I mean, it's been pointing out by many people that today's convolutional neural networks
aren't really much different than the ones we had quite a while ago.
We just, they're bigger and train more,
and we have more labeled data and so on.
But I don't think you can get to the kind of things
I know the brain can do,
and that we think about as intelligence
by just scaling it up.
So, it may be, it's a good description of what's happened
in the past, what's happened recently, with
the reemergence of artificial neural networks.
It may be a good prescription for what's going to happen in the short term, but I don't
think that's the path.
I've said that earlier.
There's an alternate path.
I should mention to you, by the way, that we've made sufficient progress on our, the
whole cortical theory in the last few years.
Last year, we decided to start actively pursuing how we get these ideas embedded into machine
learning.
Again, being led by my colleague, Supertime on.
And because he's more of a machine learning guy. I'm more of a neuroscience guy. So this is now our new,
I wouldn't say our focus,
but it is now an equal focus here.
Because we need to proselytize what we've learned,
and we need to show how it's beneficial
to the machine learning.
So we're putting, we have a plan in place right now.
In fact, we just did our first paper on this.
I can tell you about that.
But, you know, one of the reasons I want to talk to you
is because I'm trying to get more people
in the machine learning, the community to say,
like, I need to learn about this stuff.
And maybe we should just think about this a bit more
about what we've learned about the brain.
And what are those team at Nementa,
what have they done?
Is that useful for us?
Yeah, so is there elements of all the cortical theory that things we've been talking about
that may be useful in the short term?
Yes, in the short term.
Yes.
This is the start to interrupt, but the open question is, it certainly feels from my perspective
that in the long term, some of the ideas we've been talking about will be extremely useful.
The question is whether in the short term?
Well, this is always what I would call the entrepreneurs dilemma. So you have this long term vision. Oh, we're going to all be
driving electric cars or all going to have computers or all going to whatever. And you're
at some point in time and you say, I can see that long term vision. I'm sure it's going
to happen. How do I get there without killing myself, you know, without going out of business
right? That's the challenge.
That's the dilemma.
That's the really difficult thing to do.
So we're facing that right now.
So ideally, what you'd want to do
is find some steps along the way.
You can get there incrementally.
You don't have to throw it all out and start over again.
The first thing that we've done
is we focus on these sparse representations.
So just in case you don't know what that means
or some of the listeners don't know what that means
or some of the list is known that it means,
in the brain, if I have like 10,000 neurons,
what you would see is maybe 2% of them active at a time.
You don't see 50%, you don't think 30%,
you might see 2% and it's always like that.
For any set of sensory input.
It doesn't matter if anything,
it doesn't matter if any part of the brain,
but which neurons differs? Which neurons are active?
Yeah, so let's say I take 10,000 neurons that are representing something, they're sitting
in a pool of block together. It's a team of their block in a room, 10,000. And they're representing
a location, they're representing a cop, they're representing the input from my sensors. I
don't know, it doesn't matter. It's representing something. The way the representations occur,
it's always a sparse representation.
Meaning it's a population code.
So which 200 sales are active,
tells me what's going on.
It's not individual sales aren't that important at all.
It's the population code that matters.
And when you have sparse population codes,
then all kinds of beautiful properties come out of them.
So the brain uses sparse population codes
and we've written and described these benefits
in some of our papers.
So they give this tremendous robustness to the systems.
The brain's incredibly robust.
No runs are dying all the time, it's basiming
and synapses are falling apart and all the time
and it keeps working.
So what Sympathy and Louise, one of our other engineers here, have
done, have shown that they're introducing sparseness into convolutional neural networks.
Now, other people are thinking along these lines, but we're going about it in a more principled
way, I think. And we're showing that with you, in forced sparseness throughout these convolutional
neural networks, in both the sort of which neurons are active and the connections between
them that you get some very desirable properties. So one of the current hot
topics in deep learning right now are these adversarial examples. So you know
I can give me any deep learning network and I can give you a picture that looks
perfect and you're gonna call it you know you're going to call it, you know, you're going to see the monkey is, you know, an airplane.
So that's a problem.
And DARPA just announced some big thing.
We're trying to, you know, have some contests for this.
But if you enforce sparse representations here,
many of these problems go away.
They're much more robust.
And they're not easy to fool.
So we've already shown some of those results.
It was just literally in January or February just like last month. We did that
And you can I think it's on bio archive right now or on i-cry you can read about it
But um, so that's like a baby step. Okay, that's take something from the brain
We know we know about sparseness. We know why it's important
We know what it gives the brain. So let's try to enforce that on to this.
What's your intuition why sparsity leads to robustness?
Because it feels like it would be less robust.
So why would you feel the rest robust to you?
So it just feels like if the fewer neurons are involved,
the more fragile the representation.
But I didn't say there was lots of fudent.
I said, say 200.
That's a lot.
It's still a lot, is it?
Yes, so here's an intuition for it.
This is a bit technical,
so for engineers, machine learning people, this is easy,
but all the listeners maybe not.
If you're trying to classify something,
you're trying to divide some very high dimensional space
into different pieces, A and B, and you're trying to classify something, you're trying to divide some very high dimensional space into different pieces, A and B, and you're trying to create some point where you say,
all these points in this high dimensional space are A and all these points in this high dimensional
space are B. And if you have points that are close to that line, it's not very robust.
It works for all the points you know about, but it's not very robust because you can just
move a little bit and you've crossed over the line.
When you have sparse representations,
imagine I pick, I'm gonna pick 200 cells active
out of 10,000, okay?
So I have 200 cells active.
Now let's say I pick randomly
another different representation, 200.
The overlap between those is gonna be very small,
just a few.
I can pick millions of samples randomly of 200 neurons and not one of them will overlap
more than just a few.
So one way to think about is if I want to fool one of these representations to look like
one of those other representations, I can't move just one cell or two cells or three cells
or four cells, I have to move 100 cells.
And that makes them robust.
In terms of further, so the emissions barcity, is there?
Well, we'll be the next thing.
Yeah.
So we have, we picked one.
We don't know if it's going to work well yet.
So again, we're trying to come up in incremental ways
to moving from brain theory to add pieces to machine learning, current machine learning world,
and one step at a time.
So the next thing we're going to try to do
is sort of incorporate some of the ideas of the 1000
brains theory that you have many, many models
and that are voting.
Now, that idea is not new.
There's a mixture of models has been around for a long time.
But the way the brain does is a little different.
And the way it votes is different.
And the kind of way it represents uncertainty is different.
So we're just starting this work, but we're going to try to see if we can sort of
incorporate some of the principles of voting or principles of a thousand brain theory, like lots of simple models that talk to each other
in a very certain way.
And can we build more machines,
the systems that learn faster and also,
well, mostly our multimodal and robust to
multimodal type of issues.
So one of the challenges there is the machine learning
computer vision community has certain sets of benchmarks.
So it's a test based on which they compete.
And I would argue, especially from your perspective,
that those benchmarks aren't that useful for testing
the aspects that the brain is good at or intelligent.
They're not really testing intelligence. They're very fine.
And it's been extremely useful for developing specific mathematical models, but it's not useful
in the long term for creating intelligence. So you think you also have a role in proposing better
tests? Yeah, this is a very, you've identified a very serious problem.
First of all, the tests that they have are the tests that they want,
not the tests of the other things that we're trying to do, right?
You know, what are the, so on?
The second thing is sometimes these, to be competitive in these tests,
you have to have huge data sets and huge computing power.
So we don't have that here.
We don't have it as well as other big teams that big companies do.
So there's numerous issues there.
We come at it, we're our approach to this is all based on,
in some sense, you might argue elegance.
We're coming at it from a theoretical base that we think,
oh my god, this is so clearly elegant.
This is how brains work.
This one told you this, but the machine learning world
has gotten in this phase where they think if it doesn't matter,
it doesn't matter what you think.
As long as you do 0.1% better on this benchmark,
that's all that matters.
And that's a problem.
We have to figure out how to get around that.
That's a challenge for us.
It's one of the challenges that we have to deal with.
So I agree you've identified a big issue. It's difficult for those reasons, but you know
Part of the reasons I'm talking to you here today is I hope I'm gonna get some machine learning people to say
I'll read those papers. Those might be some interesting ideas. I'm tired. I'm tired doing this. .1% improvement stuff, you know
Well, that's what I'm here as well because I think machine learning now as a community
is at a place where the next step needs to be orthogonal to what has received success
in the past.
You see other leaders saying this, machine learning leaders, Jeff Hinton with his capsule's
idea. Many people have gotten up to say,
you know, we're going to head road, but maybe we should look at the brain, you know, things like that.
So hopefully that thinking will occur organically and then we're in a nice position for people to
come and look at our work and say, well, what can we live from these guys? Yeah, MIT is launching
billion dollar computing college, the center around this idea.
So on this idea of what of a well, the idea that you know the humanities, the college
and your science have to work all together to get to build the.
Yeah, I mean, I Stanford just did this human center day ice.
I'm a little disappointed in these initiatives because yeah.
You know, they're they're, they're they're focused on the human side of it and it could very easily slip into how
humans interact with tellger machines which is nothing wrong with that but
that's not that is orthogonal to what we're trying to do we're trying to say
like what is the essence of intelligence I don't care I affect I want to build
intelligence machines that aren't emotional that don't smile at you that you know that aren't emotional, that don't smile at you. That aren't trying to tuck you in at night.
Yeah, there is that pattern that you, when you talk about understanding humans is important
for understanding intelligence, you start slipping into topics of ethics or, yeah, like you said,
the interactive elements are supposed to, no, no, no, let's assume it on the brain study,
study what the human brain, supposed to, no, no, no, we have to zoom in on the brain study what the human brain,
the baby, the,
what study what a brain does does.
And then we can decide which parts of that we want to recreate
in some system.
But do you have that theory about what the brain does?
What's the point, you know, you're going to be wasting time,
I think.
Right. Just to break it down on the artificial neural
network side, maybe you can speak to this on the,
on the biological neural side, the process of learning versus the process of inference.
Maybe you can explain to me, is there a difference between, you know, an artificial neural network
is a difference between the learning stage and the inference stage.
Yeah.
Do you see the brain is something different?
One of the big distinctions that people often say, I don't know how correct it is, is
artificial neural networks need a lot of data.
They're very inefficient learning.
Do you see that as a correct distinction from the biology of the human brain that the
human brain is very efficient, or is that just something we deceive ourselves with?
No, it is efficient.
Obviously, we can learn new things almost instantly.
And so what elements do you think? Yeah, I can talk about that. You brought up two issues there.
So, remember I talked early about the constraints. We always feel, well, one of those constraints is
the fact that brains are continually learning. That's not something we said, oh, we can add that later.
That's something that was upfront had to be there from the start. Made our problems harder.
But we showed going back to the 2016 paper on sequence memory,
we showed how that happens,
how the brains infer and learn at the same time.
Our models do that.
They're not two separate phases or two separate sets of time.
I think that's a big, big problem in AI,
at least for many applications, not for all.
So I can talk about that.
There are some, it gets detailed.
There are some parts of the Neocortex in the brain
where actually what's going on,
those are these cycles of activity in the brain.
And there's very strong evidence
that you're doing more of inference on one part of the phase
and more of learning on the other part of the phase.
So the brain can actually sort of step
with different populations of cells
or going back and forth like this.
But in general, I would say that's an important problem.
We have all of our networks that we've come up with
do both.
And they're learning, continuous learning networks.
And you mentioned benchmarks earlier.
Well, there are no benchmarks about that.
Exactly.
So we have to like, we get in our little soapbox,
and hey, by the way, this is important.
And here's a mechanism for doing that.
But until you can prove it to someone
in some commercial system or something, it's a little harder.
So yeah, one of the things I have to link on that is in some ways to learn the concept
of a coffee cup, you only need this one coffee cup and maybe some time alone in a room with
it.
Well, the first thing is, I imagine I reach my hand into a black box and I'm reaching
I'm trying to touch something.
I don't know up front if it's something I already know or if it's a new thing.
And I have to, I'm doing both at the same time.
I don't say, oh, let's see if it's a new thing.
Oh, let's see if it's an old thing.
I don't do that.
As I go, my brain says, oh, it's new or it's not new.
And if it's new, I start learning what it is.
And by the way, it starts learning from the get go, even if it's going to recognize it.
So they're not separate problems. So that's the thing. The other thing you mentioned was the fast learning. So I
was just talking about continuous learning, but there's also fast learning. Literally, I
can show you this coffee cup and I say, here's a new coffee cup. It's got the logo on it.
Take a look at it. Done. You're done. You can predict what it's going to look like. You know,
in different positions. So I can talk about that too.
In the brain, the way learning occurs, I mentioned this earlier, but I mentioned again.
The way learning occurs, I imagine I have a section of a dendrite of a neuron, and I want
to learn, I'm gonna learn something new.
I'm just, it doesn't matter what it is, I'm just gonna learn something new.
I need to recognize a new pattern.
So what I'm going to do, I'm going to form new synapses.
New synapses, we're going to rewire the brain
onto that section of the dendrite.
Once I've done that, everything else that neuron has learned
is not affected by it.
Now, it's because it's isolated to that small section
of the dendrite.
They're not all being added together like a point neuron.
So if I learn something new on this segment here,
it doesn't change any of the learning that
current anywhere else in that neuron.
So I can add something without affecting previous learning.
I can do it quickly.
Now, let's talk, we can talk about the quickness,
how it's done in real neurons.
You might say, well, doesn't it take time to form synapses?
Yes, it can take maybe an hour to form a new synapse.
We can form memories quicker than that,
and I couldn't explain that out, I doubt it was too, if you want.
But it's getting a bit neurosciency.
That's great.
But is there an understanding of these mechanisms at every level?
Yeah. So from the short-term memories and the forming...
Well, we...
So this idea of synaptogenesis, the growth of new synapses, from the short-term memories in the forming many good ideas.
Well, so this idea of synaptogenesis, the growth of new synapses, that's well described,
as well understood.
And that's an essential part of learning.
That is learning.
That is learning, okay?
You know, back, you know, going back many, many years, people, you know, was what's
his name, the psychologist proposed the Hebb, Donald Hebb. He proposed that learning was the modification of the strength of a connection
between two neurons. People interpreted that as the modification of the strength of a synapse.
He didn't say that. He just said there's a modification between the effect of one neuron
and another. So synapse de genesis is totally consistent with Donald Hes said. But anyway, there's these mechanisms that grow with the new
synapse. You can go online and you can watch a video of a synapse growing in real
time. It's literally, you can see this little thing going, it's pretty impressive.
So those mechanisms are known. Now there's another thing that we've
speculated and we've written about, which is consistent with no neuroscience,
but it's less proven. And this is the idea, how do I form a memory really, really quickly?
I can't centenius.
If it takes an hour to grow synapse, that's not instantaneous.
So there are types of synapses called silent synapses.
They look like a synapse, but they don't do anything.
They're just sitting there.
It's like if an action potential comes in,
it doesn't release any neurotransmitter. Some parts of the brain
have more of these than others. For example, the hippocampus has a lot of them,
which is where we associate most short-term memory with. So what we we
speculated again in that 2016 paper, we proposed that the way we form very
quick memories, very short-term memories, or quick memories, is that we convert
silent synapses into active synapses.
It's like seeing a synapse as a zero weight and a one weight.
But the long term memory has to be formed by synapse agenesis.
So you can remember something really quickly by just flipping
a bunch of these guys from silent to active.
It's not from point 1 to point 1.5.
It's like, doesn't do anything to release this transmitter.
And if I do that over a bunch of these,
I've got a very quick short-term memory.
So I guess the lesson behind this is that most neural networks
today are fully connected.
Every neuron connects every other neuron
from layer to layer.
That's not correct.
In the brain, we don't want that.
We actually don't want that.
It's bad. You want a very sparse connectivity so that any neuron connects to some subset of
the neurons in the other layer. And it does so on a on a dendrite by dendrite segment basis.
So it's a very part-related out type of thing. And that then learning is not adjusting all these
ways, but learning is just saying, okay, connect to these 10 cells here right now.
In that process, you know, with artificial neural networks,
it's a very simple process of back propagation
that adjusts the weights.
The process of synaptial genesis.
Synaptial genesis.
Synaptial genesis.
It's even easier.
It's even easier.
Back propagation requires something we,
they really can't happen in brains. This back propagation of this aeros something we, it really can't happen in brains.
This back propagation of this error signal,
it really can't happen.
People are trying to make it happen in brains,
but it's not happening in brains.
This is pure heavy and learning.
Well, it's not the tendency, this is pure heavy and learning.
It's basically saying, there's a population of cells
over here that are active right now,
and there's a population of cells over here active right now.
How do I form connections between those active cells?
And it's literally saying saying this guy became active,
these 100 neurons here became active
before this neuron became active.
So form connections to those ones.
That's it.
There's no propagation of error, nothing.
All the networks we do, all the models we have
work almost completely on a heavy and learning,
but in undendered segments and multiple synopsis at the same time.
So now let's sort of turn the question that you already answered and maybe you can answer it again.
If you look at the history of artificial intelligence, where do you think we stand? How far are we from
solving intelligence? You said you were very optimistic. Yeah. Can you elaborate on that?
Yeah, it's always the crazy question to ask.
Because no one can predict the future.
Absolutely.
So I'll tell you a story.
I used to run a different neuroscientist
called the Redbra Neuroscience 2.
And we would hold these symposiums
when we get like 35 scientists from around the world
to come together.
And I used to ask them all the same question.
I would say, well, how long do you think it would be
before we understand how the Neurocortex works?
And everyone went around the room and they had introduced
the name and they have to answer that question.
So I got the typical answer was 50 to 100 years.
Some people would say 500 years.
Some people said, never.
I said, why are you a neuroscientist?
You never get it. It people said never. I said, why are you, why are you in your science? It's good pay. It's interesting. So, you know, but it doesn't work
like that. As I mentioned earlier, these are not, these are step functions. Things
happen and then bingo that happen. You can't predict that. I feel I've already passed
a step function. So if I can do my job correctly over the next five years,
then meaning I can proselytize these ideas.
I can convince other people they're right.
We can show that other people, or machine learning people
should pay attention to these ideas.
Then we are definitely in an under 20-year time frame.
If I can do those things, if I'm not successful in that,
and this is the last time anyone talks to me, no one reads our papers and I'm wrong or something like
that, then I don't know. But it's not 50 years. It'll, the same thing about electric cars,
how quickly are they going to populate the world? It probably takes about 20 years span.
It'll be something like that, but I think if I could do what I said, we're starting it.
And of course, there could be other usage step functions. It could be everybody gives
up on your ideas for 20 years, and then all of a sudden somebody picks it up again. Wait,
that guy was on to something. Yeah, so that would be a failure on my part, right?
Yeah. Think about Charles Babbage.
You know, Charles Babbage used to be invented the computer back in 18-something, in 1800s.
And everyone forgot about it until, you know, 100 years later, and say, hey, this guy
figured this stuff out a long time ago.
But he was ahead of his time.
I don't think, you know, like, as I said, I recognize this is part of any entrepreneur's
challenge.
I use it entrepreneur broadly in this case.
I'm not meaning like I'm building a business trying to sell something.
I mean, I'm trying to sell ideas.
And this is the challenge is to how you get people to pay attention to you.
How do you get them to give you positive or negative feedback?
How do you get to people actively based on your ideas?
So, you know, we'll'll see how we do on that.
So there's a lot of hype behind artificial intelligence currently.
As you look to spread the ideas that are of a neurocortical theory, the things you're
working on, do you think there's some possibility we'll hit an AI winter once again?
Yeah, certainly a possibility.
No question about it.
That's something you worry about.
Yeah, well, I guess do I worry about it?
I haven't decided yet if that's good or bad for my mission.
That's true, that's very true because it's almost like you need the winter to refresh
the palette.
Yeah, it's like, I want, here's what you want to have it is. You want like to extend it, everyone is so thrilled
about the current state of machine learning and AI
and they don't imagine they need anything else.
It makes my job harder.
If everything crashed completely and every student left the field
and there was no money for anybody to do anything
and it became an embarrassment to talk about machine intelligence and AI.
That wouldn't be good for us either.
You want sort of the soft landing approach, right?
You want enough people, senior people in AI and machine learning and say, you know, we need
other approaches.
We really need other approaches.
Damn, we need other approaches.
Maybe we should look to the brain.
Okay, let's look to the brain.
Who's got some brain ideas?
Okay, let's start a little project on the side here trying to do brain idea related stuff
That's the ideal outcome we would want. So I don't want a total winter and yet I don't want it to be sunny all the time either
So what do you think it takes to build a system with human level intelligence?
Where once demonstrated you would be very impressed
So does it have to have a body?
Does it have to have the the the the
seawater we use before consciousness?
Yeah.
As as as an entirety as a holistic sense.
So first of all, I don't think the goal is to create a machine that is human level intelligence.
I think it's a false goal.
Back to touring.
I think it was a false payment.
We want to understand what intelligence is.
And then we can build intelligent machines
of all different scales, all different capabilities.
A dog is intelligent.
I don't need that.
I'd be pretty good to have a dog.
But what about something that doesn't look like an animal at all?
In different spaces.
So my thinking about this is that we want
to define what intelligence is, agree upon what
makes an intelligent system.
We can then say, okay, we're now going to build systems that work on those principles
or some subset of them, and we can apply them to all different types of problems.
And the kind, the idea, it's not computing.
We don't ask, if I take a little one chip computer, I don't say, well, that's not a computer
because it's not as powerful as this, you know, the server over here. You know, no, because we know that what I don't say, well, that's not a computer because it's not as powerful. It's just, you know, like, server over here.
You know, because we know that what the principles
are computing are, and I can apply those principles
to a small problem or into a big problem.
And, same, and tells you the needs to get there.
We have to say, these are the principles.
I can make a small one, a big one, I can make them distributed.
I can put them on different sensors.
They don't have to be human like at all.
Now, you did bring up a very interesting question
about embodiment.
Does that have to have a body?
It has to have some concept of movement.
It has to be able to move through these reference frames.
I talked about earlier.
Whether it's physically moving, like I need,
if I'm going to have an AI that understands coffee cups,
it's going to have to pick up the coffee cup
and touch it and look at it with its eyes in hands
or something equivalent to that.
If I have a mathematical AI, maybe it's needs to move through mathematical
spaces. I could have a virtual AI that lives in the internet and its movements are traversing
links and digging into files, but it's got a location that it's traveling through some space.
You can't have an AI that just takes some flash thing input,
you know, we call it flash inference.
Here's a pattern done.
No, it's movement, time, movement pattern,
movement pattern, movement pattern, attention,
digging, building, building structure,
just thinking about the model of the world.
So some sort of embodiment,
embodiment, whether it's physical or not,
has to be part of it.
So self-awareness in the way to be able to answer where am I?
Well, you're bringing up self-awareness to different topics, self-awareness.
No, the very narrow definition of self, meaning knowing a sense of self
enough to know where am I in this space where I'm interested.
Yeah, the system needs to know its location, or each component of the system
needs to know where it is in the world at that point in time.
So self-awareness and consciousness, do you think one from the perspective of neuroscience
and your cortex, these are interesting topics, solvable topics, do you have any ideas of
what, why the heck it is that we have a subjective experience at all?
Yeah, I have a lot of fun.
And is it useful or is it just a side effect? It's
interesting to think about. I don't think it's useful as a means to figure out how to build
intelligent machines. It's something that systems do and we can talk about what it is that are like,
well, if I build a system like this, then it would be self-aware or if I build it like this,
it wouldn't be self-aware. So that's a choice I can have. It's not like oh my god it's self-aware. I can't turn, I heard
interview recently with this philosopher from Yale I can't remember his name I apologize for that
but he was talking about well if these computers are self-aware then it would be a crime done
plug-um and I'm like oh come on you know I myself every night, I go to sleep, but instead of crying, you know, I'm just, I employ myself in again
in the morning, I'm there, I'm so.
People get kind of bent out of shape about this.
I have very definite, very detailed understanding
or opinions about what it means to be conscious
and what it means to be self-aware.
I don't think it's that interesting a problem.
You've talked to a Christophe Cork, you know, he thinks that's the only problem.
I didn't actually listen to your interview with him,
but I know him and I know that's the thing you should've thought of.
He also thinks intelligence and consciousness have disjoint.
So I mean, it's not, you don't have to have one or the other.
So he is easy.
I disagree with that.
I just totally disagree with that.
So where is your thoughts and consciousness?
Where does it emerge from?
Because it is. So then we have to break it down to the two parts, because consciousness isn't one thing.
That's part of the problem with that term. It means different things to different people,
and there's different components of it. There is a concept of self-awareness.
That can be very easily explained.
You have a model of your own body, the neocortex, models things in the world,
and it also models your own body.
And then it has a memory. It can remember what you've done.
Okay, so it can remember what you did this morning, can remember what you had for breakfast, and so on.
And so I can say to you, okay, Lex,
were you conscious this morning when you had your, you know, bagel, and you'd say, yes, I was conscious.
Now, what if I could take your brain and revert all the synapses back to the state they were this morning when you had your you know, bagel and you'd say yes, I was conscious. Now what if I could take your brain and
revert all the synapses back to the state they were this morning. And then I said to you,
Lex, well you're conscious when you ate the bagel and you said no, I wasn't conscious.
I said here's a video reading the bagel and you say I wasn't there. I have no that's not possible
because I was I must have been unconscious at that time. So we can just make this one-to-one
correlation between memory of your body's trajectory through the world
over some period of time, a memory
and the ability to recall that memory
is what you would call conscious.
I was conscious of that, it's a self-awareness.
And any system that can recall,
memorize what it's done recently
and bring that back and invoke it again,
would say, yeah, I'm aware. I remember what I did.
Yep.
All right, I got it.
That's an easy one.
Although some people think that's a hard one.
The more challenging part of consciousness
is this one that sometimes used to go by the word
of qualia, which is, you know, why does an object seem red?
Or what is pain?
And why does pain feel like something?
Why do I feel redness?
So what do I feel painless?
And then I could say, well, why does sight
seems different than hearing?
That's the same problem.
It's really, you know, these are all just neurons.
And so how is it that why does looking at you feel different than, you know, hearing
you?
It feels different, but this is neurons in my head.
They're all doing the same thing.
So that's an interesting question. The best treat I've read about this is by guy named
O'Ragan, a book called Why Red Doesn't Sound Like a Bell. It's a little, it's not a
trade book, easy to read, but it's an interesting question. Take something like color. Color
really doesn't exist in the world.
It's not a property of the world.
Property of the world that exists is light frequency.
And that gets turned into we have certain cells
in the retina that respond to different frequencies
different than others.
And so when they enter the brain, you
several bunch of axons that are firing at different rates.
And from that, we perceive color.
But there is no color in the brain.
I mean, there's no color coming in on those synapses.
It's just a correlation between some axons
and some property of frequency.
And that isn't even color itself.
Frequency doesn't have a color.
It's just what it is.
So then the question is, well, why does it even appear
to have a color at all?
Just as you're describing it, there seems to be a connection to those ideas of reference
frames.
I mean, it just feels like consciousness, having the subject, assigning the feeling of
red to the actual color or to the wavelength is useful for until.
That's a good way of putting it.
It's useful as a predictive mechanism,
or useful as a generalization idea.
It's a way of grouping things together to say,
it's useful to have a model like this.
Think about the well-known synchome
that people who've lost a limb experience
called phantom limbs.
And what they claim is they can have their arm
is removed, but they feel the arm. They not only feel it, they know it's there. It's
there. I know it's there. They're swear to you that it's there. And then they can feel
pain in the arm. And they'll feel it in their finger. And if they move their, they move
their non-existent arm behind their back, then they feel the pain behind their back. So
this whole idea that your arm exists is a model of your brain.
It may or may not really exist.
And just like, but it's useful to have a model of something that sort of correlates to
things in the world so you can make predictions about what would happen when those things occur.
It's a little bit of a fuzzy, but I think you're getting quite towards the answer there.
It's useful for the model to express things certain ways that we can then map them
into these reference frames and make predictions about them.
I need to spend more time on this topic.
It doesn't bother me.
Do you really need to spend more time?
Yeah, I don't.
It does feel special that we have subjective experience, but I'm yet to know why.
I'm just personally curious. It's not necessary for
the work we're doing here. I don't think I need to solve that problem to build intelligent machines
at all, not at all. But there is sort of the silly notion that you describe briefly
that doesn't seem so silly to us humans is, you know, if you're successful building intelligent
machines, it feels wrong to then turn them off. Because
if you're able to build a lot of them, it feels wrong to then be able to, you know, to
turn off the...
Well, why? Let's break that down a bit. As humans, why do we fear death? There's two reasons
we fear death. Well, first of all, I'll say when your debt doesn't matter. Oh, who cares? You're dead. So why do we fear death?
We fear death for two reasons one is because we are
Program genetically to fear death that's a that's a survival and prop getting the genes thing
And we also a program to feel sad when people we know die
We don't feel sad for someone we don't know dies as people dying right now, they don't even come to say, I'm so bad about them because
I don't know them. But I know them, I feel really bad. So again, these are old brain genetically
embedded things that we fear death. Outside of those, those uncomfortable feelings, there's
nothing else to worry about.
Well, wait a minute a second. Do you know the denial of death by Becker and Denel?
There's a thought that death is our whole conception
of our world model kind of assumes immortality.
And then death is this terror that underlies it all.
So like, well, some people's world, not mine.
But, okay, so what Becker would say
is that you're just living in an illusion.
You've constructed an illusion for yourself
because it's such a terrible terror,
the fact that this end, the illusion that death is a matter.
You're still not coming to grips with,
the illusion of what?
That death is going to happen.
Oh, it's not going to happen. You're a met you're actually operating.
You haven't even though you said you've accepted it,
you haven't really accepted the notion of dies.
What he's saying. So it sounds like it sounds like you disagree with that notion.
I mean, yeah, totally.
I like like I said that's not every night.
Every night I go to bed. It's like dying a little death
It's a death and if I didn't wake up it wouldn't matter to me
It only if I knew that was gonna happen would it be bothers me
But I didn't know it's gonna happen. How would I know no?
Then I would worry about my wife. Yeah, so imagine imagine I was a loner and I lived in Alaska
And and I lived them out there and there was no animals. Nobody knew I existed
I was just eating these roots all the time. And nobody knew I was there.
And one day I didn't wake up.
What pain in the world would there exist?
Well, so most people that think about this problem
would say that you're just deeply enlightened
or are completely delusion.
What else?
But I would say that's very enlightened way to see the world. That's the rational
one. I think the rational, that's right. But the fact is we don't have an understanding
of why the heck it is we're born and why we die and what happens after we die.
Well, maybe there isn't a reason, maybe there is. So I mentioned those big problems too, right?
You know, you, you interviewed Max Tagmar,
you know, and there's people like that, right?
I mentioned those big problems as well.
And in fact, when I was young,
I made a list of the biggest problems I could think of.
First, why does anything exist?
Second, why did we have the laws of physics that we have?
Third, is life inevitable? And why is we have? Third, is life inevitable?
And why is it here?
Fourth, is intelligence inevitable?
And why is it here?
I stopped there, because I figured
if you can make a truly intelligent system,
that would be the quickest way to answer
the first three questions.
And serious.
Yeah.
And so I said, my mission, you asked me earlier,
my first mission is to understand the brain, but I felt that is the shortest way to get
to true machine intelligence. And I want to get to true machine intelligence
because even if it doesn't occur in my lifetime, other people will benefit
from it because I think it'll occur in my lifetime, but you know, 20 years, it's
you never know. And but that will be the quickest way for us to, you know, we
can make super mathematicians, we can make super space explorers, we can make super physicist brains that do these things.
And that can run experiments that we can't run.
We don't have the abilities to manipulate things and so on.
But we can build and tell the machines to do all those things.
And with the ultimate goal of finding out the answers to the other questions.
a goal of finding out the answers to the other questions. Let me ask you know the depressing and difficult question, which is once we achieve that goal
of creating it, no, of understanding intelligence, do you think we would be happier and more
fulfilled as a species?
To understand intelligence or understanding the answers to the big questions.
Understanding intelligence. Oh the answers to the big questions. Understanding intelligence.
Oh, totally.
Totally.
It would be far more fun place to live.
You think so?
Oh, yeah, why not?
I mean, just put aside this, you know, terminator nonsense.
And just think about, you can think about,
we can talk about the risk of AI if you want.
I'd love to, so let's talk about.
But I think the world is far better knowing things.
We always better than no things.
Do you think it's better?
Is it a better place to live in that I know that our planet is one of many in the
solar system and the solar system is one of many of the galaxies?
I think it's a more, I dread.
I sometimes think like, God, what would be like to live 300 years ago?
I'd be looking at this guy, I can't understand anything.
Oh my God, I'd be like going to bed. I'm like going what's going on here? Well, I mean in some sense I agree with you
But I'm not exactly sure so I'm also a scientist so I have I share you've used but I'm not
We're like rolling down the hill together
What's down the hill I feel it for climbing a hill
Whatever we're getting closer to enlightenment in whatever
I feel like we're climbing a hill. Whatever we're getting closer to enlightenment
in your case on town.
We're climbing, we're getting pulled up a hill
by our curiosity.
We're pushing our, we are putting our,
our curiosity is pulling ourselves up the hill
by our curiosity.
Yeah, Sisyphe is doing the same thing with the rock.
Yeah, yeah, yeah.
But okay, our happiness aside,
do you have concerns about, you know,
you talk about Sam Harris, you know,
Musk of existential threats,
of intelligence systems.
No, I'm not worried about existential threats at all.
There are some things we really do need to worry about.
Even today's AI, we have things we have to worry about.
We have to worry about privacy and about how impacts false beliefs in the world.
And we have real problems that, and things to worry about with today's AI.
And that will continue as we create more intelligence systems.
There's no question, you know, the whole issue about, you know,
making intelligent armaments and weapons is something that really,
we have to think about carefully.
I don't think of those as existential threats.
I think those are the kind of threats we always face,
and we'll have to face them here and, and, and and and we'll have to deal with them.
The IE we can we can talk about what people think are the existential threats, but
when I hear people talking about them, they all sound hollow to me. They're they're based on ideas. They're based on people who really have no idea what intelligence is. And and if they knew what
intelligence was, they wouldn't say those things. So those are not experts in the field.
You know how I'm,
so yeah, so there's two, right?
There's, so one is like super intelligent.
So a system that becomes far, far superior
in reasoning ability than us humans.
How is that an existential threat?
Then, so there's a lot of ways in which it could be one way is us humans are actually
irrational, inefficient, and get in the way of not happiness, but whatever the objective
function is of maximizing that objective function.
It's super intelligent.
The paperclip problem, things like that.
But so the paperclip problem, but with a super intelligent. Yeah, super intelligent paper clip problem and things like that. But so the paper could problem but with a super intelligent. Yeah, yeah, yeah. So we already faced
this threat in some sense. They're called bacteria. These are organisms in the
world that would like to turn everything into bacteria. And they're constantly
morphing. They're constantly changing to evade our protections and in the past they have killed huge swaths of populations
of humans on this planet. So if you want to worry about something that's going to multiply
endlessly, we have it. And I'm far more worried in that regard. I'm far more worried that
some scientists in the laboratory will create a super virus or a super bacteria that we cannot
control. That is a more of existential stress.
It's putting, and it's holding things on top of it
actually seems to make it less existential to me.
It's like, it's limits its power,
it's limits where it can go,
it limits the number of things it can do in many ways.
The bacteria is something you can't even see.
So that's only one of those problems.
Yes, exactly.
So the other one, just in your intuition about
intelligence, intelligence or us humans, do you think of that as something, if you look at
intelligence on the spectrum from zero to us humans, do you think you can scale that as something far
superior? Yeah. So all the mechanisms we've been talking about. Yeah, well, let me, I want to make
another point here, Alex, before I get there. Sure. Intelligence is the neocortex.
It is not the entire brain.
If I, the goal is not to make a human,
the goal is not to make an emotional system.
The goal is not to make a system that wants to have sex
and reproduce.
Why would I build that?
If I want to have a system that wants to reproduce and have sex,
make bacteria, make computer viruses.
Those are bad things.
Don't do that. Those are bad things. Don't do that.
Those are really bad.
Don't do those things.
Regulate those.
But if I just say I want to have a television system, why doesn't it have to have any human-like
emotions?
Why does it even care if it lives?
Why does it even care if it has food?
It doesn't care about those things.
It's just, you know, it's just in a trance thinking about mathematics.
Or it's out there just trying to build the space plan, you know, for it on Mars. It's a, that's
a choice we make. Don't make human-like things, don't make replicating things, don't make
things with how emotions just stick to the neocortex.
So that's, that's a view, actually, that I share, but not everybody shares, in the sense
that you have faith in optimism about us as engineers
of systems, humans as builders of systems to not put in stupid things.
So this is why I mentioned the bacteria one, because you might say, well, some person's
going to do that.
Well, some person today could create a bacteria that's resistant to all the non-antibacterial
agents.
So we already have that threat.
We already know this is going on.
It's not a new threat.
So just accept that and then we have to deal with it, right?
Yeah, so my point is nothing to do with intelligence.
It intelligence is a separate component
that you might apply to a system
that wants to reproduce and do stupid things.
Let's not do that.
Yeah, in fact, it is a mystery why people haven't done that.
My dad as a physicist believes that the reason,
for example, nuclear weapons haven't proliferated
amongst evil people.
So one belief that I share is that there's not
that many evil people in the world that would use, whether it's bacteria or nuclear weapons or maybe the future
AI systems to do bad. So the fact is small. And the second is that it's actually really hard
technically. So the intersection between evil and competent is small in terms. And that's the
thing. By the way, to really annih annihilate humanity you'd have to have you know
sort of the nuclear winter phenomena which is not one person shooting you know
or even 10 bombs you'd have to have some automated system that you know
detonates a million bombs or whatever many thousands we have.
So extreme evil combined with extreme competence and just by
building some stupid system that would automatically you know Dr. Strangelove type of thing
you know, Dr. Strangelove type of thing, you know,
tell them it all.
I mean, look, we could have some Luke Obama go off in some major city in the world.
I think that's actually quite likely, even in my lifetime, I don't think that's on,
I like to think and it'll be a tragedy.
But it won't be an existential threat.
And it's the same as, you know, the virus of 1917, whatever it was, you know,
the influenza. These bad things can or whatever it was, the influenza.
These bad things can happen in the plague and so on.
We can't always prevent it.
We always try, but we can't.
But they're not existential threats until we combine all those crazy things together
in one form.
So on the spectrum of intelligence, from zero to human, do you have a sense of whether it's possible to create several orders of magnitude
or at least double that of human intelligence talking about neurocontext to grow?
I think it's the wrong thing to say double the intelligence. Break it down into different
components. Can I make something at a million times faster than a human brain? Yes, I can
do that. Could I make something that has a lot more storage than a human brain? Yes, I can do that. Could I make something that has a lot more storage
in a human brain? Yes, I could do that. More cop is a comp. Can I make something that attaches
to different sensors than a human brain? Yes, I can do that. Could I make something that's
distributed? So, we talked earlier about the important ear cortex voting. They don't have to
be co-located. They can be all around the places. I could do that too. Those are the
levers I have, but is it more intelligent? Well, it depends what I train in on. What is it's doing?
Well, so he's the thing. So let's say larger in your cortex and or whatever size that allows for
higher and higher hierarchies to form, we're talking about our constraints and concepts.
So could I have something that's a super physicist
or a super mathematician?
Yes.
And the question is, once you have a super physicist,
will they be able to understand something,
do you have a sense that it will be
orders to like us compared to ants?
Could we ever understand it?
Yeah.
Most people cannot understand
general relativity. It's a really hard thing to get. I mean, you know, you can paint it in a fuzzy picture. It's a stretchy space, you know. Yeah.
But the field equations to do that and the deep intuitions are really, really hard.
And I've tried, I'm not able to do it. It's easy to get special relatively,
but general into it, man, it's too much.
And so we already live with this, to some extent.
The vast majority of people can't understand,
actually, what the vast majority of the people actually know.
We're just either we don't have the effort to,
or we can't, or we don't have time,
or just not smart enough, whatever.
So, but we have ways of communicating. Einstein has spoken in a way that I can
understand. He's given me analogies that are useful. I can use those analogies from my own work and
think about concepts that are similar. It's not stupid. It's not like he's existing some of the
playing. There's no connection to with my playing in world here. So, that will occur. It already has occurred.
That's one of my point that this story is.
It already has occurred. We live it every day.
One could argue that with we create machine intelligence
that think a million times faster than us,
that it'll be so far we can't make the connections.
But, you know, at the moment,
everything that seems really, really hard to figure out in the world,
when you actually figure it out, it's not that hard.
You know, everyone can understand the multiverses.
And most everyone can understand quantum physics.
Almost everyone can understand these basic things, even though hardly any people could figure
those things out.
Yeah, but really understand.
So, I need to, only a few people really don't understand.
You need to only understand the projections, the sprinkles of the useful insights.
That was my example of Einstein, right? His general theory of relativity is one thing that very, very, very few people can get.
And what if we just said those other few people are also artificial intelligence? How bad is that?
In some sense, they are. They already, I mean Einstein wasn't a really normal person. He had a lot of weirdo quirks. And so the other people who worked with him.
So maybe they already were sort of this
astral plane of intelligence that we live with at already.
It's not a problem.
It's still useful.
And, you know, so do you think we are the only intelligent life
out there in the universe?
I would say that intelligent life has and will exist else around the universe. I'll say that intelligent life has, and will exist,
also in the universe.
I'll say that.
There is a question about contemporaneous intelligence
life, which is hard to even answer
when we think about relativity in the nature of space time.
We can't say what exactly is this time,
someplace else in the world.
But I think it's, you know, I do worry a lot about the filter
idea,
which is that perhaps intelligent species don't last very long.
And so we haven't been around for a very long.
And as a technological species, we've been around for almost nothing,
you know, about 200 years or something like that.
And we don't have any data, a good data point on whether it's likely
that we'll survive or not.
So do I think that there have been intelligent life elsewhere in the universe, almost certain,
of course, in the past and the future, yes.
Does it survive for a long time?
I don't know.
This is another reason I'm excited about our work, is our work meaning the general world
of AI.
I think we can build intelligent machines that outlast us. They don't have to be tied
to Earth. I'm not saying they're recreating aliens. I'm just saying, if I asked myself,
and this might be a good point to end on here, if I asked myself, what's special about our species?
We're not particularly interesting physically. We don't fly. We're not good swimmers. We're not very fast. We're
not very strong. You know, it's our brain. That's the only thing. And we are the only species
on this planet that's built the model of the world that extends beyond what we can actually
sense. We're the only people who know about the far side of the moon and the other universes.
And I mean, other galaxies and other stars and about what happens in the atom there's no one that knowledge doesn't
exist anywhere else it's only in our heads cats don't do it dogs and do
monkeys don't do it this one and that is what we've created that's unique not
our genes it's knowledge and if I ask me what is the legacy of humanity what
what what show our legacy be it should be knowledge we should preserve our
knowledge in a way that it can exist beyond us.
And I think the best way of doing that, in fact,
you have to do it, is that has to go along
with intelligent machines to understand that knowledge.
It's a very broad idea, but I call it a state planning
for humanity.
We should be thinking about what we want to leave behind,
when as a species we're no longer here
And that'll happen sometime Sooner or later it's gonna happen and understanding intelligence and creating intelligence
Give us a better chance to prolong it does give us a better chance prolong life
Yes, gives us a chance to live another planet
but even beyond that I mean our solar system will disappear one day
Just give enough time.
So I don't know.
I doubt we will ever be able to travel to other things.
But we can tell the stars, but we can send intelligence machines to do that.
So you have an optimistic, a hopeful view of our knowledge of the echoes of human civilization
living through the intelligence systems we create.
Oh, totally. Well, I think the intelligence systems are greater in some sense.
The vessel for bringing them beyond earth, or making them last beyond humans themselves.
So how do you feel about that? That they won't be human quote unquote.
Okay, it's not, but human. What is human? Our species are changing all the time.
Okay, it's not, but human. What is human?
Our species are changing all the time.
Human today is not the same as human just 50 years ago.
What is human?
Do we care about our genetics?
Why is that important?
As I point out, our genetics are no more interesting than a bacterium's genetics.
There's no more interesting than monkeys genetics.
What we have, what's unique and what's valuable, there's our knowledge, what we've learned
about the world.
And that is the rare thing. That's the thing it's our knowledge. What we've learned about the world. And that is the rare thing.
That's the thing we want to preserve.
It's, what do you have about our genes?
It's the knowledge.
It's the knowledge.
That's a really good place to end.
Thank you so much for talking to me.
Oh, it was fun. Thank you.