No Stupid Questions - 109. What Is the Purpose of Embarrassment?
Episode Date: July 31, 2022What’s worse: shame, guilt, or humiliation? Does Angela have psychopathic tendencies? And where’s the worst place to sit at a magic show? ...
Transcript
Discussion (0)
some people would just crawl into a cave and die i'm angela duckworth i'm stephen dubner
and you're listening to no stupid questions today on the show what's the purpose of embarrassment
why don't you want to be up front it's like the best seat because you get called on by the magician
and you sit in a flimsy chair and it collapses.
Angela, an email today from one Fred Wildnauer.
Okay.
Fred's question has to do with embarrassment.
He writes, what is the purpose of embarrassment?
Could it be another form of empathy,
a way of shaming our hunter-gatherer ancestors to do their duty in their community.
My adult children are often embarrassed by my old man type behavior. I think that's something
many of us can identify with. Either as the embarrasser or as the embarrassee.
Fred concludes his email by saying, to what purpose are we embarrassed? Why does the behavior
of others embarrass us? Why do we care? Why does the behavior of others embarrass us?
Why do we care?
Those are a lot of really interesting questions
about embarrassment.
Can we start with a definition?
What is it?
Is it an emotion?
What do you call it?
Let me get you the APA definition,
American Psychological Association.
So like the official definition from our dictionary,
as it were, is embarrassment, comma, noun.
A self-conscious emotion in which a person feels awkward or flustered in other people's company or because
of the attention of others. It often has an element of self-deprecating humor and is typically
characterized by nervous laughter, a shy smile, or blushing. So yes, it's an emotion. It has these characteristic behaviors.
It has an element of self-consciousness.
So your emotion is about yourself,
not about something that's outward.
But I've also read papers that are like,
oh, it's really complicated and it's hard to define.
What would you say is the relationship
between embarrassment and shame?
So one of my favorite researchers is June Tangney.
She studies shame and embarrassment and also guilt.
These are all from the family of self-conscious emotions.
But I do think the distinction is often made
that shame is about you as a person.
So I will say to you, and I can't even believe it,
but when I was in my 20s, I remember distinctly both writing in my journal and saying in my head,
like, I'm bad. Get out of here. I know this doesn't sound like an Angela Duckworth thing to say.
Not like I'm bad at playing lacrosse. Just literally, I'm bad, full stop.
When you say bad, what does that mean? Like broken, cruel, selfish?
stop. When you say bad, what does that mean? Like broken, cruel, selfish? I can't even recreate with any confidence exactly what it was that led me to that or what I even really meant.
I just remember those words. I'm bad. That's a shame statement, right? It's about you as a person
in your entirety. I'm sure there was some feeling of inadequacy, particularly when I
was just starting out in college. But the fact that I said that doesn't actually have to be
located in any like objective reality. As a young woman, I fell short of some expectation I had set
for myself. I think for many people, guilt can be more local. It could be like, I feel guilty that I
didn't call this person back. I feel guilty that I broke up with that person on the phone.
It's about an act. It's not about the totality of you. There's something less extreme about it.
I think these are self-conscious emotions that can clearly be distinguished from embarrassment.
You know, shame and guilt are typically moral. I think embarrassment tends not to have that moral tone. The big question that I would like to know, and sounds like Fred
would like to know as well, is what is the purpose of embarrassment? As a layperson,
I would think that embarrassment serves as sort of quality control in a way, right? If I have
a job to do, let's say I'm interviewing an important person
for an article or a radio show or whatever, I don't want to be, quote, embarrassed by being
poorly prepared or asking bad questions. That's going to drive me to prepare better. So is that
the way we should look at embarrassment as a kind of negative consequence that produces
theoretically positive behaviors? All emotions, every single one, envy, jealousy, joy,
all of our feelings have a purpose. We evolved over many generations to have signals of how
things were going to motivate us to do certain things. In the case of these self-conscious
emotions, shame, embarrassment, guilt, the function is about preserving a kind of social order,
adhering to norms that are good for other people in particular.
Like, I've wondered whether it's possible to be embarrassed on a desert island.
Let's get real here, Stephen.
Name some times where you have been embarrassed,
and now pretend you were on a desert island,
and I wonder whether you would still do it.
So, do you want me to go first, or would you like to start with confessions of embarrassment?
I'm happy to go first, but I think you're totally right.
To me, the embarrassment is the result of feeling like you are being critiqued or observed
for having done something.
By somebody else, right?
By at least one other person.
Yeah, of course. From my own life, since you're asking, when I think about being embarrassed
myself, most of my memories, which are very, very, very strong, are distant from when I was
quite a bit younger, which suggests either that I no longer do embarrassing things,
which is not likely, or that embarrassment may be much more salient
when you're younger.
Wait, but you haven't given a single example yet.
Come on.
I know I'm good at dodging.
OK, so I have probably two or three moments, if I think about embarrassment, that were
really strong.
And I think I've discussed both of them on this show already.
One was being asked to play pomp and circumstance on the organ
at high school graduation when I was maybe 11, 12, 13 years old and botching it. That was horribly
embarrassing. Another kind of similar-ish one when I was about maybe 11, 12, 13. I played baseball and I was asked to umpire for a little league game.
This was the minor leagues of little leagues. So this was like the little, little kids,
eight, nine years old. And I remembered hearing that it was in the umpire's discretion
when to call a game, when to say, okay, the game is over because you couldn't always get
the required innings in
little league because they took so long because the pitching was terrible. So there'd be like 12
walks in a row that took forever. And I think for the minor leagues, they were required to get in
three innings or darkness. And one day I was umping a game and they played three innings and
it was pretty fast, but I kind of wanted to get paid my $3 or $5 and go to the concession stand and buy candy. The sun was still like high in the
sky and I called the game and everybody's looking around like, what are you talking about? It's not
remotely dark. And I was scolded later for that. I was embarrassed about that for a long, long time
because it was plainly like a really bad decision.
So when I'm thinking back to these incidents, like I say, I think it's not a coincidence that the most memorable ones come from a long time ago because for me, at least, embarrassment seems to
serve a function of telling you how to behave a little bit better. Oh, so it's like a tutor. It
helps you learn the social norms. Exactly. It makes you
consider the downside of an action and then reconsider and maybe refrain. Now, how about
you? What's the most embarrassing thing you've ever done? Well, I farted in this exam that was
like a math placement exam. You had me at farted. It doesn't matter where.
It was like the first day of class at Harvard.
Hundreds of my classmates.
And I don't know what I'd eaten, but whatever.
It was a very loud fart.
And because the room, you know, you could hear a pin drop.
Well, you could certainly hear a fart.
I think it was a long one too.
Anyway, there was a flash of embarrassment. But I will say this. I don't think I was as embarrassed as some other people would be.
How did you respond in the moment?
Well, it was an exam. I didn't respond at all. I just kept taking the math exam, right?
You didn't say like, oh, I guess I shouldn would just crawl into a cave and die. But I'm like, whatever.
And you're right that we kind of think back to these adolescent moments.
You know, when you are a teenager and you are entering adulthood, it's not a surprise that self-conscious emotions are at their high watermarks.
You as a child aren't really thinking about how you are relative to other people. When you're a teenager, that is all you think about and care about.
But there's another teenage moment.
In this case, it was high school, I think.
And I have my little plastic tray.
And it's a huge cafeteria.
I mean, there were like 800 kids in my graduating class.
And I just dropped it, the whole thing.
Like clattering silverware, water everywhere.
The whole cafeteria then applauded, not in a nice way.
So I guess that
was a moment of embarrassment. But there too, Stephen, I remember thinking to myself, I mean,
I'm sure I blushed. I'm not like a psychopath, like I can experience self-conscious emotions,
but I don't think my capacity or tendency for embarrassment is very great.
And do you consider that a trait of strength?
is very great. And do you consider that a trait of strength? Well, if you go back to the function, actually, I knew a little about this because I was a teaching assistant for John Sabini.
And John Sabini thought a lot about embarrassment. And he talked about somebody who preceded him,
who's very famous. He's a sociologist, not a psychologist named Irving Goffman.
So I'll tell you a little bit about what John Sabini said and also what Irving Goffman said about the function of this emotion of embarrassment.
The basic idea is we have embarrassment in part to keep the social fabric from tearing.
When we have these transgressions, like I say or do something that is a violation of well-accepted social norms, like this is the
way we do things around here, then we experience embarrassment. It's essentially a way of making
a reparation and also signaling to everyone that like you did wrong. And by that, we keep the
fabric from tearing. So a function of embarrassment is to create a sustainable and good civilization.
Yes, that's right, which is, again,
good for ourselves and then definitely good for others. Let me go back, though. You said I felt
a little bit embarrassed in the moment. I'm not a psychopath. So let's talk about those who don't
feel embarrassed on a scale of, let's say, zero to five. Let's say five is maximum embarrassment
for dropping a tray of food in a high school cafeteria. You sound like you're putting yourself on the low side, right?
Well, let's use my favorite scale.
I'm a zero to 10 girl just because I feel like it's very intuitive for people.
On a scale of zero to 10, then you're closer to psychopath.
I might be like a two.
Wow.
Jason the other day was like, you have a four inch rip in your jean shorts.
And I was like, so what?
He's like, well, you can see what's, you knowan shorts. And I was like, so what? He's like,
well, you can see what's, you know, there. And I was like, so what? He's like, yeah,
but you were already out today. And I was like, so what? He's like, you should be embarrassed.
And I was like, I'm not. I don't care. Was he embarrassed that you weren't embarrassed?
Maybe he was embarrassed for me. I was just confused. I don't know.
I mean, you can't spell embarrassment without bare ass right in the
middle. That's true. So you're saying you're pretty low on the scale. I am. How close are
you to psychopathic? So when we said it has a purpose, some form of pro-sociality, look,
Goffman was a long time ago. He's a sociologist from the 1950s. John Sabini sadly passed away
more than a decade ago, but there is a psychologist
who's still around, Dacher Keltner, and he wrote a paper not too long ago entitled Flustered and
Faithful, colon, Embarrassment as a Signal of Pro-Sociality. And this is getting to your question
because it is saying that if you completely lack embarrassment, there is something antisocial about that. So they do a series of studies, five in total, where they test the hypothesis that really goes all the way back to Goffman that embarrassment, though it's unpleasant, that it serves a vital social function, as they nonverbal apology and appeasement gesture, the authors demonstrate that the observers recognize the expression of embarrassment as a signal of prosociality and commitment to social relationships.
In other words, not only am I signaling, hey, sorry, I didn't mean to.
When we observe someone being embarrassed,
we're sort of accepting that olive branch, if you will.
And if we observe someone doing something that we think should warrant embarrassment,
and they don't exhibit embarrassment, we think they're a little bit off.
Yeah. I mean, this arc of experimental studies say that when you observe someone and they are
appropriately embarrassed, then you infer that
they are more pro-social. So that makes sense. And let's just consider this a spectrum. Obviously,
on one side of the spectrum, experiencing no embarrassment or shame or guilt, let's put that
whole family down there. I could go over to my next door neighbor's house and burn it down and kill them and not think a thing went
wrong. And there I'm plainly psychopathic. What about on the other end of the spectrum of people
who are paralyzed even by feeling embarrassment? Well, I think there are extremes of every
emotional dimension, and you can bet your bottom dollar that the minimum or the maximum
are bad. So people who are so embarrassed that they would never do something that would
run the risk of being embarrassed, like raise your hand and ask a question. At the other end,
like somebody who lacks any feeling at all for their fellow creature, I mean, you could
argue that I should have been more embarrassed, right, by like smelling up the classroom or making a bunch of noise in the cafeteria.
So it's not hard to imagine that the extremes are not great.
Still to come on No Stupid Questions, Angela quizzes Stephen to find out what form of embarrassment he dreads the most.
I love that you just get to ask me questions about how embarrassed I am in situations where I didn't even do anything wrong.
Before we return to Stephen and Angela's conversation about embarrassment, let's hear some thoughts from a listener.
Here's one especially illustrative voice memo from Carissa Prins.
Can one die from secondhand embarrassment?
My dad is a pastor, and one Sunday morning,
he made the unfortunate error of calling the church
the orgasm of Christ instead of organism.
Freudian slip? We'll never know.
Thus began my sex education.
Thanks to Carissa for sharing her experience. Now, back to Stephen and Angela's conversation
about the psychology of embarrassment.
So there is a famous study that of course you know about, the Milgram studies that were
conducted at Yale University, where Stanley Milgram would bring a research
subject into the room. And you're asked to essentially increase the electric shock for
this person that you thought was an actual other research participant, but they're just an actor.
And the electric shocks were not real. They made a sound effect that made you think they were real.
Everything was a sham. But you as a research research participant, just think it's all real.
Like, hey, this is a learning experiment.
We're interested in how pain and electric shock
can either make things easier or harder to learn.
So this person's going to be like reading these words.
Every time they make a mistake,
turn up the shock and shock them more.
And by the way, the actor would pretend
to be in more and more pain.
In fact, at the very top end of the shock scale would be saying things like, my heart, my heart, I can't take it anymore.
So many people turned up the shock.
And in fact, I think nobody actually left the experiment in protest.
So John Sabini taught social psychology.
That's the class that I TA'd for him.
taught social psychology. That's the class that I TA'd for him. And the popular interpretation of this classic experiment was like, wow, situations have a huge impact on behavior. And we have the
false impression that really we're driven by conscience and integrity, but really in the
right situation, any of us could do anything. It was actually a kind of response to Nazi Germany,
like how do people do cruel things? But Sabini had a very, very different
and unpopular interpretation of those exact results. Those research participants turn up
the shock. Okay. He thinks that they were embarrassed. These research participants
found it awkward to say to the experimenter, no. In fact, he wrote this paper called Who is Embarrassed by What?
Published in 2000.
And he says there are three categorically different reasons why we get embarrassed.
They're kind of fun to read.
So first, let me read you this scenario.
And then you can answer on a scale from 0 to 10 how embarrassed you would be.
Okay.
I was the best man at my brother's wedding. I got up at the reception to make the ceremonial toast. Halfway through,
my brother leaned over to me and whispered, your fly is open. Okay, seven. Here's another one.
I recently attended my friend's college graduation. I was sitting on one of those
flimsy metal folding chairs. As I shifted my weight to get more comfortable, the chair collapsed and went crashing to the floor
during the honorary degree recipient's speech.
Yeah, the old collapsing chair.
Okay, Stephen, don't overthink this.
Give me a number, zero to 10.
Eight, eight and a half, nine.
Yeah, that feels terrible.
So those are items from the faux pas subscale.
These are classic canonical embarrassment moments.
I think some version of this probably has happened to most of us.
But here are two items from another subscale.
I was attending a magic show with some friends.
It was a packed auditorium,
but we were sitting at the front, close to the stage.
Mistake number one.
The magician said she needed an assistant from the audience for her next trick.
She called on me to come up on stage and help.
Zero to ten.
Go.
Ten and a half.
In addition, I'm yelling at myself for buying tickets that are up front or for being invited by somebody who had the temerity to buy tickets up front at a magic show.
You don't do that.
Wait, why don't you want to be up front?
It's like the best seat.
Because you get called on by the magician and then you get up there and your
fly is down and you sit in a flimsy chair and it collapses. Let me see one other one. As I entered
my apartment on my birthday, 25 people yelled, surprise. Give me a zero to 10. How embarrassed are you? I actually had that happen for my 50th birthday.
My wife threw a pretty great surprise party.
It was in a restaurant.
And it's funny because I would not have associated it at all with the emotion that we're talking about today, embarrassment.
But apparently, I don't really remember this, but apparently I just turned around and walked out of the room for about two minutes.
I was happy and grateful and excited, but I'm not big on being the focal point, I guess.
And that is the center of attention subscale.
So the claim here is that sometimes we experience the emotion of embarrassment without having done anything wrong.
It's just because we're the center of attention.
Especially when you're not choosing to do so,
like at the magic show or the surprise party.
Interesting.
Let me now move on to the third and last subscale.
I love that you just get to ask me questions
about how embarrassed I am in situations
where I didn't even do anything wrong,
and yet you're still getting embarrassment out of me for non-action.
I know.
This is great.
I love this.
I'm having a great time.
And also, by the way, I'm making you the center of attention.
Yeah.
Great.
Perfect.
Okay.
You're almost done.
Third and final subscale that John Sabini and colleagues created and administered.
It's got an item that goes like
this. I suspected, but I wasn't sure that an employee I supervised was stealing from the
petty cash box. It was clear that I was going to have to confront him. Give me a number, zero to
10. How embarrassed are you? Gosh, again, I never would have attached the word embarrassment to
that scenario. I would definitely feel uncomfortable about it.
Maybe conflict-averse would come to mind.
But if I'm looking for a negative association on a scale of 0 to 10, 10 being the most negative, I would say 8.
So this is the sticky situation subscale.
And so to summarize, Sabini was interested in embarrassment.
He wondered whether there was something else going on in those Milgram studies that nobody really understood. And this last subscale is
exactly what he thought was going on. It's like, awkward. It's like you don't have a script. You
don't know exactly what to say. So that he offered was not only an explanation for the Milgram study,
but like many other findings that
he cataloged where people thought, oh, I wonder what's really going on. Maybe it's a situation.
He was like, no, people are just embarrassed. Now, I don't think you would agree that's exactly
embarrassment, but you probably would agree with the general thesis.
Yeah. And I think that what you're describing is a broader thing within a lot of academic research where there's
research done that would seem to be logical and sensible, but the conclusions drawn from
it may feel exaggerated.
I'll give an example.
These are maybe descendants of Milgram, and it would be interesting to know where Sabini
lay on this scale.
I think of our mutual friend and great economic researcher, John List, who 20 years ago or so started recreating a very famous lab experiment called the dictator game. These experiments that have been run over the years, mostly by psychologists originally, but then economists started to do them too. They thought that they were measuring altruism, essentially.
altruism, essentially. And John List came along and said, you know, I think the way these experiments are set up, it may appear to be measuring altruism, but I think what it's measuring more
is the effect of scrutiny. In other words, there's an authority figure who says, here's an envelope
with 10 single dollar bills in it. And down the hall, there's someone a lot like you, a student
who shows up for this experiment and they don't get an envelope with any money.
If you so choose, you can give some or none or all of your money to that person.
How much would you like to give?
And on average, I think people gave roughly $3, which seems to be incredibly altruistic,
right?
In the real world, how often does that happen?
Let's say you find some money.
Do you immediately go out and give 30%
to someone that you don't even know and maybe will never see?
Hard to imagine.
Right. So John List was thinking, well, maybe this is not so much about altruism.
Maybe it's about the power of scrutiny. So I really like what you're saying about Sabini
taking a deeper or different look at the Milgram experiments because it's hard to explain otherwise.
And it does make me think of when there are these heightened moments, especially played
out in public, just how incredibly awkward it is where people know there's a social norm,
but they can't quite let themselves act on it.
I think back to the Oscars where Will Smith gets up and slaps Chris Rock.
And the weirdest thing of all the things that were weird about it was that he where Will Smith gets up and slaps Chris Rock. And the weirdest thing of all the
things that were weird about it was that he, Will Smith, afterwards didn't seem embarrassed or
ashamed or guilty, at least in the moment. Yes. By the way, you know, I'm a huge Will Smith fan.
So that was a very dark moment in my life. I was like, what? But you're right. In the aftermath,
the ceremony went
on. And when the camera went to Wilson, it wasn't what everybody was hoping for. People were
expecting him to feel one of these self-conscious emotions, shame, guilt, embarrassment.
Let me ask you this. Fred, in his email, wrote, my adult children are often embarrassed by my
old man type behavior. To what purpose?
Why does the behavior of others embarrass us?
Why do we care?
So let's put ourselves in that case.
What function is that emotion serving?
That's interesting.
I have to guess, you know, my mom answers her cell phone all the time and anytime.
I remember calling her once.
She's like, oh, I'm just at the front row of the opera.
And I was like, why are you answering your phone?
Hang up.
Or I'll hang up.
I'm hanging up.
So I was embarrassed, I guess, for her, from her, of her.
All of the above?
I guess when we are embarrassed about our parents, and I think it often is that direction, right?
I think rarely are parents as embarrassed about their children as children are seemingly. Oh, I don't know.
My children have embarrassed me thousands of times.
Okay. Maybe it goes both ways. But the point is that, you know, I always think about family.
You feel like you're an individual, but you're also just part of like one unit. So being embarrassed
about your mom or your dad is like being embarrassed about your collective self. It's hard for me to
imagine, for example, a complete stranger doing something like I watch it on TV or whatever for that embarrassment to
feel the same way as me and being embarrassed about my mother. When you're looking at someone
that you don't know very, very, very well and you make eye contact, but then you feel yourself
compelled to look away, you feel that it's hard to maintain eye contact.
Is that embarrassment?
I think that in the catalog of things that people do,
like cover their mouth, blush, make a self-deprecating comment,
I think gaze aversion, like looking away.
It's a response to embarrassment, perhaps.
Yeah, I mean, the funny thing about emotion is I think people
immediately think of the feeling of emotion.
We know what it feels like to feel guilty. We know what it feels like to be happy. But when you study emotions, they typically
have a cognitive element, like here's the thought that goes with that emotion. And also there's
often a behavior that goes with it, or sometimes a physiological response, like your heart rate,
et cetera. So this is all part of an emotional response, and there is a behavior element.
And I think the gaze aversion, if you want to go all the way back to this evolutionary story, could be like a submissive
behavior. Some emotion psychologists have argued that when you look at primates, for example,
and there is a violation of the societal hierarchy. And also when you are trying to signal
submission to a more dominant ape, you do these like bowing behaviors and gaze aversion
could also be one of those. But it also could just be like, I don't want you to look at me,
right? Center of attention. So embarrassment, as we've been talking about, I would assume
is what we would call a learned behavior. If you were born and put on a desert island
all by yourself, even if then we imported a bunch of tourists on a cruise ship to watch you,
you might not be embarrassed by any of the things you did that they might consider embarrassing
because they don't violate any social norms, right?
Oh, because you don't have the norms to be violated. I think that's right, partly because
it does not seem like very young children, let's say before the age of 18 months or two years, they don't seem very capable of
embarrassment or actually in any meaningful way, other self-conscious emotions like shame or guilt.
You know, like babies don't feel bad that the bowl clatters to the floor and sprays
spaghetti everywhere. They don't feel embarrassed. They don't feel shamed. So it's interesting that
in that third year of life, you start to feel those self-conscious emotions, not nearly at the magnitude of a teenager. And another psychologist that I worked for, Jerry Kagan, thought the emergence of these self-conscious emotions that have something to do with societal norms, something to do with morality, he had this provocative theory that it's around age two or three that your mom is
likely to have another kid. And the reason why we have these emotions come online when they do is
that otherwise you would just kill the other kid. You would just be like, oh, baby, competition.
So that's hard to test empirically, but I'll just say like, yes, it's learned to some extent,
but also it's kind of programmed to be learned. What to be embarrassed by might differ a little bit across culture,
but embarrassment is universal. Circling back to Fred's question, where he writes that he is often
a source of embarrassment for his adult children, who he says are embarrassed by my old man type
behavior. Notice he adds the word type. It's not just old man behavior. by my old man type behavior. Notice he adds the word
type. It's not just old man behavior. It's old man type behavior. Old man Fred. So this implies
that we're looking at an inverted U shape of embarrassment recognition that little, little,
little kids don't recognize it, don't feel it, don't exhibit it. And then maybe older, older
people don't either. That's the Hollywood cliche, right? Grumpy old men. I don't give a crap what you there's a peak, I'm going to guess that teenage
years are its very, very high watermark. And then maybe it sort of declines over adulthood,
such that when you get to the 70s and 80s, you're like, the hell with all of you. I don't care.
I think that's a very good hypothesis. We'll call it the Dubner curve.
Let's call it the Fred Wildnauer curve.
I don't want to make you the center of attention. So, yes. But it does sound like Fred is kind of taking delight in the fact that his behavior
embarrasses adult children. It doesn't bother him at all to the point where he wrote an email to us
using his real name. He's drawing attention to himself for having done these allegedly
embarrassing things. But it sounds like he doesn't give much of a crap.
So all I'm saying is that if we're looking for a role model to follow in our lives of
withstanding embarrassment, maybe even embracing it, I think Fred might be my new hero.
We could do worse than Fred Wildenauer.
No Stupid Questions is produced by me, Rebecca Lee Douglas.
And now here's a fact check of today's conversation.
In the first half of the show,
Stephen recalls that when he was working as a Little League umpire,
regulations mandated that the game needed to reach three innings or darkness.
It's unclear what the rules were when young Stephen was umping,
but according to Little League International,
today's teams are required to complete four innings, not just three.
Later, Angela says she doesn't think that any of the participants
in the infamous Milgram experiments left the experiment in protest.
It's true that nobody literally walked out of the room,
but participants did resist to different degrees.
In the original experiment, 65% of the participants continued to the highest level shock, 450 volts, and all
participants delivered at least 300 volts. However, in later versions of the experiment,
obedience dropped depending on different factors. For example, when participants were paired with
others who seemed to disobey the authority figures, their obedience level dropped to only 10%.
Finally, Stephen and Angela wonder if embarrassment diminishes with age.
While I couldn't find a solid answer for this specific question, there has been research on
the correlation between age and other self-conscious emotions, like guilt and shame.
A 2010 article published in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology examined self-conscious emotions in nearly 3,000 individuals from ages 13 to 89. The authors found that the
older people tended to be more prone to what they called psychologically adaptive self-conscious
emotions, such as guilt and authentic pride,
and less prone to psychologically maladaptive ones,
such as shame and hubristic pride.
That's it for the Fact Check.
Coming up next week on No Stupid Questions,
why is Angie stepping down as CEO of Character Lab?
I am not someone who wants to lead.
I want everyone to use my ideas and pay attention to me,
but I don't want to do the hard work of leadership.
That's next week on No Stupid Questions.
No Stupid Questions is part of the Freakonomics Radio Network,
which also includes Freakonomics Radio,
People I Mostly Admire,
and Freakonomics MD. All our shows Admire, and Freakonomics MD.
All our shows are produced by Stitcher and Rainbud Radio.
This episode was mixed by Eleanor Osborne.
We had help this week from Lyric Bowditch and Jacob Clemente.
Our staff also includes Neil Carruth, Gabriel Roth,
Greg Rippin, Morgan Levy, Zach Lipinski,
Julie Canfor, Ryan Kelly, Jasmine Klinger, Emma Terrell, and Alina Coleman. We had additional research assistance from Anya Dubner.
Our theme song is And She Was by Talking Heads.
Special thanks to David Byrne and Warner Chapel Music.
If you'd like to listen to the show ad-free, subscribe to Stitcher Premium.
You can follow us on Twitter at NSQ underscore show and on Facebook
at NSQ show. If you have a question for a future episode, please email it to NSQ at Freakonomics.com.
To learn more or to read episode transcripts, visit Freakonomics.com slash NSQ. Thanks for listening.
Thanks for listening.
What about as a parent with young kids, let's say, and your kid stands up on the table in the restaurant and starts throwing mashed potatoes?
That's a reflection on you, Stephen.
What kind of parent are you to have raised a kid to stand on the table?
I'd say a very good parent because potatoes are meant to be thrown.
The Freakonomics Radio Network.
The hidden side of everything. Stitcher.