No Stupid Questions - 12. Does “As If” Thinking Really Work?
Episode Date: August 3, 2020Also: how effective is the placebo effect? ...
Transcript
Discussion (0)
I can do open heart surgery. Come on, I've seen it three times.
I'm Angela Duckworth.
I'm Stephen Dubner.
And you're listening to No Stupid Questions.
Today on the show, can you actually fake it till you make it?
So I'm a nine-year-old kid. Yeah, I can drive.
Also, why do placebos make us feel better?
Oh, I know you fell down and you're leading profusely, but I have a teaspoon of sugar
for you. Angela, I read about a psychological trick of sorts, and I'm wondering if it has any
validity. Are you willing to hear me out and pass judgment? Of course I am. Go ahead.
So the purpose of this trick is to get yourself out of a depressive or bad mood. And this comes from Quora. I'll read
the entry. It was written by a mental health counselor named Aaron Doodle. I hope I'm
pronouncing it right. D-U-T-I-L. And he says this came from a client of his. So I'm quoting Aaron
Doodle talking about his client. When he woke up in the morning, if he felt the symptoms of
depression, he would pretend that he was an actor in a movie. After he would shower,
he would stare into his bathroom mirror and shout out loud to himself, action. He would then act as
if he was a non-depressed movie character. Many times, people will allow the symptoms to keep them
from functioning in their daily lives, making the symptoms even worse. A vicious cycle soon occurs.
making the symptoms even worse. A vicious cycle soon occurs. Sometimes if you act as if long enough, you become that role for good or bad. So Angela, I'm curious, is this a real thing
in your discipline psychology as if thinking? And if so, is it reliably successful?
Well, the short answer is yes, it's a thing. It often works to pretend that you're in a certain mood or you're
acting in a certain social role. There's one study about pretending to be an extrovert,
being assertive, talkative, the first to raise your hand. In that study, it was done by Will
Fleeson, a personality psychologist. The people who were told act like an extrovert actually ended up enjoying one of the benefits of extroversion, which is they were happier, at least for the short time of the experiment.
We don't know about any long term effects.
So if I were to ask you to extrapolate from that experiment and any other literature, you know, do you think that it's essentially repeatable?
In other words, can you live your life you think that it's essentially repeatable? In other words,
can you live your life as if? The evidence that comes to bear on this question is something called
behavioral activation therapy. And it's really actually what the Quora post was. Imagine what
a non-depressed person would do. You're like, well, I'm depressed and I want to stay in bed
all day and do nothing and not see anyone and not even eat. Well, what would a non-depressed person would do. You're like, well, I'm depressed and I want to stay in bed all day and do nothing and not see anyone and not even eat. Well, what would a non-depressed person?
Well, they'd probably get up, go for a run, drink a smoothie and call three friends, right? And then
you just do it. This idea of actually changing the behavior in order to change the mood state of the person is a pretty reliable therapeutic technique.
So that makes sense. And it coincides with what I've read about cognitive behavioral therapy and
other therapies.
But that doesn't mean it works for everyone and it's 100% and it lasts forever. Lots of other
things influence how you're going to feel other than just behaving a certain way. Let's at
least unpack why it might work a little bit for some people sometimes. So does it work for some
people sometimes because it actually triggers a broader change? Well, I don't know that it's known,
but I'm going to speculate a little bit if that's okay. One possible explanation for why acting as if you were extroverted or happy might quote unquote work, at least briefly, is because we infer a lot about what we're feeling from actually observing ourselves.
You literally are almost watching yourself and like, oh, I must be scared.
My palms are sweaty.
My breath is short.
My thoughts are racing. I must be having
a panic attack. And so if you're drawing conclusions from your own behavior, then acting as if you
could sort of trick yourself. That's not the only reason, though. Let's say that a depressed person
would imagine that a non-depressed person would get out of bed as opposed to stay in, would actually
go for a workout instead of not, and call friends instead of remaining socially isolated. What happens then
when you actually force yourself to do these things that you don't really want to do, but you
act as if you wanted to do, is that those behaviors then tend to have effects that are real. Like your
friends start talking to you, you're more energized because you went for a run. You feel better because you had breakfast. So I think that there are multiple reasons why acting
as if you were happy might work. One of them is that those behaviors themselves have a causal
downstream benefit. Can you give an example of you using as if thinking in your
professional or personal life?
You know, I actually can. Here's a true story. When I was on tenure track, I was probably like
most professors periodically concerned that I wouldn't keep my job, particularly my first year,
which, you know, wasn't going so well. Not a lot of publications were materializing. I felt a little
bit lost in the direction that I was going to take my work.
And I do remember calling our common friend, Anders Ericsson, who passed away, as we've
discussed on a recent episode.
And he did give me great advice, which is to think of the person I wanted to be with
complete specificity.
I named Carol Dweck, the great Stanford psychologist who gave the world
the idea of growth mindset. And I acted as if I were her. I was like, what would Carol Dweck do?
She would not be discouraged. She would probably think in a programmatic way about her work.
And can you point to any specific changes you made as a result of that thinking?
Did you actually do things differently? Did you approach an idea differently?
Well, I think Carol Dweck is really good at doing what she wants to do and not angsting
unnecessarily about things that she doesn't want to do, which is a real time killer, especially for
a people pleaser like me who feels compelled to read every email and send a personalized response.
And I remember once I was at dinner with Carol and
she had a glass of wine and I apologetically said like, oh, you know, I'm trying to learn
how to drink wine. It's been a lifelong failing of mine. And she just looked at me and said,
why do you care? I don't care if you have a glass of wine, do what you want to do.
I thought you were going to say that she told you that you just hold the glass up and put it
to your lips and tip it up and that it's actually very similar to drinking other
things that you've drunk before.
Like it's really pretty straightforward.
But let me ask you this. When you're thinking about your inability to be
Dweckian in that phase of your life, how much of that is a product of the fact
that you're not at a point in your career yet where you can afford to be Dweckian in that phase of your life, how much of that is a product of the fact that you're not at
a point in your career yet where you can afford to be Dweckian? In other words, when you're younger,
you feel you have to kind of entertain every possibility. You can't say no to people maybe
who are more senior than you because you're curating your reputation. In other words, maybe
you shouldn't have been acting as if you were someone like that at that time. Well, I am sure there are things that she can do that I ought not do because I'm not yet
where she is, right? But let me give you this as a rebuttal, Stephen. Have you ever heard of
Lev Vygotsky?
Yet.
Okay. Well, Lev, he was like the Freud of Russia. One of his enduring contributions was that as somebody who
was fascinated by children, he studied their pretend play. And Vygotsky thought that what
children do is pretend to be, as he put it, one head taller than they are. So if you're four,
you can pretend that you are older than four, right?
And I actually think that's good advice for all of us, because maybe there are some things that Carol Dweck is in a position to do that I can't do and are inappropriate for me to
try to do.
But in so many ways, that's what growth is, right?
You know, I say to my graduate students, if you're a first year student, pretend that
you're a fourth year student.
If you're a fourth year student, pretend that you're a postdoc.
If you're a postdoc, pretend that you're a fourth year student. If you're a fourth year student, pretend that you're a postdoc. If you're a postdoc, pretend that you're a professor. But couldn't that just tip
quite easily into delusion? Like, I can pretend that I rule the universe and have magical powers.
Well, okay, you don't have to go that extreme. Let's just take the case of your childhood example.
So I'm a nine year old kid. Yeah, I can drive can drive and I find the keys and I take off. Or,
you know, I'm a first-year medical resident. I can do open heart surgery. Come on. I've seen
it three times. Yeah. Maybe that's why kids do it in play. There's something very safe about play,
like let's pretend as if. And you're right that obviously you don't want to pretend to have a
competence that you don't yet have if that's going to harm someone.
And obviously every domain is different, right?
I mean, like the domain of academia, having ideas, right?
There's not that much harm in daring yourself to have an idea that's complicated or bigger
than you think you're entitled to, right?
Writing an arrogant paragraph is unlikely to do lasting harm.
There's no heavy machinery.
Exactly. Limbs won't be lost. I would say that unless you think that your as if thinking is going to get someone
else into trouble, it's good advice. But there's another caveat, by the way, that I think is worth
bringing up. All these examples that we're giving, you know, the depressed person who pretends that
they're not depressed, somebody who wants to be bubbly, pretending like they're an extrovert. If you choose autonomously
to do that, I think it can be beneficial. If someone else is making you do that,
or you are feeling pressured. It's exhausting.
Exactly. And I could also just see that it could lead you to dismiss the underlying causes of the problem.
Let's say that someone's in an abusive relationship, okay?
And I wake up and I say, I'm going to act as if my significant other is not emotionally terrorizing me.
I'm going to act as if when they say things that feel hostile,
it's not a threat to me.
Or you could act as if they didn't even say anything at all. Complete denial.
Yeah. So again, I don't mean to dismiss the value of as if thinking because it's very,
very attractive to me. It seems like a hack that is well worth everybody knowing how to do.
I'm just really curious if it might be more of a band-aid that would lead us to
dismiss underlying problems. You know, very often the downside is that you're not solving the
fundamental problem to the extent the fundamental problem can be solved. Could this hack work for
you? I think the answer is yes, it might work for you, but it's very hard to make general recommendations.
How about one specific recommendation then, which is a low risk as if thinking that you
would suggest anyone try?
I think you could pretend as if you were nice.
This is so directed at me, isn't it?
No, Stephen, you are about as nice as...'m going to get as a podcast host. But most
of us could be nicer. Don't you think you could be nicer? You know, honestly, I love this suggestion.
I truly do. Right. Because you can make a Kantian kind of like I rule that all people
pretend that they are five percent nicer. I'm going to try out the as if style of thinking
right now. OK, what are you going to do? I'm going to act out the as-if style of thinking right now. Okay, what are you going to do?
I'm going to act as if this conversation is over and see if it actually stops.
Still to come on No Stupid Questions,
what does the placebo effect say about our perception of reality?
We just got to settle this for once and for all.
Just got to settle this for once and for all.
Stephen, let me go out on a limb here and say that you seem to have healthy skepticism about most things.
Would you agree with that?
As long as you modify it with healthy, then I'll embrace your skepticism charge.
Do you experience the placebo effect?
Are you saying it's more skeptical to believe in the placebo effect or to not believe in the placebo effect?
I mean, probably you experience it, you know, early in life.
I remember my mother directly teaching me.
Wait, literally said the word placebo effect? Yeah, she said something to the effect of, I can give you the medicine or I can give you, it was literally like a spoonful of sugar.
We were a fairly rough and tumble family.
A lot of bones were broken in this family. And I was the youngest of many. So she'd fairly rough and tumble family. A lot of bones
were broken in this family. And I was the youngest of many. So she'd been through a lot and she'd
been trained as a nurse. I think I probably, you know, banged something and I was bleeding,
but not even needing stitches and certainly nothing broken. And she was like, well,
I could give you this or I can give you a spoonful of sugar and your mind will think
it's medicine and you'll be fine. Really? What did you choose?
I took the sugar because we weren't allowed to have much sugar in our house.
This is sounding like Little House on the Prairie. And we got an orange for Christmas.
Just one.
We kind of like that. Actually, a lot like that. We had very little refined sugar
available to us, but there was sugar kept for baking purposes.
And for the administration post-injury.
Exactly. No, but I remember my mom telling me, like, your mind is a powerful thing,
and if it thinks that it's getting something that's going to make you feel better or happier,
whatever, you will feel better or happier. So I guess in answer to your question,
do I believe in the placebo effect? I do, especially when it gets you a teaspoon of sugar.
Okay, so what happened after you a teaspoon of sugar.
Okay, so what happened after you got the teaspoon? Because by the way, usually, in fact,
always, you're supposed to administer the placebo without telling the patient.
Of course. Yeah, I think by the time we got to the end of the conversation about the sugar, I was feeling pretty good. But I do care a lot about the actual placebo effect,
which I guess most people think of as happening in the medical realm, but I think of happening in the social realm as well.
So maybe we should define it.
I do know there's a pain researcher named Jeffrey Mogul who says the placebo effect is the most interesting phenomenon in all of science.
It is at the precise interface of biology and psychology.
The mind-body connection.
Yeah, it's not just about pain relief. It's not just about medical treatment. It's about
how many of our outcomes are driven by the decisions we make and how we can use some
form of a placebo effect to either trick ourselves or nudge ourselves. You know,
the notion of as-if behavior.
I think of that as a sort of placebo effect.
You know, I'm going to take a test right now, and I'm going to act as if I'm really
well-prepared, even though I'm maybe only 90% prepared.
Will that help me do better?
Possibly it will, because I'll have a feeling of confidence that will allow
some sort of flow, some sort of recall, and so on. So I guess the short answer is, even though
everything I've read indicates that the placebo effect has some mixed results in the medical
literature, it does seem to really work sometimes, but not in all cases, not for all time durations.
I believe the idea is extremely powerful because we're very suggestible.
I agree. The placebo effect is fascinating to me because it was like magic, right?
You give people a blue pill and it has nothing in it, really. And it has effects on their
immune function, on what they do.
Sounds nuts, right? If you didn't know human beings, you'd say there's no way that can happen. Right. And I know it doesn't always work, but it works so often. And
the effects are not small. They're whopping. If you just take the positive findings, you have to
ask yourself, well, you have to ask why you have to ask why. And I think it is if you're tricked
into thinking that this is an active allergy medication
or that you really did have surgery on your knee that was endoscopic.
And there have been studies where there's fake surgery even, right? Placebo surgery.
Literally, they'll just do a little nick on your knee and then it's like fake surgery. And then
people are like, wow, thank God I got my knee replaced. It's like I'm a different person.
So that is the initiation of an
expectation of something that's going to happen next. And I think the reason why the placebo
effect works is that there's, of course, objective fact, but then there's our interpretation of
objective fact. At any given time, in any moment, there are a number of different interpretations
that we can have. And when we have an expectation in a certain direction, because of the placebo effect, we choose selectively, like, for example, let's take
knee surgery. You wake up, you see these little nicks on your knee, you fully believe that you
had knee surgery. The next day, you're feeling a certain way. And you can interpret those signals
of pain or not pain in a multitude of different ways, this placebo effect is just tilting you
in one direction. If you're like, oh, well, you know, today I feel a little bit better.
Then you're like, you know what? I'm going to go for that walk around the block.
And then you start a virtuous cycle of behavior.
I think I'm going to fly to California to visit my grandchildren. And then you go through TSA
and they say, do you have any artificial limbs?
And you say, actually, I just had a knee replaced. Then you get in the machine. They said,
no, you didn't. And then they arrest you.
And then you can't even walk out of line because all of a sudden your expectations are reversed.
The reactions that you're describing for why the placebo effect is effective are what? Is it
confirmation bias?
Well, you have certain expectations. You have a prediction, like a hypothesis about what's
about to happen.
And if the reality matches the expectation, the effect is stronger.
Correct. And that's where confirmation bias comes in, because then you could selectively
curate the evidence in your mind. You're like, oh, that confirms my hypothesis, and ignore
disconfirming evidence. So it starts with having an expectation, but like, oh, that confirms my hypothesis and ignore disconfirming evidence.
So it starts with having an expectation, but then yeah, these biases can affirm the expectation.
And then what happens, I think, is behavior. You do something which is congruent with this
interpretation of what just happened. And when you can create a positive cycle of like, oh,
I guess I'm feeling better. I'm going to be a little more active.
That reinforces the belief.
Exactly.
You mentioned earlier that it has actual physiological effect or can have an effect on your immune
system and so on.
How can that happen with a sugar pill?
What is the mental process or the cognitive process that could trigger that reaction?
So I don't know if it's like a, you know, Pavlovian thing.
I think it's more like you have some understanding of what's
happening that is like, oh, now something good's going to happen. Now I'm going to get relief. Now
I'm going to have a lot of energy. So you're interpreting your reality in a way that is
tilted toward the positive.
You know, this reminds me, there is a doctor who's now a famous doctor, but before he
was famous, he was just a doctor who happened to be a heart surgeon. And he thought that there were
a lot of traditions in Eastern medicine that would be really helpful and useful, but they just
weren't ingrained in the way medicine is practiced here. But he decided that he was just going to,
for himself, set up what he called a complementary medicine practice. This was at Columbia Presbyterian
Hospital. And he would bring patients in and have them go through, you know, maybe some massage,
maybe listening to music, maybe deep breathing things that certainly then, 20, 30 years ago,
were not in practice in the typical surgery suite, at least. And he claimed that it had a huge effect simply because
it got the mind involved in a kind of positive chain reaction. I'm feeling better about going
into surgery. I'm feeling stronger. Coming out, I feel like I know how to relax and so on.
And it was incredibly interesting for me to learn about this because it showed me,
A, how powerful the mind can be in relation to the body this because it showed me, A, how powerful the mind can
be in relation to the body. And it showed me how typically doctors and hospitals in the West,
let's call it, don't consider the strength of that mind-body connection. The fact that what
he was doing seemed radical was to me radical. And the doctor, just to close the circle,
is a guy named Mehmet Oz, Dr. Oz,
who went on to become like a TV doctor who a lot of people think of as something not quite like a
real doctor, but in fact, he's a heart surgeon. Well, outside of the medical realm, I was at a
sales event talking about my research, and there were a bunch of salespeople. And one of them who was
like a top producer in this very large corporation gave me a book that he had published on his kind
of philosophy of like motivation. And I think it was called Bullshit. And I read it on the plane
back. I mean, cover to cover. The point of it was that in a way, the placebo effect
and motivating talks and the things that we say to young people in a way are quote unquote bullshit.
I mean, not in a negative way, right? So what's the line between bullshit and placebo?
He told the story in the book of an athlete, a sprinter, who's like really in a slump and not
feeling confident about the season ahead and his long-term prospects. And the coach has the sprinter
run around the track and the coach knows that there's all this underconfidence going on.
And so the kid completes the couple of laps and he comes back and it's like, what's my time? And the coach just lied, just subtracted a second or two. And then the story ends very positively for the coach and
the sprinter. And yeah, that is a kind of lying, right? Well, I don't even know if it's a kind of
lying. It's lying. It's just lying. There is a study, I can't remember the details,
about physical training, which is basically having someone on a bike to test what is their maximum output. And it essentially concerns lying to them. And it
turns out that they can surpass what they thought was their max. So let's say, you know, they think
the max they can get to is 200. When they get to 200, you say, okay, you're at 195, keep going.
And it turns out they can get higher. Yeah. And I think that's all tied up in the same thing,
right? Which is that when we experience reality, it doesn't feel like a
subjective interpretation of reality. It just feels like reality. But that is what it is. It's
a movie that we make up in our mind and we layer on to anything that's actually happening, our own
interpretation. And I think what the placebo effect is doing is just tilting us in a certain
direction so that when we interpret what's happening,
we interpret in a certain way.
So, Angela, let me ask you, this is a potentially disturbing question.
What about the placebo effect vis-a-vis religion?
You could argue, I guess, that if one has a belief in a deity and acts in a certain way because of some idea of a
down-the-road consequence or an interaction with said deity, that that changes your behavior.
It's very hard to prove the existence of said deity, but the belief in existence of said deity
may have much more actual effect on
my lifetime behavior than, you know, even a sugar pill would.
I mean, I don't want to, you know, come to any conclusions about cosmology and who's
right and who's wrong.
You know, I walked in today thinking we've got to come to some conclusions about cosmology.
We just got to settle this for once and for all.
I do think that there's so much, I mean, there is no human realm of experience that is not this. And honestly, like, don't you think a lot of parenting is this way too? You have a kid who's like, this school year is going to be terrible. I'm never going to make any friends. And you say with a lot of confidence in your voice, I did this also with graduate students. They have moments of self-doubt and wavering, and I'm not lying to them. I'm just giving them a firm conviction and a very clear
picture of a future reality that I know is possible. I mean, that's the thing. These are
self-fulfilling prophecies, so we're not painting impossible pictures.
Sure. Well, it reminds me of that line of BS you gave on the show a while back about when
you're delivering criticism to someone.
This is the wise feedback study by David Yeager.
What do you say to people?
The line is, I'm giving you this feedback because I have high standards,
and I know you can achieve them. And maybe you'll say that I know you can achieve them
as a placebo, right? Is that what you're thinking?
Yes. And when I called it a line of BS, I meant it half admiringly, because yeah,
I believe in the power of the placebo.
If you're my professor and I believe that you believe in me, then I'm going to believe in myself.
And it may turn out not well.
And then I may think, oh, boy, now I'm never going to trust what this person says again.
So that's the cost, right?
But the benefit is potentially large.
And remember, the line is, I know you can achieve them.
Not will.
Not that you will achieve them.
You know, there are a lot of psychologists who think that this is most of psychotherapy,
that you go to a therapist and therapist says, look, I have been with maybe a thousand people
who had a story that was almost the same as yours.
Six months from now, you're going to be in my office and you're not going to feel the
way you do today.
Right.
And that is a reality that the therapist knows can happen and maybe knows is likely to happen. And I think for me, when I
learned about this placebo effect in psychotherapy, in surgery, in pretty much every realm in which
human beings are making judgments, it made me think that the way to optimize this or like use this in life is to basically start off with
a positive expectation. Human expectations matter a lot and they do have a causal effect on our
futures. You can choose to have expectations that tilt you in good directions. I like that advice.
We once did on Freakonomics Radio an experiment. The episode was about
generic products, mostly pharmaceuticals. There was a nice paper done which looked at the efficacy
of generic drugs versus the brand name and who was more likely to buy generics versus name brand.
And it turned out that pharmacists were very, very likely to buy the generic because they knew that it was the same thing.
But then there was also a look at whether people think that the brand name drug is more effective.
And the answer was yes.
So we were trying to mirror that for our podcast without giving people drugs because that's hard to do on podcasts.
Possibly unethical.
A little bit unethical.
So we did it instead with peanut butter and jelly sandwiches.
We gathered together a large group of people who worked at WNYC.
We had two sets of sandwiches, each on their own plate.
So there was a patterned plate and a plain plate.
And we told the research subjects that one of the sandwiches was made with name brand peanut butter.
I think we mentioned Skippy or Jif.
And with name brand strawberry preserves, maybe Smuckers. And the other was made with store brand
of both. And we put it on identical bread. And we asked them all to take the taste test.
And in fact, they were all identical. We'd made all the sandwiches from the same peanut butter and the same jam.
But some would say like, well, there's a lot more texture in this peanut butter.
And so I think that's got to be the brand name one.
And then they take a bite of the other sandwich and say, oh, yeah, there's so much less texture in this one.
And in fact, they were exactly the same texture and same peanut butter.
And if I recall correctly, about 98% of people were
very, very sure of their choice. So do you consider yourself fairly susceptible to casual placebos?
If your mom said to me, oh, I know you fell down and you're bleeding profusely,
but I have a teaspoon of sugar for you, Would it work on me? I think it would work
enormously well on me. Okay, so I'm reading a study here, comes from a journal I hadn't heard
of, but it looks like a good study. And it said that there were a series of words that were done
an experiment to find which had the most salubrious effect on those who hear the word.
which had the most salubrious effect on those who hear the word.
For whatever reason, this word and the rhythm and the sounds of the word just makes people feel better more than any other word.
So I'm going to try it on you.
Are you ready?
Okay, hold on.
I got to brace myself.
Is this going to make me feel better?
Yeah, a lot.
A lot.
Cucumber.
Cucumber.
Cucumber. Cucumber. Cucumber. Cucumber. Cucumber. That's not a word I would choose for this. Cucumber. I thought you said it was going to be a word that
had like soft edges. Is it working yet? Maybe tomorrow. Cucumber. Cucumber. Okay, let me try
number two. Zucchini. Zucchini. Was this funded by like the vegetable lobby?
There actually was no list.
There was no paper.
There was no journal.
Oh, Stephen.
And you're not as susceptible to the easy, cheap placebos as I thought you would be, though.
Yeah.
No Stupid Questions is part of the Freakonomics Radio Network.
This episode was produced by me, Rebecca Lee Douglas.
And now here's a fact check of today's questions.
To introduce the concept of as-if thinking,
Stephen reads a Quora entry from a mental health counselor named Aaron Dutelle,
not Doodle as Stephen pronounced.
I reached out to Aaron to see if he could give us any additional insight
about his patient who pretended to be a non-depressed person. Aaron couldn't recall much about the post, which
was from 2016, but he did want to credit another group that uses as-if thinking just as much,
if not more, than cognitive behavioral therapists. Twelve STEP programs have been practicing as-if
thinking since Alcoholics Anonymous was founded in the late 1930s.
Even when they may not feel they're improving, participants are encouraged to go through the steps of the program as if they're making progress with addiction.
Later on in the show, Angela tells the story of a young sprinter from the book Bullshit.
The book is actually written by former NFL player John W.
Wright. The coach in the story is his father, Robert C. Wright, and the sprinter is Nigerian
track and field star Charlton Ahiswole. The story takes place at the 1974 NCAA Track and Field
Championships. An hour before his triple jump competition, Ahiswala was feeling under the weather and considered dropping out of the contest.
Wright encouraged Ahiswala to run 100 meters as a test of his physical state.
And, as Angela recalled, he secretly gave him the head start by not quite starting the stopwatch on time.
According to the book, this trick gave Ahiswala the confidence to participate in the competition, and an hour later, he became the NCAA champion in triple jump, with the second longest jump in the world that year.
Finally, if you're interested in checking out the great Freakonomics Radio peanut butter and jelly experiment, you can listen to the full story in our archives, episode 178, How to Save $1 Billion Without Even Trying.
Participants not only insisted that one sandwich had more texture than the other,
some were also convinced that their name brand pick tasted, quote, nuttier and more honest.
That's it for the Fact Check.
No Stupid Questions is produced by Freakonomics Radio and Stitcher.
No Stupid Questions is produced by Freakonomics Radio and Stitcher. Our staff includes Allison Craiglow, Greg Rippin, James Foster, and Corinne Wallace.
Thanks also to our intern Emma Terrell for her help with this episode.
Our theme song is And She Was by Talking Heads.
Special thanks to David Byrne and Warner Chapel Music.
And if you'd like to listen to the show ad-free, subscribe to Stitcher
Premium. You can also follow us on Instagram and Twitter at NSQ underscore show and on Facebook
at NSQ show. Also, if you heard Stephen or Angela drop a reference to something you'd like to learn
more about, you can check out Freakonomics.com slash NSQ, where we link to all of the studies that you
heard here today.
Thanks for listening.
Do you think Cole Porter ever rhymed placebo with gazebo?
And if not, why not?
I hope so.
Gazebo, placebo.
Stitcher.