No Stupid Questions - 38. What Does It Mean to Be a “Good” Man?
Episode Date: February 7, 2021Also: how can you stop ruminating? ...
Transcript
Discussion (0)
I had no point. I just wanted to criticize myself.
I'm Angela Duckworth.
I'm Stephen Dubner.
And you're listening to No Stupid Questions.
Today on the show, we know misogyny is wrong, but what about misandry?
Hey, tell me more about how you hate me.
Also, how do you stop obsessing over things you can't change?
All who ruminate are ruminants, but not all ruminants necessarily
ruminate. Angela, my question for you today is a little circular and a little meta.
It's totally fine.
So you recently sent me an email after an episode in which we had discussed the Dunning-Kruger
effect. And you wrote, I was chatting with a colleague yesterday about his skepticism of the Dunning-Kruger effect. I think my updated view is that it may occur and likely does, but does so alongside regression to the man.
Okay, typo.
typo. No, plainly, that was just a typo. You meant to type regression to the mean.
But when I saw that, I thought, wow, regression to the man, that is maybe the world's best Freudian slip ever, especially in light of a different recent conversation you and I had. Because in
this other conversation, you had brought up the Trier stress test. The Trier social stress test.
I just want to play you the clip to get it fresh in our mind. Okay. Have you heard of the Trier social stress test. I just want to play you the clip to get it fresh in our mind.
Okay. Have you heard of the Trier social stress test? I've not. I have a theory that only men
name things after themselves. So guess who came up with this social stress test? Yes, that's right.
Trier did. And he was a dude. Okay. Now, interestingly, Angela, that turned out to be
wrong. Wait, what turned out to be wrong.
Wait, what turned out to be wrong specifically?
Well, here, I'll play you the fact check,
because Rebecca came in at the end of this episode to tell us what was actually what.
So let's hear that clip too.
Angela says that she has a theory that only men name things after themselves, and she cites the Trier social stress test as an example.
Men have certainly named quite a few things
after themselves throughout history.
However, the Trier social stress test doesn't fit the bill.
The test for psychological stress was developed
in the early 1990s by Clemens Kirschbaum
and his colleagues at the University of Trier
in the German city of Trier.
So, Angie, you are so rarely wrong.
That's a kind of humiliating wrong.
Trier, the city.
Oops.
But look, this is not about making an error,
because I've made, I'm sure, many more errors than you.
But what struck me is the ease with which
you made that disparaging comment about dudes
did make me wonder about regression to the man and about current attitudes
toward men generally. If I had said something similar about women or pretty much any group
of people other than white men, I would check myself. Misogyny in all forms is wrong. Racism
and prejudice in all forms is wrong. So here's my question. What about misandry, the contempt for or prejudice against men?
Do you think it's legit, on the rise?
I'd like to think I understand fully the desire to correct the past.
But I am wondering where this leads and how it ends.
I, first of all, would agree with you that if you wanted to say something
disparaging about the half of the species that self-identifies as women, that you'd probably
pause. You might seek legal counsel or PR advice first, and then you might not say what you would
have said. Whereas there seems to be a liberty in doing the opposite, like beating up on dudes.
I agree that there's
more hesitation. I think the question would be, what reasons are legitimate and what reasons are
less legitimate for that hesitation or that asymmetry? In the column of like, well, here's
a reason why you should pause more. I mean, we still live in a society where the power is not
only in the United States, but around the world in favor of men on any metric.
And also the amount of misogyny is still massive.
I was looking at this U.N. study from last year, which looks at gender inequality around the world.
And it said the analysis reveals that despite decades of progress closing the equality gap between men and women, close to 90 percent of men and women hold some sort of bias against women.
Yeah, I read a book just last month.
It was an advanced copy.
I think it's coming out in the spring.
I read it cover to cover.
It's called I'm a Girl from Africa, and it's by a woman named Elizabeth Naya Mayaro.
And when she describes the levels of misogyny and not just attitudes, but violent behavior toward women around the world, I mean, there may have been misogyny in Cherry Hole, New Jersey, where I grew up, but it's at a level that I couldn't even really fully picture except by reading what somebody else has experienced. So I think that is one reason why the hesitation to say disparaging things about the less powerful half of the species is warranted.
There is a reality there that we're pointing to. So I am, I think, aware of how much deeper this history is and how much larger female inequality is throughout history than we can address here.
equality is throughout history than we can address here. But I guess when I look at just the last five to ten years, especially with Me Too, Me Too exposed publicly for those who hadn't
acknowledged or been aware the horrible behavior of a bunch of men. But then the question becomes,
well, what share of men are horrible? And then how much guilt by gender association is there or
should there be? So I start to think about this question in context, of course, of Nazism in
World War II, just because it somehow is a frame that I revert to. But I think about the old notion
of the good German. What is that? Look, I think people have been having this conversation about the Republican Party in the age of Trump. You know, what does it mean to be a Republican if the leader of the party is someone that you find objectionable on one or many levels?
So the question becomes, how do you separate out individuals from a larger group, especially when the larger group is under assault?
And I'm not saying men are under assault.
Some men would say that men are under assault. And I'm not saying men are under assault. Some men would say that men are under assault.
I guess people do say that. And I guess what I say to that is that even if there's
widespread misandry, and I'm not saying there is, it's probably just a shadow of the gender
inequality directed at women.
Right. So misandry, I don't think can ever be said to be a good thing. I
don't see how hating any large segment of the population only knowing their demographic
characteristic could ever be good. But I have to say, I understand why it is that there's
asymmetric sensitivity to things that guys could be doing that are questionable or wrong.
I do wonder about the ramifications of the way the relationship
between men and women is changing. And one parallel, which is probably a terrible parallel,
but it's what came to my mind, is some research done around the Americans with Disabilities Act
years ago, which found that after the Americans with Disabilities Act was passed, which was meant
to provide all sorts of opportunities and protections for people with a variety of disabilities. One result of that was that people with certain disabilities
were actually hired much less often, and it was a sad, unintended consequence.
And why was that?
The reason, as best I understand, was that if you're an employer and you hire someone with
a disability, they are now protected to a degree that even if they're
really bad at their job, you're not going to be able to fire them as readily as you might have
before. And therefore, the response is to just avoid hiring them in the first place.
And so that's obviously counter to what the legislation was meant to accomplish,
even though the legislation did accomplish many other things. But I did begin to wonder over these last several
years if men who still run a lot of stuff will, in some circumstances, avoid hiring women who
should be hired. And I think that'd be a brute force response to what's going on, but I wouldn't
be shocked to see it. It wouldn't be new either, don't you think? Men might, men or women, honestly, say, oh, well,
she's going to get pregnant or like she's going to get married. Anyway, that's a very tough nut
to crack policy-wise. If there is this unintended consequence, you don't want to have play out in
the way that you just said. I don't have a great idea of how to counter that. Do you?
Well, there are some policies in, I want to say Denmark, might be Sweden. I'm lumping all the Scandinavians together.
Where if you make paternity leave as long as maternity leave, then that tends to equilibrate
things pretty well.
Oh, interesting.
So you just create incentives for equality.
And I think if I recall correctly, this can't be right.
So I'm sure I'm recalling incorrectly, but I was going to say that men are maybe required to take that paternity leave just to not give them an unfair advantage. But I don't know, maybe I am right. Maybe Rebecca can find that out for us.
I know that for tenure in many universities, you have like an optional year to add on to your clock if you have a baby.
So eight years instead of seven, maybe? Yeah. I think it was a relatively new change that many universities said that applies if you're a
male faculty member or a female faculty member. And has that been done in enough places that
there's a natural experiment where the effect can be measured well?
I don't think so. We're talking about small ends, and then there are lots of features of
universities that make that kind of change that go along with that. So it's very hard to say. But
to your point, then you get this sometimes unintended consequence that you can signal
that you don't need the extra year. Interesting. So maybe enforcing the extra year, but then there's
downsides. This is, by the way, even why I don't do policy. It's too hard. It's like, oh, right,
on the other, other, other hand, this might happen. So I don't really have a good answer to this. But I mentioned that book, I'm a Girl from Africa. I learned about this solidarity movement for gender equality called He for She.
going to be looking for a more equitable society where there's less misogyny, that you have to turn the tables a bit. And instead of making men the opponents, that you would make them your
allies. So a lot of it is about getting like prominent male CEOs or world leaders to stand
up and say, I commit to, you know, this hiring practice or this policy change.
Right. I love that idea. And look, there's been interesting
research about how even men who had opposed or been neutral on certain kind of female positive
legislation, they will change if they have daughters, for instance. The famous daughter
study. Yeah. Do you want to recap it? I love this study. You can recap. This is like the kind of
study that just makes me jealous that I didn't have this great idea. So you asked me about random assignment studies.
Well, having a kid and their gender is pretty much as random assignment as it can get.
You don't have control over it.
It's not confounded with anything.
And the question would be, if by chance you happen to have girls and not boys or girls in addition to boys, what's the influence on you?
and not boys or girls in addition to boys. What's the influence on you? What kind of intervention effect is there on your attitudes towards women because you've had daughters?
This is studied with large corporation CEOs. Companies led by CEOs who have daughters
have much more progressive policies for parental leave and the like. And I think it's such a
beautiful experiment. And I kind of wonder what the effect of having
boys is. I have two girls and I don't know anything about what it's like to have a son.
And I'm guessing that my perspective on growing up as a guy, being a guy is fairly limited
because in our family, there's only one of them and it's my husband.
Yeah. I think for a lot of young men, even in grade school, but
especially middle school and high school, there's a lot that they're trying to figure out and learn
right now. I think there are a lot of people who are struggling to have conversations with their
sisters and their mothers and their girlfriends and their friends who are female about how I can be who I am and also be the kind of young man or
older man that is not seen as A, an other, and B, as a detriment or an enemy. I read a piece by
Sarah Begley in Time magazine. It was a while back. She was writing about sardonic misandry.
I'll read a little chunk. When feminists joke that they are misandrists,
they're riffing off the misguided popular notion that they are man-haters. But inherent in this
word misandry is hatred, and inherent in phrases like ban men and male tears are cruelty and
violence. What feminists really hate, she writes, is the patriarchy, the web of institutions that
systematically oppress women. And to tear it
down, we, meaning women, need as many allies as we can get. Telling half the population that we
hate them, even in jest, is not the way to do that. I think that's very wise. I think that's
like the whole logic of he for she. And none of us, when attacked, are like, hey, tell me more
about how you hate me. So there's no logic really in making
the half of the species that you feel is doing all the oppressing into an even more defensive,
aggressive opponent. I think that one of the things we hope wouldn't come out of a historical
era like the one we're living in, we don't want to have just like, let's go in the opposite
direction.
Let's have other stereotypes to replace the stereotypes
that we've had for years.
Still to come on No Stupid Questions,
Stephen wonders if his way
of dealing with negative events
is psychologically healthy.
The speed at which you're able
to process it
and then put it in your rearview mirror
is startling to me.
Stephen, I got an email from my sister, which I'm going to read to you verbatim. Are you okay with
that? I am. Your sister is a doctor? She is a reproductive endocrinologist. What does that
mean exactly? She helps people have babies?
Oh, if you're having a difficult time getting pregnant, then she's the doctor for you.
I've had a very difficult time for years.
I don't know if she can help you specifically.
But anyway, my sister Annette, who is actually a big fan of this show, writes,
Here's an idea for your podcast.
a big fan of this show, writes, here's an idea for your podcast. How do you stop consternating about something, especially something that you can't change because it is in the past?
Woulda, coulda, shoulda. So Stephen, do you ruminate? And if so, do you feel,
as my sister seems to feel, that it affects you negatively?
So by ruminate, I assume you do not mean to ask whether I chew my own cud,
which is what cows do, which is what makes them ruminants. This is something different
that you're asking about. Oh, yes. That is actually another definition for rumination,
but I did not mean chewing cud as a cow does. Right. But it is true that a ruminant also
describes a person who ruminates. So it's interesting that all who ruminate are ruminants, but not all ruminants necessarily ruminate.
And maybe there's a deeper meaning there because I meant it as obsessive thinking about a past experience that is negative.
So it's a kind of dwelling on a specific past thing that you don't like.
There's this really strong connection between this thought pattern and clinical depression.
And maybe there is a tie-in with rumination of the cow chewing their cud because you really are sort of grinding, you know, masticating on something over and over again.
Well, I will say this.
masticating on something over and over again.
Well, I will say this.
You have answered my follow-up question, which was,
I assume you're here to tell us that rumination is not a good thing, at least in excess.
Correct.
You strike me as a non-ruminant, I have to say.
I don't ruminate that much, honestly.
When she sent me that email, I started looking up all the research on rumination.
It was like exploring a foreign country.
Who are these people?
Exactly. It's very interesting to me because I don't actually dwell on past episodes that have a lot of negative from childhood, from Little League, from a specific night where I was pitching on this night and I was never a very good pitcher.
And we lost because I gave up two home runs to the same kid, Dave Preece.
And I could not go to sleep that night.
And I couldn't stop thinking about it for days and days and days.
I think that was ruminating, right? Yes.
I was obsessively circling in my mind this negative event. But I will say this,
as much as that memory jumps out, I think I've gotten a lot better. In fact, I may not be as
non-ruminatory as Angela Duckworth, but I think I might be, if you're a black belt,
what comes before black? I don't know, brown belt, I think.
I'll say maroon. I'm like maroon belt here. And now I'm curious to know whether this is an
individual gain that I've actually done something to promote that or whether we all just get better
as we mature. Do you know? I don't know whether in general we ruminate less as we get older. I started reading actually the research of
a former student of my same PhD advisor. Her name was Susan Nolan Huxema. She was at Yale and the
founding editor for the annual review of clinical psychology. She was a giant in the field. She
unfortunately died early in her life, but she actually was the world expert on rumination and
depression. And so I have been reading her work obsessively, not ruminating on it, but I've been
reading it. I don't know whether she said anything about age and rumination. She said a lot about
gender, by the way, as many people have. Tell us what you can about gender then.
So women and girls ruminate a whole lot more than males of the same
age. And this in part explains why there is a gender difference with women experiencing higher
levels of clinical depression and anxiety. That's a pretty well-documented difference. And by the
way, it doesn't mean that it's permanent. It could be that 10 years from now it flips, as a lot of
things have actually flipped for gender differences. But I think the thing to understand about rumination is that it probably stems from an adaptive capacity for self-reflection. So it's a good thing that we can turn things over in our mind about what happened and replay them and think about what role we had and what we would have done differently.
It's kind of like a feature that becomes a bug for some people.
Along the lines of too much of a good thing, essentially?
Yeah, too much of a capacity or a tendency that can serve us sometimes,
but can get us into trouble.
Like eating.
Yeah, exactly. And I think this idea that we can have self-reflection and self-focused thinking in more adaptive ways,
I mean, really, this is the great lesson of cognitive therapy, right? That you can reflect
on yourself, but maybe process it more. So I think what Susan Nolan-Huxma would have said is
reflection is better than rumination. Right. As you mentioned that, and I'm thinking through
the biggest adverse events in my life, I think that when I get hit with a hard negative
memory, I do try to process it fully. What does that mean to you?
Well, I examine it from as many sides as I can. I try to talk myself through
why it's so hurtful, why it happened, what could have been done differently.
Were there real causal mechanisms that could have been changed or prevented, or was it bad luck? And then after I interrogate it,
I really do try to tell myself that is a fixed item in the rearview mirror. It's not a dynamic
one that will continue to haunt me or hurt me, even if, in fact, I'm deceiving myself a little
bit. So now that I think about it, I guess I don't ruminate very much.
And I'm really grateful.
I feel like I can turn off bad news or bad events and that I'm really either lucky or cloistered or maybe this is evasion and defense mechanism.
Well, it doesn't sound like you're in denial, which is one of the coping mechanisms that people use.
By the way, let's make this concrete.
Say, for example, you have an argument with your spouse. It sounds like you're not like, what argument? I have no idea what you're talking about. You're processing it in a
more reflective, rational way, which is actually not denial. And it's very adaptive. It's what you
go to cognitive therapy to learn how to do.
And one of the reasons why it's adaptive is it leads to action. And one of the problems with rumination is that by indulging ourselves in these tail-spinning, never-ending cycles of replaying a negative event and then having the negative emotion, it doesn't actually propel you into action.
And the kind of thinking that I hear you talking about does.
The more you talk about it, the more I think about it, the more I realize that I don't seem to ruminate at all.
And while I might have seen this five minutes ago as a strong positive, you're kind of making me rethink it to consider that maybe sometimes I go too far.
You mean you don't even do the reflective processing?
You just don't think about it?
I think I do a really quick version of it.
Like the express lane.
Yeah, expressination.
And I can see that there would be danger in that.
I think I tend to cut things off entirely more than many people do and maybe more than is advisable.
I've witnessed this.
You know what?
more than is advisable.
I've witnessed this.
You know what?
Sometimes, not often,
we get a little bit of a nasty gram in the form of an email
from somebody who doesn't like the way
we discuss something on this show.
And the number of seconds that I spend
thinking about the email,
rethinking about the email,
trying to remember what we said on the show
is about 10x the amount of time that you spend on the same email.
Do you think I have that right?
Maybe 12x, actually.
Yeah, I know, right?
To be honest, I'll give it a look and I'll say, is there a legitimate point here?
And I would say maybe about 8% of the time there is.
And look, I've been doing this a long time, as have you, in different arenas.
Now, that said, this show is different because we're just talking here.
And so, inevitably, we, probably more likely I, am going to say something that just sounds, you know, snarky is one thing I get called occasionally.
And that Angela is not only way smarter than me, which I fully acknowledge, but also so much kinder and nicer.
Don't stop. Keep going. But your point is that you...
I had no point. I just wanted to criticize myself.
Well, look, I think that the speed at which you're able to
process it and then put it in your rearview mirror is startling to me.
And I acknowledge that as well. It strikes me that if you've got,
let's say, a negative thought, it seems that there are a few potential routes you could go.
You could try to replace the negative thought with a positive one. You could just try to clear
your mind or you could try to suppress it. Do you have any idea which is the most effective?
So my good friend and colleague James Gross at Stanford
is one of the world experts in emotion regulation. And he would say the worst way to deal with a
negative emotion in particular, so if it's in a very, very emotional memory and you're trying to
manage the feelings, that suppression, directly trying to will yourself not to feel bad.
That's the way to go, you're saying, right? That's definitely the way to go.
No, no, no. This is going to come in last place, Stephen.
Yeah, I thought so.
Very, very, very bad. Which, by the way, is the way that most people think is intuitively the
way you deal with negative emotions, like just don't feel that way. So suppression is not good.
I think he is actually a fan of strategically distracting yourself. For example, many people Yeah. thoughts that do kind of crowd out or replace the negative thoughts. And I have to just say that
there's a psychologist I really admire on this topic, Ethan Cross, who's also a good friend.
He trained under Walter Mischel, the marshmallow test guy, and he studies self-talk. The little
conversations we have in our head. It's what you people call chatter, right? That's the psycho
phrase for it? Ethan, in particular, says chatter is a great word for it because it's constantly
going on. And sometimes we pay attention to our inner chatter and sometimes we don't,
but we all have inner chatter. And I think the most nefarious forms of chatter are these
ruminatory, negative, pessimistic thoughts. It's one of the strongest predictors of clinical
depression there is. And his prescription is that you want to process things in a more
reflective way. I think they would like you to spend more than two milliseconds on negative
events, but fewer than, you know, 20 hours. And then you're supposed to actually take action
afterwards, like problem solving as an orientation. And maybe you just do it fast, even.
Well, again, I don't mean to claim that I'm really good at not ruminating, but I do think
that practice helps and experience helps.
And as I think about this topic, I think a lot of it does go back to the fact that I
lost a parent when I was a kid.
I was 10 when my dad died.
And I'm not suggesting that everyone should go through this, but it definitely was a lesson and an opportunity
in how to process and deal with adverse events.
And I'm not saying there isn't perhaps
some scar tissue from that.
I mean, one thing that's been observed about me,
at least for many years,
was that I was reluctant to form really close relationships
with a lot of people because,
as the psychologist would say, when you're a kid and a parent dies.
Oh, it's an attachment theory thing. You had an insecurity that you didn't want to get close to
people anymore.
Right. Even if not a conscious reckoning of that, just this idea that someone that you assume is
permanent is suddenly gone. And what do you do with that? So, I'm not sure that that is necessarily connected to my
non-ruminatory nature now, but I do think that I learned from a pretty early age that if I dwell
on sad or painful things from the past, at least I personally couldn't find a way to turn that into
something fruitful and productive. The way I could find to be fruitful and productive would be to, as we've been discussing, process it, come up with a plan of action that's often unrelated to the adverse event and move into that plan of action.
So maybe that's a form of distraction that lies a bit short of denial, but I'm happy with it, honestly.
You should be happy with it, Stephen.
a bit short of denial, but I'm happy with it, honestly.
You should be happy with it, Stephen. The thing I think that happens to some people is they never discover the ladder out of the pit of rumination. You found a ladder. I don't know if
you tripped on it. I don't know if somebody put it there for you, but you climbed out and you're
like, oh, wait, there's a much better way to move past negative events. And I think this is partly
why maybe largely why a lot of people go to therapy is that they can't find the ladder on their own. And all that happens is they reinforce this habit.
about the ultimate non-ruminator, I think about like a gangster who goes out and has to kill someone and stabs them repeatedly in the face and the heart. God, Steven, that was vivid.
And then goes home and eats a steak bloody rare. I think that's actually a scene from a movie.
I'm sure it is. So I'm guessing what we all want is to have the steak without having to do the
murder. That's the goal here, if you're a meat eater? And the example of the assassin that can put the past totally in the rearview mirror without any residue that's lingering, I think that tells us that it's not entirely a bad thing that we have this capacity to ruminate.
It's just really that we don't want it to become a real problem. And in fact, if we didn't have the capacity to be bothered by an argument
with our spouse or something that we should have said but didn't, then like what kind of human
being would we be? We don't want to be that assassin having the stake. Yeah. So Angela,
do you think that your sister Annette will be remotely satisfied by this answer?
Or will she ruminate about the terrible job that we've done with this
wonderful question of hers?
No Stupid Questions
is part of the Freakonomics Radio
Network, which also includes Freakonomics
Radio and people I mostly
admire. This episode was
produced by me, Rebecca Lee Douglas,
and now here's a fact check
of today's conversations.
In the first half of the episode, Stephen and Angela discuss the effect of parental leave on gender equity.
Stephen says he remembers reading about a Scandinavian policy that required new fathers to take paternity leave.
He insists that his recollection must be incorrect, but he was actually right.
In Sweden, new parents are entitled to 240 days of leave each.
They're required to take a minimum of 90 days, regardless of gender.
The remaining 150 days can be transferred to the other parent upon request.
This number may be surprising for Americans.
Out of the world's 196 countries, the United States and Papua New
Guinea are the only places that don't have federally mandated paid time off for new parents.
While Sweden's policy is undeniably progressive in comparison, the country still hasn't escaped
traditional gender expectations. Only 13% of Swedish couples share leave equally,
and a recent study in the International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy
found that mothers with a firstborn used approximately nine and a half months of paid leave,
and fathers in the same group used just three months.
Angela then references gender-neutral tenure clock-stopping policies
adopted by many research-intensive universities in the United States.
She says that she doesn't think there are studies on whether this policy affected equality.
But in 2018, the American Economic Review did publish a comprehensive report on the
measurable effects of this policy over a 25-year period.
It found that while the policy may have attempted to level the playing field, at least in economics departments, it resulted in the reverse.
In theory, no research is expected while the clock is stopped.
But men are more able to use the time to work on research if productivity loss associated with having a child is higher for women.
Men are subsequently 17% more likely to get tenure in their first job once an established gender-neutral clock-stopping policy is in place,
while women are 19% less likely.
Later, Stephen and Angela wonder what comes before a black belt.
Angela guesses brown, and Stephen says maroon.
Many forms of martial arts use belt colors to signify the wearer's skill level.
Often, white signals a beginner, and black or red and white indicates a master.
Brown comes before black in jujitsu, judo, karate, and several other martial arts.
There is no maroon belt.
Finally, during the discussion about rumination, Angela says that, quote,
we all have inner chatter.
This is incorrect. Not all
people have inner monologues. Russell Hurlburt, a psychologist who specializes in this area,
estimates that inner monologue is a frequent thing for only 30 to 50 percent of humans.
Some people have absolutely no inner monologue and may instead experience their thoughts in
more abstract ways. That's it for the Fact Check.
No Stupid Questions is produced by Freakonomics Radio and Stitcher.
Our staff includes Allison Craiglow, Greg Rippin, Mark McCluskey, James Foster, and Emma Terrell.
Our theme song is And She Was by Talking Heads.
Special thanks to David Byrne and Warner Chapel Music.
If you'd like to listen to this
show ad-free, subscribe to Stitcher Premium. You can also follow us on Twitter at NSQ underscore
show and on Facebook at NSQ show. If you have a question for a future episode, please email it to
NSQ at Freakonomics.com. And if you heard Stephen or Angela reference a study, an expert, or a book
that you'd like to learn more about, you can check out Freakonomics.com slash NSQ, where we link to all of the major references that you heard about here today.
Thanks for listening.
As a ruminator who went to therapy for years to figure out how to stop ruminating, it was interesting to listen to that question.
Does it make you jealous to hear non-ruminators talk about it?
Oh, yes. And you're like, how does this happen? Why do people do it? That ignorance is bliss.