No Stupid Questions - 59. Do Dreams Actually Mean Anything?
Episode Date: July 11, 2021Also: why is music so memorable? ...
Transcript
Discussion (0)
I love this. We should get this produced.
I'm Angela Duckworth.
I'm Stephen Dubner.
And you're listening to No Stupid Questions.
Today on the show, is there any significant reason we should pay attention to our dreams?
And then the cake turns out to be made of shards of glass.
Also, what is the connection between music and memory?
Conjunction, junction, what's your function? Hooking up words and phrases and clauses.
Angela, how much attention should I or anyone pay to their dreams? I know there's a long
literature in psychology about the meaningfulness of dreams
and dream interpretation. But I've also read that the content of dreams is often overvalued,
that the content itself can be essentially meaningless and it's a waste of time to
interpret them per se. So where does the truth lie?
Dreams really do have this long and rich history in psychology, most notably beginning with Freud, of course, in his classic work, The Interpretation of Dreams, which he wrote just at the dawn of the 20th century. This is a way that we play out our unconscious impulses. You know, we really want to have sex with our mother and we can't say that in polite society.
And we can't even consciously grapple with this unconscious impulse.
But nevertheless, it's there and it comes out in our dreams.
And that's why psychoanalysts spent so much time talking to people about their dreams.
spent so much time talking to people about their dreams.
Is there data on the share of, let's say, men who have dreamed about having sex with their mothers?
Because I have not. And honestly, that's one thing that always made me suspicious whenever I read Freud and Dreams.
Yeah, Freud was obsessed with, you know, I should have had the electrocomplex,
so I should have been hankering for my dad and you should have been hankering for your mom.
And I also needed to kill my father while having sex with my mother.
Yeah, he got a little carried away.
And eating some birthday cake at the same time or something.
It's as if he had watched one bad movie and then just extrapolated to all of human nature from it.
And he wasn't wrong about everything, but I think he was off track with that.
I know that there have been some studies documenting the content of dreams,
and it's a tiny sliver, if at all, that people have this particular fantasy.
People do think about sex during dreams,
and they do have sexual fantasies that play out of dreams.
But I think that particular narrative has been shown to be pretty rare.
And to be fair, I believe that when Freud wrote about dreams,
he differentiated between manifest and latent content, correct?
The manifest content meaning that's what actually happened,
but then the latent content was the symbolic meaning of it.
Yeah, that's my understanding, at least.
Yeah, and Freud was into this in general. Like, is that a cigar or is it a symbol of a phallus? He always was wondering
whether things that you could interpret literally, like a slip of the tongue, was actually indicative
of something much deeper. And of course, he often said, yes. The problem, by the way, with Freud is
that these are non-falsifiable claims. How would you ever argue against it?
Like, oh, you really secretly want to run off with your dad.
It's like, no, I don't.
Well, that's you having a repressed impulse.
So that was Freud, and that was more than 100 years ago.
And there's been some progress.
I was recently talking about dreams with my now colleague, but formerly my PhD advisor, Marty Seligman.
with my now colleague, but formerly my PhD advisor, Marty Seligman. He actually has done some work on this as a scientist, but we were just talking about the fact that there hasn't
been as much work on dreams as you would expect there to be, because it's just such an interesting
human phenomenon that we have this alternate reality that we experience when we go to sleep,
that we sometimes remember, often we don't, like what the heck is
going on with them? But there isn't a whole lot of research on dreams. How much would you say
that our understanding of what dreams are for or what function they serve within the mind and body,
how much has that understanding changed over the past 100 years or so?
The idea that dreams are a way of working out certain issues, that is still a theory
of dreams. It can be things that we have emotional feelings about, especially negative ones, like,
oh, I'm really anxious about this interview that I'm going to have to do, or I have resentment
towards my spouse. In your sleeping hours, especially during REM sleep, you're working it out. Matthew Walker
at Berkeley has a theory that your dreams are your way of working out things that you have a lot of
emotion about and actually kind of self-administer exposure therapy.
When I read about one function that dreams are meant to accomplish, it is this sort of
flushing the toxins.
But it reminds me of Stephen King.
I once spent a little bit of time with him, and that was the reason he said that he writes
to get rid of those toxins, to deal with fears and anxieties.
And I thought it was a pretty rare thing to want to expose yourself to them constantly
while waking.
I've always assumed the reason so many of us
dream about things that make us scared or anxious is because we don't want to deal with them while
conscious. The question is, when we are quote-unquote dealing with these anxiety-triggering
thoughts, what are we doing? What does it mean to deal with them when we're asleep? And Matthew Walker's idea is that you create a movie in your head, like you're experiencing a kind of hallucination
of this dreaded event, like the job interview that goes terribly. And he has a theory that
noradrenaline is not released when you are experiencing it in sleep. It would be if it were actually happening for real
and you're awake. And the pairing of the episode with the absence of noradrenaline,
that's actually what exposure therapy is. Exposure therapy is all about being exposed to something
that is making you fearful without bad things happening. So his idea is it's therapeutic.
But Freud had a whole idea of catharsis that was a little different from this. Freud's view might be that working it out in your dream, your wish is fulfilled. And that's why you're better off having had the dream, right? That's the healing power of it. Because for there to be a science of dreams, there needs to be some kind of common symbolism or something.
If you and I both have a dream where we are eating our own birthday cake, and then the cake turns out to be made of shards of glass.
Oh, that's terrible, Stephen.
I can't believe you just said that.
Sorry, the birthday cake turns out to be made of puppies and cotton candy.
It's worse. Oh, sorry, we're eating puppies. Okay, it turns out to be the best cake ever.
Yes, thank you. Does that mean the same thing to both of us? I don't know that we need to have
a common vocabulary in dreamland for there to be a science of dreams. Jung, by the way, a psychoanalyst
in the Freudian tradition, although he eventually broke from Freud, did believe that there would be
these archetypes, there would be these iconic forms, and one way or another, they'd come out.
You're naked in public, for instance.
Actually, I don't know if Jung named that, but I personally don't think we need to have
any uniformity to the narratives for there to be a function of dreams and also a science of dreams.
I think we all believe that dreaming can be an act of great creativity because it's your subconscious doing things that for some reason your conscious being won't allow or encourage.
We hear about how Paul McCartney apparently wrote part of at least two
songs yesterday and let it be in his dreams. We know that with Julius Caesar, his wife apparently
had a dream about his impending death the night before he was assassinated. She dreamed that
she was holding his dead body in her arms. And when she woke up, she begged him to not
go to that meeting with the Senate. And he agreed. But then Brutus, whom he trusted,
said, no, it's okay. It's okay. Come on. And then he was assassinated. So the notion that
we should pay attention to our dreams is plainly rooted in all of our collective history. I just
wish I knew which kind of attention to pay to which dreams
and which functions those dreams are serving. That's all I'm asking for.
Is that so much to ask? I will, however, but only if you do the same,
share with you a recurring dream.
Oh, yes, please.
So when I was a little girl, I dreamt a lot about spider webs, and there would be these
tiny little gray spiders that would be skittering all over.
There wasn't really like a narrative, but it was just terrifying to me.
So the extent of your nightmare was that there were some spiders.
Yeah, that's pretty much it.
We're going to need a screenwriter to make this sexier.
But yeah, that was it.
Just spiders.
Do you have any idea what that represented?
What you were scared of?
Because I had this dream repeatedly, I did later in life look it up.
Like, what does it mean to have a dream about spiders?
And wow, there are so many ridiculous speculative accounts like spiders are a symbol of deceit because a web of lies or like you must feel trapped.
There is an interpretation that when you dream about a spider,
it's somehow a mother figure.
Spiders are supposed to symbolize feminine power.
But I think these might actually be romantic interpretations of something
which I'm guessing is more likely,
which is that in the little house that we had at 423 Jamaica Drive, I'm guessing
I saw a spider. And I'm guessing I had the innate reflexive response that human beings do, which is
to fear spiders. And I probably went to bed and dreamt about it. And maybe that was that. So
that's mine. Let's transition to you. I did have a recurring dream.
I want to say it was every night for a few years.
It probably wasn't, but it was extremely routine and it was exactly the same from night to night.
This is going to take a little while.
Are you sitting down?
This is a much more elaborate dream than my spider dream.
A wee bit.
All right, go.
Yeah.
I'll tell you a relevant,
perhaps, trigger, which is that my father had died not long before. So I was about 10 or 11 years old.
And I had a favorite football player whose name was Franco Harris, who played for the Pittsburgh
Steelers. I met him. Oh, you did? Yeah. I didn't know who he was. And he started talking about
football. He's a lovely man and was a great football player.
Franco Harris at the time was a superstar.
And in my dream, my father was dead.
And I had heard that Franco Harris from the Pittsburgh Steelers was for some reason going to be giving a talk at the VFW Hall in Schenectady or Albany, which I knew about because my father was in the VFW, Veterans of Foreign Wars.
And so I would somehow get myself to this VFW dinner where Frank L. Harris was speaking and
somehow persuade him to come back to my house, like 40 miles away in the middle of nowhere.
And I would promise him a meal, which was spaghetti and meatballs. He was half Italian,
half black. And I thought spaghetti meatballs would be good because my mom cooked for quantity, not quality. But spaghetti meatballs was,
I would say, her best meal. And since he was half Italian, I thought it was good.
So we ate. And then I persuaded him to come out in the backyard and play football with me.
This is a lovely dream, Stephen.
So we're playing football. And it's as if it's a real game. Like we're the Steelers, but it's really not because it's just him and me.
But it still feels like there's a big crowd there.
Feels like there's another team.
And it's got the tempo and pace of a game where we're behind.
And it's late in the game.
We're trying to win.
And he was a running back, which means that on most plays, the quarterback would hand
off the ball to him and then he would run through the line of scrimmage to try to get as far
as he could.
So I was a little embarrassed about bringing him back to my house because we were poor
and the house was ramshackle.
And even the yard where we went out to play, it was basically where we would stake the
cow and the cow would leave these big hoof prints and it would get cold and they'd freeze.
So it wasn't even level.
That's where we're playing.
cold and they'd freeze. So it wasn't even level. That's where we're playing. And in the dream,
he's carrying the ball and he steps into one of these frozen cow hoof prints and twists his ankle and goes down really hard. We are at the five yard line. We just need to get a touchdown to
when he stands up limping, takes the ball. He gives it to me and says, Hey kid, you're going
to have to take it from here. And the dream ends every night.
This is like an after school special.
I love this.
We should get this produced.
So I later interpreted it as my father saying, hey, look, it would be nice if every little
boy, every little girl had someone to guide them through the yada yada, whatever.
But if you don't, figure it out.
And so that was my childhood recurring dream.
No spiders, I'm afraid.
Have you ever told Franco Harris this?
Because you should.
So years and years and years later, I saw Franco Harris on the cover of a magazine called
Black Enterprise Magazine.
He had become a successful business person.
And I hadn't thought about him for years. And all these memories, this dream came flooding back. And so I sought him out
and I wanted to know what had become of this person who was my childhood hero. And I ended
up writing a book about him. It was called Confessions of a Hero Worshipper. And it was
about the notion of heroes. So I got to hang out with him,
talk about this notion.
Well, what do you make of all this, Stephen? Did we answer this question about dreams and
what they're good for? I think the ratio of speculation to science is still
heavily speculation.
I have to say, I'm frustrated by the gap between what we know and what we would like to know about
what dreams are, what they represent, whether they represent something that's really important
or something that's a little bit more trivial. And it does make me want to ask you, how do you
psychologists know what you say you know about dreams? Is it really at all empirical?
Well, there's no way that you can scan somebody's brain or do any other kind of test to know what
they're dreaming about. And I think that's one big limitation. The research on it relies on a
post-talk. So like you can't talk to the person while they're dreaming, but anything that's
post-talk immediately and without even being able to control
it, we start to make up causal narratives. Like, was that just your narrative reflex creating that?
Maybe you just had some images. So that's hard to know.
And as you've taught us all here via Danny Kahneman, I guess,
we do have this appetite for coherence, right?
This is one of my major take-homes from Danny Kahneman. I know everyone thinks about thinking fast and slow and judgment decision-making and biases and heuristics, but I think one of his core insights is that human beings have certain fundamental limitations and inclinations. create causal narratives and for there to be coherence. And we hate having the opposite of
that, which is a sense of dissonance, things not adding up. So we're always trying to square things
and make them make sense to us. It's almost like you've got a shoebox and there's a bunch of photos
in there. And spontaneously, we create a story out of them, like this happened and then that
caused that to happen. And then that's why the next thing happened. We do that while we're waking and while we're asleep. We're constructing
narratives that may be true and maybe not. Maybe I didn't even have that spider dream when I was
little. Maybe it wasn't quite like I remember. So sometimes a cow hoof print is just a cow
hoof print, in other words. Sometimes, but we will interpret it as more than that.
Last night, I had a very memorable dream.
Do, tell, go.
We were making this podcast and it ended.
And then we woke up.
Still to come on No Stupid Questions.
Still to come on No Stupid Questions,
Stephen and Angela discuss why music from our adolescence has such a strong psychological and emotional impact.
Don't say anything bad about air supply.
I'm going to kick you in the shins.
Stephen, we have an email from a Kelly Zimmerman.
Can I read it to you?
Please.
I recently went down a rabbit hole, Kelly writes, listening to country music songs that I haven't heard in over 20 years.
I'm 44, and these were songs I listened to in high school.
I was amazed how I immediately and almost innately recalled the lyrics, the intonation, and the music in these songs.
intonation and the music in these songs. My question is this, why does this occur with such little effort while I struggle to recall items for the groceries or meals I ate at a restaurant
last week? And how can we harness this superpower to our benefit?
Boy, do I love that question. I have often thought that music has some qualities that are
unique. I've read a fair bit about it, but most of this is just based
on my having been a musician for a bunch of years. During and after college, I spent my entire waking
days writing music, rehearsing, recording, performing. So I had a very intense relationship
with music, and I did feel that our relationship to music was a superpower. As Kelly noted, music seems to
act upon the memory in a way that other things just don't, at least for me. But there's got to
be a lot of heterogeneity. So some people, like me and maybe like Kelly, you'll hear a piece of
music and you never forget it really, but also when you hear it again, it conjures an
emotional state or some kind of memory. Other people don't remember music that way, but they
may remember other things that way. It could be a conversation. It could be a painting. I get
emotional sometimes if I watch a movie or a play or if I see people that I really love. But somehow music seems to have transcendent
qualities that I don't experience in other things. And I've always thought about the why.
I don't think I have great answers, but maybe between you and me, we can come up with some
responses to Kelly. I want to begin by saying, by the way, Stephen, and I know this is going to
make me a lot of enemies. You hate music. Well, I don't hate it, but I am so indifferent to it. At the same time, I know what you mean
by the emotional power of music. Like you're watching a commercial for auto insurance or
something, and the music is making you feel sad or happy or wistful or energetic. And it doesn't take more than a few strains of music to make you
feel that way. And that is really crazy cool. And at the same time, I think in the last year,
the number of times that I have gone out of my way to click on one button to make music play
is probably fewer than five. So you feel like you were making an argument that would hurt my position
somehow, but I feel like you're just making the argument that yes, heterogeneity exists.
Yeah, fair point.
I absolutely respect and indeed revere the fact that there is this kind of heterogeneity,
but I do think that the kind of connection that Kelly and I are talking about to music is
plainly fairly common because music is a pretty
popular thing. I have a couple thoughts about why it is so memorable. But before we get into my
homemade theories, what about the brain and music? Because you are the former would-be
neuroscientist, after all, and there must be a fair amount known about how the brain
processes, stores, and recalls music, yes?
There must be.
Stephen, I got my neurobiology degree in 1992.
Did they not have music then?
Well, and I studied Alzheimer's disease and neurofibrillary tangle formation.
However, I do know that there is research, for example, by Emily Pronin on the mechanics. I mean, it's such a given that music has an effect on us. Emily Pronin has shown that fast tempo music can actually get us into a very high positive affect state. We're feeling energetic and excited. And then a really slow, moody piece can actually do the opposite. And then the question is, why? And are we
matching our heart rate to the tempo of the song? So the question is not whether music influences
us, but why and how? So I know that there's a lot of discussion about the evolutionary progression
of language and its relationship to, if not music per se, then melody at least. Some of our oldest
literature was obviously oral for a long, long, long time before it was written down,
and much of it had a poetic or musical component to it. Scientists have talked about the fact that
animals don't have the spoken language that we do, but they essentially have music that
serves as language. So it makes sense that music
is a deep, deep, deep part of humanness and a part of human evolution and so on.
If I think about the why in a modern context, why, for instance, Kelly can recall these things,
I do think there are some really concrete things to think about. Let's say it is music with lyrics,
which is what she's talking about, country music. I don't think there's a whole lot of instrumental country music,
as far as I know. So lyrics are pretty predictable because they are repetitive.
They rhyme sometimes.
They rhyme. They have rhythm. You can almost fill in the blank, much more than normal prose. But
even if you don't have lyrics, the instrumental components of music can be pretty memorable. So, okay, you, a non-music
lover, I'll give you a couple notes and you'll tell me if you know the song.
I don't know what it's called, but I can keep going with that.
What is that like? Then it starts with the voice in it. Is it Mick Jagger?
Yeah. Okay, good. And it's something that he can't get any of.
Oh, satisfaction.
That was pretty good for someone who hates music.
I can't get, no.
Okay.
And then there's also the fact that music is, almost all music, is inherently repetitive.
Yeah.
There's always that refrain.
Right.
So there are classical structures, like a sonata form.
What's that?
It has three main sections. There's exposition, then development, and then recapitulation. And
I don't want to say it's all about encouraging memory, but it all conspires.
Wait, are those the three elements of a song?
Those would be the sections of a sonata form, which is one form of classical music. But if
we're talking about pop music, you know...
Yeah, let's talk about pop music. That's more my vibe.
So pop music basically goes verse, verse, chorus, verse, chorus, bridge, chorus, fade.
And what does chorus mean?
Chorus is like the hook, the main thing of a song. Like name a pop song that you're familiar with.
Oh, like the one with Taylor Swift. I'm in my bedroom.
You know what I mean?
That one.
Oh, shucks.
I don't know Taylor Swift.
I'm sorry.
Gosh, it's been a little while, but there was a long period where I went to all the concerts.
For yourself or for your daughters?
Well, it's unclear, but let's just say that I was not unenthusiastic.
So you don't hate music as much as you were leading me on to think that you hate music.
I guess I shouldn't discount my love of Taylor Swift. Okay, so for Satisfaction,
your favorite Rolling Stones song, I can't get no, I can't get no. That's the chorus.
The verses are like, when I'm driving in my car and a man comes on the radio and he's telling me
more and more about some useless information supposed to fire my imagination. That's the
verse. Okay, go on. What I'm saying is that music,
whether it's classical or pop, is essentially engineered to create memory by its repetition.
So I don't think that it's a coincidence that Kelly, when she hears something that she hasn't
heard even in 24 years or whatever, does remember it in a deep way because it was kind of made to do that. I was just thinking about the alphabet.
Like you could teach kids A, B, C, but so many of us literally sing the song in our
heads when we're trying to remember the order.
And then also we've made them rhyme.
The song lands on like A, B, C, E, F, G, H, I, J, K, L, M, N, O, P.
It could have gone L, M, N-O, then nobody would know their alphabet.
Exactly.
Let me ask you this.
As an educator, you don't teach little children how to read and therefore you don't teach
them the alphabet.
But I'm sure you remember Schoolhouse Rock, How a Bill Becomes a Law.
Yeah, I'm just a bill sitting on Capitol Hill.
And also Conjunction Junction. Conjunction Junction, what's your function? Yeah. I'm just a bill sitting on Capitol Hill and also conjunction junction.
Conjunction junction.
What's your function?
Yeah.
Hooking up words and phrases and clauses.
So obviously there's a strong relationship between music and memory in early education.
What about when you get into the super smart people that you teach, do you feel like there's any value in music, melody, tone, rhythm, etc. in education at this level or no? mnemonic device. And it has all to do with the rhythm and the rhyme and alliteration.
Now, that's if you're trying to remember things. Honestly, Stephen, I think in today's day and age,
young people, or really any of us, we're not in the business of memorizing because there's
Google. So since I don't have to ask my students to commit large stores of information to memory,
I'm not using music in that way. I will say that in the pandemic remote teaching era that I've just emerged from, to help my
students, I asked them, what can I do to make my three-hour Zoom class better? And they asked me
for music breaks. And did you tell them, I'm sorry, I hate music? I did tell them, I'm sorry,
I hate music. However, we had a TA named Maya who loved music.
She was the DJ.
And these students filled the chat with like, oh my God, I love this song.
It was incredibly positive.
Did it make you feel like a freak, like such an outlier?
Like, wow, everybody loves this.
Well, here's the thing.
While I was listening to them on the break, because it's a break, camera's off.
I encouraged them to stretch.
I'm sure they didn't do that.
But anyway, I would lie down on my carpet and stretch out and take a 10-minute
rest. And while I was listening to this music, I said to myself and later to my students,
wow, this is amazing. It is beautiful and emotional. And I got to learn a lot about rap
and all these forms of music that I hadn't
any knowledge about before. So it was really enjoyable. Now, here's the thing. I didn't
continue listening to music afterwards, but I can see a universe in which I start to listen
to music more often, actually. I'll tell you what, you start listening to music a little bit more
and I'll subscribe to
Us Weekly and then we can get together and we can play your favorite music and read Us Weekly. I
think that would be a really lovely way to spend an afternoon. I need to negotiate one clause in
the agreement that you're about to hammer out. When was your birth year? 1963. All right. Well, there's a little wrinkle
here, Stephen, which is that... I'll listen to whatever music you want. It can be music from
your... My generation? Yeah. I was only born seven years later. But the reason I asked is that I
think Kelly's question was about why it is that she can remember her high school songs.
Ah, yeah, yeah, yeah.
You got excited about music in general,
but she specifically wanted to know about that.
And when she sent us that email, I thought about Faith,
which was, I think, the number one song in 1988.
George Michael.
I was listening to Asia, you know, the heat of the moment.
Anything by Bryan Adams, Air
Supply, Chicago.
God, I'm so sorry you lived through that.
I love that stuff.
No, it's not good.
Don't say anything bad about Air Supply or I'm going to kick you in the shins.
Really?
You're going to defend Air Supply in public?
What is wrong with Air Supply?
I love Air Supply.
There's nothing bad about Air Supply.
Now I know why you don't listen to music. Oh, come on. What do you have against air supply?
Wait, seriously, is that like a snob thing? Are people down on air supply?
Yeah, they are. Well, they're wrong. But here's the thing, Paul Rosen, the psychologist,
better known for his research on disgust, but also just the most curious psychologist there is,
he wondered whether
there was a sensitive period for the songs that we remember. Because when I say, oh my gosh,
Air Supply Chicago, George Michael, for my husband, it's like Joy Division, New Order.
He's even dragging me to a New Order concert. I don't know how old these people are. I think
they must be octogenarians, but we're going. Now, why is it that he's dragging me to a concert for music that he heard in his teens or in early 20s? There's an argument that
there is a sensitive period and that we don't imprint on music that we hear in our 30s or our
40s or even our childhood. I think it's a really provocative thesis, and Paul did a little bit of
research on it. Other scientists have done even more research on it.
And there's this graph in one of these papers I'll never forget because it's a perfect inverted U.
You never get data like this.
And the graph suggests that at age 24, that is the peak for when we will be forming our musical tastes.
But really, it's kind of anywhere from 18 to 25 or something.
So I don't want to sign this agreement unless I know that we're going to be
kind of kismet on the songs that we're going to listen to.
We will listen to the music from your sensitive period, as long as I have control over some volume
knob and can occasionally rewind or fast forward. But let me ask you this,
what would be the mechanism for that sort of magnetic attraction to music during the quote
sensitive period? Does it have something to do with how the brain is growing at that point?
Does it have something to do with how our critical sense is developing? Does it have something to do
with maybe the fact that after you're 25, you probably spend less time hanging out, listening to music and more time working?
I don't think it's known, but there are some neuroscientists who believe that adolescence
is a sensitive period in general for learning. So one could make the broader argument that we're very plastic during our teenage years and early 20s. And more specifically you should be especially sensitive to what do people do? What's popular? What's out of fashion? How do I ingratiate myself to the highest status people in society? And that makes a lot of sense to me as the mother of two teenage daughters. It's interesting how the sensitive theory fits into the Sapolsky theory. Robert
Sapolsky is, I guess he's a neuroscientist by training, but also a primatologist. Stanford,
right? Yes, at Stanford. And he has a theory, which is honestly not super scientific. It was
done with some survey data, but it wasn't the kind of study that we might think would hold up under deep, deep, deep scrutiny. The research that he did was trying to figure out how and when people develop
preferences for things like music, but also food and travel, these different things that make up
who we are essentially as people. And his argument was that by roughly the age of 35,
most of us don't adopt any new preferences.
Basically, if we haven't started eating sushi by 35, most people are not going to.
If you haven't started listening to opera or hip hop or Bob Dylan by the age of 35,
you're probably not going to.
That might be one explanation to Kelly for why music, even from a long time ago,
has such great resonance, because you're not constantly replacing it with newer material.
That's a theory, at least. Well, Paul Rosen's view, I think he would say that what happens during the friendship period
is that we are adopting and creating social norms. And what happens when we enter our 30s is
that we stop hanging around with our friends and we start raising kids and basically being within
our nuclear family. And I will say that Paul's overall theory of preference formation is very
much around social cues. So he thinks that for lots of things like
drinking your coffee with or without sugar or whatever, that you can say there's some biological
functions of certain things. But partly the more arbitrary taste could be like, I watch other
people in my peer group do it and then I want to do it. So that would be another argument for
music preference. I'm on board with that. I find that really a compelling argument. I will offer one last argument in favor of the power of music
to connect with memory. One thing I've noticed is that there are songs that I've come to know
in foreign languages, even if I'm not fluent in the language. Oh, interesting. What language?
Well, I was learning Hebrew at one point several years ago.
I never became fluent.
I can kind of get around in Hebrew if I work really hard at it.
But if I think of a song I was listening to during that period, I feel fluent.
That sounds really good, by the way. these different things, emotion, melody itself, rhythm, to create this alchemical stew that I really do find, at least in my life, unique. And someday, I'm going to persuade you to feel the
same, Angela. Well, Stephen, I know Mick Jagger said that he couldn't get no satisfaction,
but I thought that was pretty damn good.
no satisfaction, but I thought that was pretty damn good.
No Stupid Questions is part of the Freakonomics Radio Network, which also includes Freakonomics Radio, People I Mostly Admire, and the Freakonomics Radio Book Club. This episode was produced by me,
Rebecca Lee Douglas, and now here's a fact check of today's conversations.
Angela says that spiders
are said to represent a mother figure in dreams. In fact, in Freud's introductory lectures,
he cites his collaborator, German psychoanalyst Carl Abraham. Quote,
According to Abraham, a spider in dreams is symbolic of the mother, but of the phallic
mother of whom we are afraid, so that the fear of spiders expresses dread of mother incest So, according to Freud and Abraham,
Angela's dreams may not just have been about spiders in the family's South Jersey home,
but rather, surprise, surprise, about sex with her mother.
Later, Stevens says that he doesn't think there is much instrumental country
music. While the vast majority of popular country music songs today have lyrics, that wasn't always
the case. Many records from past decades were instrumental. In 1922, fiddler Eck Robertson
became the first country musician to make a commercial record, and the McGee brothers,
a string instrument duo,
were one of the longest-lasting acts at Nashville's Grand Ole Opry. In the 1960s and 70s,
pianist Floyd Kramer and guitarist Chet Atkins had hit records with their instrumental sounds.
Finally, Angela says that her husband Jason is, quote,
dragging her to an upcoming New Order concert, and she notes that the band members
must now be octogenarians. The members of the English rock band are actually quite a bit younger
than she anticipated. The band currently consists of Bernard Sumner, age 65, Stephen Morris, age 63,
and Gillian Gilbert, age 60. The founders of the soft rock duo Air Supply are actually closer to octogenarian age.
Graham Russell is 71, and Russell Hitchcock is 72. That's it for the Fact Check.
No Stupid Questions is produced by Freakonomics Radio and Stitcher. Our staff includes Allison
Craiglow, Greg Rippin, James Foster, Joel Meyer, Trisha Bobita, Emma Terrell, Lyric
Bowditch, Jasmine Klinger, and Jacob Clemente. Our theme song is And She Was by Talking Heads.
Special thanks to David Byrne and Warner Chapel Music. If you'd like to listen to the show ad
free, subscribe to Stitcher Premium. You can also follow us on Twitter at NSQ underscore show and on Facebook at NSQ show.
If you have a question for a future episode, please email it to NSQ at Freakonomics.com.
And if you heard Stephen or Angela reference a study, an expert, or a book that you'd like to
learn more about, you can check out Freakonomics.com slash NSQ, where we link to all of the major references
that you heard about here today. Thanks for listening.
I think the feedback will be, tell Dubner to sing Never.
The Freakonomics Radio Network.
Stitcher. The Freakonomics Radio Network.