No Stupid Questions - Why Are Stories Stickier Than Statistics? (Replay)

Episode Date: September 6, 2024

Also: are the most memorable stories less likely to be true? Stephen Dubner chats with Angela Duckworth in this classic episode from July 2020. SOURCES:Pearl S. Buck, 20th-century American novelist.J...ack Gallant, professor of neuroscience and psychology at the University of California, Berkeley.Steve Levitt, professor emeritus of economics at the University of Chicago, host of People I (Mostly) Admire, and co-author of the Freakonomics books.George Loewenstein, professor of economics and psychology at Carnegie Mellon University.Deborah Small, professor of marketing at Yale University.Adin Steinsaltz, rabbi, philosopher, and author.Diana Tamir, professor of neuroscience and psychology at Princeton University. RESOURCES:"The Representation of Semantic Information Across Human Cerebral Cortex During Listening Versus Reading Is Invariant to Stimulus Modality," by Fatma Deniz, Anwar O. Nunez-Elizalde, Alexander G. Huth and Jack L. Gallant (Journal of Neuroscience, 2019)."Reading Fiction and Reading Minds: The Role of Simulation in the Default Network," by Diana Tamir, Andrew B. Bricker, David Dodell-Feder, and Jason P. Mitchell (Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2016).Think Like a Freak, by Stephen Dubner and Steve Levitt (2014).SuperFreakonomics, by Stephen Dubner and Steve Levitt (2009).Encyclopedia of Ethical Failure, by the Department of Defense (2009)."Stories or Statistics? Farmers' Attitudes Toward Messages in an Agricultural Safety Campaign," by S. E. Morgan, H. P. Cole, T. Struttmann, and L. Piercy (Journal of Agricultural Safety and Health, 2002)."Explaining the Identifiable Victim Effect," by Karen Jenni and George Loewenstein (Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 1997)."Explanation-Based Decision Making: Effects of Memory Structure on Judgment," by N. Pennington and R. Hastie (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 1988).The Good Earth, by Pearl S. Buck (1931). EXTRAS:"Abortion and Crime, Revisited (Update)," by Freakonomics Radio (2022)."This Is Your Brain on Podcasts," by Freakonomics Radio (2016).

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Hi, NSQ listeners. If you've been following us for a while, you know that Stephen Dubner used to co-host the show. We thought you might enjoy this classic episode from 2020, featuring him in conversation with Angela. On Saturday, we'll be back with a brand new episode featuring Angela and Mike. Oh yeah, I know this, bud. You hang out with Max, right? brand new episode featuring Angela and Mike. Oh yeah, I know this, but you hang out with Max, right?
Starting point is 00:00:31 I'm Angela Duckworth. I'm Stephen Dubner. And you're listening to No Stupid Questions. Today on the show, why are stories more memorable than other types of information? Never saw a rom-com I didn't like a little bit. Steven, I wonder if you would agree with me about the following. I think stories are just about the stickiest form of information that there is and much stickier in people's minds than statistics. What do you think? I agree. And this was a great show. I enjoyed it.
Starting point is 00:01:17 Done. Drops mic. I do agree. So, I mean, you're kind of preaching to the choir. You're asking the choir if they like singing. Yeah. Kohl's to Newcastle. Although, okay, I will say there are some caveats. I think there are some people for whom data, statistics, theory, et cetera, do stick to their brains better than stories. Like who? Literally, name one person. Steve Levitt.
Starting point is 00:01:43 Really? He will forget a story but remember a statistic? Yes. I don't mean to say it's as extreme as forgetting a story. If I tell him a story, I went to the Grand Canyon and I tried to go bungee jumping, but they didn't have bungee jumping, so they shot me out of a cannon and I lived. He'll remember that. He'll probably remember that. Okay. And I will now remember that forever. Thank you. By the way, that didn't happen. Okay. But the way that memory works, you'll probably remember that. Okay. And I will now remember that forever. Thank you. By the way, that didn't happen. Okay.
Starting point is 00:02:07 But the way that memory works, you'll probably think that it actually did. I'll remember it anyway. Yes. Then I'll soon believe it. But if we're talking about the conveyance of information, if that's the purpose, then I do know people who are really good with, like I said, theory and data and statistics and so on. And that may be because they are data people and therefore they tend to dismiss stories as a form of evidence.
Starting point is 00:02:32 They down weight them. Yeah. It's an N of one, but I have spent a lot of my life thinking about like why storytelling is successful or at least useful. So give me your theory. Well, I guess if you thought about it from the evolutionary biology side, you'd say stories are sticky because we needed to pass long information
Starting point is 00:02:52 in the era before there was written language. And so you've got these massively long and complicated religious stories and mythic stories. There was the era of bards. Troubadours and the Bible, the Old Testament, at least I should specify, all of which existed pre-written language. It's hard to imagine that they were passed along orally, but they were. So here's one answer to your question, like, why does storytelling stick or does storytelling stick at the expense of something else? Think about the Bible.
Starting point is 00:03:22 It is the most read book in the history of the universe. Is it really? I just was actually wondering, is that true? We'll have to find out in fact checking, but I think that's true. Among, and whenever you're uncertain, you say among the most. Among, yeah. And I think many people would also agree that the Bible contains among the most influential set of rules in human history, which is the Ten Commandments. So So we like to think that we remember things like rules and laws and things like that. But if you ask people to name the Ten Commandments. Even if you ask Catholics, I bet it would be pretty damning as it were.
Starting point is 00:03:57 Well, so I don't know the response by denomination, but I do know that there was a survey that found that 14% of US adults could recall all Ten Commandments, but I do know that there was a survey that found that 14% of US adults could recall all 10 commandments, which I thought was pretty good, but only 71% could name even one commandment. Okay, thou shalt not kill. Was that one of them? And then covening your neighbor's wife, I remember that one. Best remembered were thou shalt not kill or murder, stealing. It's the big prohibitions. We should say there are a few different versions. Even in the Old Testament, in the Jewish Bible,
Starting point is 00:04:29 there are two, I believe, different versions, which vary slightly. So, some people remember differently. But the point is, this is the most famous set of rules in the history of the universe in what is probably the most read book in the history of the universe. And yet, most people can only name maybe one or two or three. But if you ask people who don't come from a religious tradition at all, they know the stories. Like the story of Moses is known, Adam and Eve. Noah's Ark. Right. So then you may be thinking, well, wait a minute, maybe it's just said, like, you know, memories are bad. Maybe people don't remember the 10 commandments because their memories are not good.
Starting point is 00:05:06 I can't remember anything. Right. But check this out in the same survey that found that people were so bad at recalling the 10 commandments. It turns out that 25% of the respondents could name the seven principle ingredients of a Big Mac to all beef patties, sauce,, three cheese, because I'm a sesame seed bun, and 35% could name all six kids from the Brady Bunch. So human memory clearly has power. Now the Ten Commandments, of course,
Starting point is 00:05:34 are not statistics, right? I'm not saying that this question was only about stories versus statistics, but it does make the point, I guess, that stories are sticky. Yeah, so the Ten Commandments are not statistics, but, you know, it calls to mind something like, let's say you're an institution, a government, a family,
Starting point is 00:05:51 and you're trying to tell people why they should do the right thing. I mean, look, there's a ton of research from your field that shows that telling them to do the right thing often doesn't work, but there's also research that shows that telling them the rule itself is much less effective than telling them a story. So a lawyer for the US Department of Defense
Starting point is 00:06:12 here is going to name Steve Epstein. His job was to brief supervisors in different government departments on the kind of things that their employees should and should not be doing. And he found that if he would tell them the rules and regs, that people would read it and their eyes would glaze over. So instead, he created this book of true stories that he called the Encyclopedia of Ethical Failure. And it was nothing but
Starting point is 00:06:38 a catalog of epic screw-ups perpetrated by federal workers. And his claim was that it was much more successful. You've mentioned something on the show before about the identifiable victim effect, yes? Yes, that's the work of my colleague at Wharton, Deborah Small, and then also our common friend, George Lowenstein. And that effect is, in a nutshell, that if you have a victim of, say, a crime or a war, and you talk about that one victim's story and who they were and what happened to them, that can be much more compelling. And, for example, get people to give more money to a cause than a statistic about millions of people. Right. Further evidence, the neuroscientist Jack Gallant at Berkeley, he put people in an FMRI
Starting point is 00:07:24 machine to measure their brain activity and he had them listen to stories. In fact, it was podcasts. And he found that podcasts, the storytelling stimulated much more brain activity than other types of information. You were searching Google Scholar for podcast evidence. Of course I, no, I wasn't actually. I was reading this study, I think it's from like 20 years ago or something the Journal of Agricultural Safety and Health About trying to get farmers to use tractors in the appropriate way I guess like seatbelt or what and what they found in this randomized controlled trial was very small was that stories
Starting point is 00:08:01 Actually did better than, like you said, just informing farmers of what they ought to do or providing statistics about ways that they could get injured or die. So, I think that we would agree that there's a lot of evidence that storytelling is sticky, right? Well, there's a lot of anecdotal evidence, as it were. Well, I'm reading a paper from the Journal of Experimental Psychology. It's a good journal. It shows that jurors rely heavily on stories
Starting point is 00:08:25 to decide their verdicts. Jurors confront masses of facts presented in a scrambled sequence, which is disorienting, with substantial gaps in the record filtered through the obvious personal biases of witnesses. So how do they deal with this complexity? It turns out they spontaneously construct a story
Starting point is 00:08:42 to account for this welter of information, then match their personal story with the stories told by the prosecution and defense and choose whichever side tells a story that best matches their own. So that actually to me is the best explanation of why stories work, which is that we are all narcissists to some degree. We're all creating a story. Yeah, and when we hear a story that has nothing to do with us, it could be the farmers in the study
Starting point is 00:09:11 that you're talking about looking into tractor safety. It could be people in an FMRI listening to podcasts. It could be people on a jury. It could be Moses, Cain and Abel, Noah's Ark. Whenever we hear a jury, it could be Moses, Cain and Abel, Noah's Ark. Whenever we hear a story, I believe that we inherently insert ourselves in it to a certain degree and as the story is unspooling, it appeals to the narcissist in all of us because as the cast of characters moves through time and they make decisions, we invariably put ourselves in their shoes and we think, oh,
Starting point is 00:09:43 yeah, I would have done that too, or no, no, no, I never would have made that decision. And that's why I think stories engage us in a way that statistics and data don't. Okay, so one of my favorite neuroscientists is Diana Tamir at Princeton. One of her studies was on what happens when we read fiction. And I guess you want to call us narcissists for,
Starting point is 00:10:04 like, what would I do, would I do what they did? She found that when you read fiction. And I guess you want to call us narcissists for like, what would I do? Would I do what they did? She found that when you read something like a novel, you are better able to empathize with and understand the feelings of other people. There is this kind of transporting yourself into the story. But there are other kinds of stories that are not about people, right?
Starting point is 00:10:22 I mean, you could have a story about like, a dog chasing a fox or something. Do you think we are also narcissistically wondering whether as a dog we would have made the same choice? Because lots of children's stories have no people in them. Yes, but I think that most children's stories that have animals doing things are anthropomorphized for the very purpose that you're saying they don't serve, which is they are meant to represent people. I would also challenge you to think of great or memorable stories that don't have people
Starting point is 00:10:50 in them. It reminds me a little bit of photography, like my wife Ellen, who is a photographer, and she did a lot of what she called documentary photography. Many people would call it photojournalism, whatever, but she would immerse herself in a place or a situation. Sometimes it was a war. She went to the former Soviet Union, right, as glasnost was happening. She went to Romania right after Ceausescu fell.
Starting point is 00:11:12 These very dramatic situations. And she shot this compelling stuff. And it was almost always of people. And the more I learned about photography through her, the more I realized that beginning photographers almost always don't shoot people. And the more I learned about photography through her, the more I realized that beginning photographers almost always don't shoot people. They like to shoot the dramatic landscape. The mountain range. Like if they're assigned to do something on, you know, the decline of labor in the middle of America, they'll shoot the warehouse where things used to happen.
Starting point is 00:11:40 The empty warehouse. And the reason is it takes a lot of courage to insert yourself in the lives of people and to invade their privacy and then to get in their face and photograph them. But if you think about the photographs that really connect with you, I would argue it's very similar to the stories that really connect with you. It's usually people. There are exceptions, sure, but I think that that's why photographs work very well too. They're this remarkable, frozen story that you can immerse yourself and interpret and perhaps even cast yourself into to some degree, narcissistically.
Starting point is 00:12:19 Still to come on No Stupid Questions, Stephen and Angela discuss what makes a story worth telling. Oh my god, you know, I don't care whether that's true. And now, back to Mike and Angela's conversation about the significance of stories. Back to Mike and Angela's conversation about the significance of stories. So look, I am plainly pro storytelling, but I also think that there's a lot of reasons to be careful, even skeptical of stories. For the same reason, right? Cause they're so sticky.
Starting point is 00:12:56 They're so persuasive. They're very sticky, even if they're not true or even if they're partially true. One reason that we particularly remember things are because they're anomalous. So it's like, if there's an N of one, we remember it. Oh my gosh, did you hear about that woman who did that? And then this happened? And it is wildly memorable. And the problem with that is it's very easy
Starting point is 00:13:17 to falsely interpret that anomalous event as normative. I mean, that's what happens in the news every day. We read about something that is unusual and often dramatic. And even though we know it's unusual and dramatic, we then, because we're pattern seeking animals, we assume that that's the way things are going to keep happening. So as a journalist, what's the ethical road to follow there? I'm really glad you asked that question because I think whether you do journalism or just
Starting point is 00:13:48 read journalism, obviously a lot more people read it than produce it, it's really important to challenge it as you're reading it. So if I'm reading the New York Times and I say, okay, this Times journalist is telling me that this thing happened to this person and therefore that means that the labor market is going to move in a certain direction. So as scientists like to say, the plural of anecdote is not data. But you know, in journalism, the kind of self-critical cliche is if something happens three times, it's a trend.
Starting point is 00:14:19 If you find three examples of it, then it's a thing. There's actually a study on that, by the way. That three is a magic number? Yeah, three makes a streak. Yeah. So that is not great because it's very easy then to find patterns that don't exist. And also getting back to just dramatic stories that seem to represent something much larger and often scarier than they are.
Starting point is 00:14:42 Look, a lot of psychology textbooks still contain the parable of the murder of Kitty Genovese as representing bystander apathy. The bystander effect, yeah. Right, this is I think a really good example of a story that was not true the way it was mostly portrayed and that came to stand for something that many people in the world now fear,
Starting point is 00:15:06 and they probably shouldn't. So, okay, for the few people who haven't heard it, now I'm saying this cautiously because I feel like I'm going to now implant in their hippocampus a story that they won't soon forget, but what is the Kitty Genevieve stories to you? So Kitty Genevieve was a young woman who lived in Kew Gardens, Queens,
Starting point is 00:15:24 who was murdered brutally, horribly. And that's all true. That's all true. And the guy who killed her, his name is Winston Mosley, and I believe he died recently. And it was a particularly horrible crime. But the story that came to be famous and that worked its way into psychology textbooks had to do with the fact
Starting point is 00:15:45 that there was an article written in the New York Times that described how 38 bystanders, people in the neighborhood had ignored her cries for help and that no one had done anything or called the police. That was kind of the headline story. It's an unforgettable story. And that generated the idea of bystander apathy, which is if something terrible is happening,
Starting point is 00:16:08 a lot of people don't want to get involved. And it turns out that if you pull apart the story of what actually happened that night, which is a little difficult to do because this was in the 1960s, but we went back and in our second book, Super Freakonomics, we actually retold the story as well as we could, including the
Starting point is 00:16:25 incentives for the reporters involved and the police involved. And it turns out that the story as rendered in the New York Times and the story is kind of magnified throughout our collective memory and into the psychology textbooks was just not right. It wasn't that nobody shouted out and tried to stop it. It wasn't that nobody had called the police, apparently, although that's contested. The fact was, was that the murder was actually interrupted. The guy had attacked her and then was scared off, ran away,
Starting point is 00:16:58 but then came back later and finished the crime. So it was a tragedy, but the general perception was that somehow 38 people were standing at their windows looking down and watching this happen and doing nothing. And that was very, very, very untrue. But the story was so compelling that it lived on. Was it an exaggeration or was it a total falsehood? It sounds like it may have been an exaggeration.
Starting point is 00:17:23 I would say it was somewhere between a medium and a grotesque exaggeration. You're now talking about how the paradox may be that the stickiest of stories are least likely to be true. Is that going too far? I think that's going too far. Do I need to step back three feet? I mean, the story of Adolf Hitler is a pretty sticky story. to step back three feet. I mean, the story of Adolf Hitler is a pretty sticky story. And the story of Adolf Hitler that most of us know from the history books seems to be pretty true.
Starting point is 00:17:51 So I don't know if I'd go that far. Well, I'm not saying that every extremes are, but in my field in social science, we're living through what is net a good thing, which is I think they're calling it the replicability crisis or the replicability revolution. And it's the idea that there are these like gee whiz findings that are just so surprising, like, oh, did you know that the color of the wall that you're looking at is going to, you know,
Starting point is 00:18:13 determine your mood and your behavior for the rest of the day, things like that. And the more improbable and surprising, astonishing and therefore sticky, the finding one could argue like Not out knowing anything else actually the less likely it's true Yeah so I'm so glad you bring that up because we've written about a lot of things that I could see easily trying to dismiss as Just a story right if you say that the legalization of abortion led to less crime because it meant that there were fewer unwanted children being born and social science shows that unwantedness is a really bad thing to have as a child.
Starting point is 00:18:51 So look, we tell the kind of stories that one is right to be skeptical of and right to challenge, which is why, whether it's in books or now in the podcast, there is what I guess I would consider a sort of responsible version of storytelling. And this gets us back to what you had asked at the beginning, which is, are stories stickier than data? And my answer would be, yes, they probably are,
Starting point is 00:19:17 but for sticky stories to also be believable, you should include as much data as you possibly can. And so that to me is why a sort of hybrid version of storytelling is very compelling, which is yes, it's causal. This happened, which led to this happening, which led to this happening. But those three sentences alone are not enough.
Starting point is 00:19:39 They're illustrative. And you need to back them up with evidence. But I argue that stories, because we gravitate toward them and because they are sticky, it's important to tell them, but to challenge them during the telling as much as you possibly can. So to include as much data as you can,
Starting point is 00:19:56 to include the magnitude of the effect, to include the time series, because if there's a huge effect, but it's gone within a year, well, the story becomes a lot less dramatic, but it's gone within a year, well, the story becomes a lot less dramatic, but it's important to tell that, because maybe it was novelty more than anything.
Starting point is 00:20:12 And so when you're telling the story, and you're saying, I believe this is what caused that event, it's also imperative to introduce the other possible causes and explain why they're not true. And this is one thing that good academic papers do. They'll say, we believe there's a strong relationship between, let's say, the number of police and the crime rate. Well, let's have some evidence, but let's also have some other potential factors that
Starting point is 00:20:42 may have led to less crime. And let's interrogate each of those as well, because a story on its own is just not compelling enough to be accepted. Well, it might be compelling enough, but it ought not be, right? Like you basically have to take the responsibility as a reader and certainly as a writer
Starting point is 00:20:59 to not prey upon our narrative loving human nature. Right, and this is why Twitter is both very effective and very frustrating because you can tell a story in 140 characters and it might even be mostly true, but what it omits is almost certainly large enough to make you question the validity of the story itself. The other thing that I find so interesting about storytelling in the modern era, there's a lot of discussion about who, quote, owns the story and who's entitled to tell the story. Wait, what does that mean? If you're telling a story about, let's say, accomplishment or education or crime or some
Starting point is 00:21:41 kind of social factor and it involves some sort of demographic groups. Maybe it's a gender group or an ethnic group or a racial group, whatever. You know, if you're describing that and you're from outside of one of those groups, there's a question of, well, that's not really your story to describe. And I find that to be a really interesting dilemma.
Starting point is 00:22:01 I thought about this, I recently read, and I cannot even tell you why. Oh, because I just found it in a used bookstore, The Good Earth. Did you read The Good Earth by Pearl S. Buck? We all remember her middle initial, I don't know why. You loved it, right? You read it and you loved it.
Starting point is 00:22:17 I loved it, I did. She won the Nobel Prize in Literature. So I just finished this like a week or two ago and I remember thinking, okay, she grew up largely in China, but she's a white woman from suburban Philadelphia really. And she wrote about my cultural tradition. By the way, as you probably also know, was made into an Academy Award winning film where of course, largely the cast was white. None of this would play well, I think in 2020 and I think that's largely good. So that book was published in like the 1930s or something.
Starting point is 00:22:48 Yeah. So look, the 1930s are not the 2020s, and circumstances change a lot. And I think that most people are smart enough to recognize that too. And yet we see a lot of now retroactive questioning of who owns a story and whether a story should be considered legitimate. In this case, do you, did you consider her telling of that story despite her outsider status to be worthwhile and legitimate? Forget about whether it holds up over time as a novel.
Starting point is 00:23:17 Like who does Pearl S. Buck think she is to be writing the history or sharing that when it wasn't really quote-unquote hers. I guess if I were really offended, I wouldn't have bought the book or read it, but many people probably are. She wrote The Good Earth relatively early in her literary career. So she was alive for a long time while controversy was swirling and she was going back and forth with her critics. And yet some questioned her legitimacy. But yeah, I mean, I thought she defended herself pretty well. I was not offended. So when it comes to storytelling, my favorite moment thinking about stories, because if
Starting point is 00:23:56 you're a human, you think about stories. But if you're a writer, which I have been pretty much my whole life, you really think about stories. Yeah, it's your currency. The biggest smile I ever got is when I was writing my first book, which was a family memoir about my parents having both been Jewish and both converting before they met each other to Catholicism and then me growing up very Catholic. And then you become a Jew again.
Starting point is 00:24:16 Yeah, yeah. So as part of this reporting, it took place over maybe seven or eight years, there was a lot of biographical reporting and so on, but I also just was absorbing a lot of theology. And so I was reading books, obviously, and going to lectures. And there was this one lecture by this very aged Jewish scholar named Adin Steinsaltz, who had compiled the modern version of the Talmud that many people study. And I attended the lecture and it was really great,
Starting point is 00:24:45 and I learned a lot from it. And then came the question and answer session, and someone asked him a question and it was such a difficult question that I felt I was the one being asked. I was horrified. It was so hard. The question was this. They said, excuse me, Rabbi Stein-Salz, if someone asked you to say, what has Judaism contributed to the world, what would you say? And I thought, oh my God, it's like such a challenging and hard question that's almost asking for a defensive answer. And he'd been talking earlier about the origins of the afterlife, the idea of the afterlife,
Starting point is 00:25:23 which was kind of simultaneously, as I recall, a Greek and Jewish idea. That there is an afterlife. Yeah, that there is one. And so he was asked this, what seemed to me, impossibly difficult question. Like, this is gonna take months to answer. And he just kind of smiled and he said,
Starting point is 00:25:40 yes, I would say that Judaism gave the world the idea of a happy ending to every story. The happy ending referring to the afterlife. And I thought, oh my God, you know, I don't care whether that's true. I hope he was able to say drops Mike, but I'm guessing that given the era and given that he was a distinguished rabbi, but he would if he could, don't you think? It was a Mike drop line. And so the thing to remember about, you know, a happy ending, I think we often fill those in for ourselves, even if we don't think they're going to go that way. And that really gets us to optimism and why, you know, stories help us, I think,
Starting point is 00:26:18 believe the best in ourselves and others that may not always be warranted, but it's desirable. And I'm no different, by the way. Never, never saw a rom-com I didn't like a little bit. And that's mostly because they have happy endings. I think what's so interesting about stories is that it's as if every story really has a moral or will make one up for it. Because we're always trying to like draw inferences and lessons learned about the universe or human nature that will then project onto our next experience. I mean, do you think we are the storytelling species? It is fascinating
Starting point is 00:26:55 to me how strong our narrative muscle is and we're flexing it all the time, whether it's good for us or not. You don't think dogs tell each other stories? Is that what they're doing? When they're sniffing each other? When they're sniffing each other's butts? Oh yeah, I know this butt. You hang out with Max, right?
Starting point is 00:27:14 I can tell there's a little bit of Max right there. How the hell are you? Yeah, but that's a pretty short story. I don't know. It's a little more complicated than that. Coming up after the break, a fact check of today's conversation. This episode was produced by me, Rebecca Lee Douglas, and now here's a fact check of today's questions. Stephen thinks that the Bible is, quote, the most read book in the history of the universe.
Starting point is 00:27:51 But Angela is skeptical. Unfortunately, LexisNexis doesn't have many statistics on intergalactic literature, but it seems safe to say that the Bible is the most read book in human history. According to the Guinness Book of World Records, approximately five billion copies have been sold. However, it's hard to know exact numbers, given that it was produced and distributed by countless publishers over many centuries.
Starting point is 00:28:16 The entire Bible has been translated into at least 349 languages, while 2,123 languages have a translated version of at least one book of the Bible. Stephen says that stories from the Old Testament were passed down in an era before written language. It's true that early Israel was an oral society, and much of the Bible was passed down by word of mouth before it was finally transcribed between the 8th and the 6th century BC. But evidence of origins of written communication go
Starting point is 00:28:49 back much earlier, to around 3000 BC in ancient Egypt and Mesopotamia. Although the earliest surviving work of literature was not written until another thousand years after that, ancient Egypt and Mesopotamia should also be credited as, among the first cultures to describe an afterlife, along with the early Greek and Jewish people. Later on, Stephen theorizes that the most compelling photojournalism features people, although he admits that there are some exceptions,
Starting point is 00:29:20 which I'm sure national geographic photographers would appreciate. But it does seem that the most lauded documentary photography of all time is of people. Only about 10 to 15 percent of Time Magazine's most influential images of all time highlight non-human subjects, including journalist Sam Shear's 1937 photo of the Hindenburg disaster and the infamous Loch Ness Monster image, now known to be a hoax. That's it for the fact check. No Stupid Questions is part of the Freakonomics Radio Network, which also
Starting point is 00:29:56 includes Freakonomics Radio, People I Mostly Admire, and The Economics of Everyday Things. All our shows are produced by Stitcher and Runbud Radio. Lyric Bowditch is our production associate. This episode was mixed by James Foster, with help from Greg Rippon. Our theme song was composed by Luis Guerra. You can follow us on Twitter, at nsq.underscore.show. If you have a question for a future episode, please email it to nsq at Freakonomics.com. To learn more or to read episode transcripts, visit Freakonomics.com slash nsq. Thanks for listening.
Starting point is 00:30:40 In the modern era, there's a lot of discussion about who quote owns the story. Wait, what does that mean? Can you, sorry, I said mm as if I actually knew, but I... I was faking comprehension. The Freakonomics Radio Network. The hidden side of everything. Stitcher. inside of everything.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.