Sold a Story - [RERELEASE] At a Loss for Words: What's Wrong with How Schools Teach Reading
Episode Date: March 30, 2023Molly Woodworth had a secret: She couldn’t read very well. She fought her way through text by looking at the first letter of a word and thinking of something that made sense. Reading was ...slow and laborious. Then she learned that her daughter's school was actually teaching kids to read that way. In this documentary, originally published in August 2019, host Emily Hanford reveals that many kids are being taught the habits of struggling readers. Winner of a Gracie Award and finalist for an EWA Public Service Award.  Read more: How a flawed idea is teaching millions of kids to be poor readersSupport this show: Donate to APM Reports
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Whether you're doing a dance to your favorite artist in the office parking lot,
or being guided into Warrior I in the break room before your shift,
whether you're running on your Peloton tread at your mom's house while she watches the baby,
or counting your breaths on the subway.
Peloton is for all of us, wherever we are whenever we need it, download the free Peloton app today.
Peloton app available through free tier or paid subscription starting at 12.99 per month.
This is Emily Hanford, host of Soul to Story. We'll have a bonus episode of the Soul to Story
podcast coming soon. If you haven't heard Soled-A-Story yet,
please stop here and go back to episode one. If you have heard Soled-A-Story and want more,
you're in the right place. Before I started working on the Soled-A-Story podcast,
I made a series of audio documentaries about how kids learn to read. This one is called
Adaloss for Words. It's about the queuing idea that you heard
about in Solta's story. I made this documentary when I first figured out that queuing was a big
part of what's wrong with how many schools teach reading. This is Adaloss for Words, first
released on August 22nd, 2019. From American public media, this is an APM reports documentary. Molly Woodworth
was a kid who seemed to do well at everything. Good grades in the Gifted and Talented program,
but she had a secret. I fooled everyone. Molly couldn't read very well. I was totally
lost. There was no rhyme or reason to reading for me. When a teacher would dictate a word
and say, tell me how you think you can spell it,
I sat there with my mouth open,
why other kids gave spellings and I thought,
how do they even know where to begin?
You know, I was totally, it didn't make sense to me.
Her classmates just seemed to know how to read,
but Molly didn't, and she says no one ever taught her.
So she did the best she could.
I came up with my own way to read.
Number one, memorize lots of words.
So you would read along and there would just be some percentage of the words you just had memorized.
Yeah, and I didn't know that then, but I had a really, really good memory.
She says words were like pictures to her.
When she came across one,
she didn't have in her visual memory.
She'd look at the first letter
and come up with a word that seemed to make sense.
Reading was kind of like a game of 20 questions.
What word could this be?
Most of the time, Molly could get the gist
of what she was reading,
but getting through text took forever.
I hated reading because it was taxing.
My brain hurt by the end of it.
And I'd get through a chapter and it was like, ugh,
I was done, I wasn't excited to learn,
I didn't wanna do anymore,
it took all of the wind out of my sail to get to that point.
It was clear to her that other kids could read faster
and better, but she had no idea how they did it. If she was called on to read out loud in
class, she'd say she had a stomach ache and go to the nurse. It all worked well enough
to keep her on the honors track through high school. Molly's reading problems didn't
really catch up with her until it was time to take college entrance exams.
I couldn't get through the ACT. Someone in the gifted and talented program couldn't get through
the test and it wasn't again. It wasn't because I was not intelligent. It was because I could not
get through the reading fast enough. My tools were too slow.
I'll tell you what happened with Molly in the ACT at the end of this program.
But for now, we're going gonna fast forward about a decade.
Molly gets married, she has a little girl.
That's Molly's daughter Claire,
playing with my recording equipment.
It's really loud.
It's loud, I know, I should turn it down.
Claire's in first grade.
Learning to read has been hard for her.
So once a week, Molly brings Claire to a reading center.
All right, tell me the sounds in clap.
Claire is working on phonyma awareness.
That's the understanding that spoken words are made up of individual sounds or phonies.
Let's trip without the urr.
You are so good at that.
Claire first came to this reading center
before she started kindergarten.
Her mom wanted to make sure she got off
to a good start in reading.
And I felt really comfortable
with where she was at going into kindergarten.
You know, she had a good base.
There was no alarming signs.
You know, she was on track.
But alarm bells started going off
when Molly saw how Claire was being taught to read in school.
One day Molly was volunteering in Claire's classroom.
The class was reading a book
and the teacher was telling the kids
to practice the strategies that good readers use.
And she said, if you don't know the word,
just look at this picture up here.
There was a fox in a bear in the picture
and the word was bear and she said, so look at this picture up here. There was a fox in a bear in the picture, and the word was bear, and she said,
so look at the first letter.
Okay, it's a B, what sounds a B, you know,
is it the fox or the bear?
Molly was stunned.
I thought, oh my god, those are my strategies.
Those are the things I taught myself
to look like a good reader and not the things
that good readers do, and I didn't know a good reader to do,
but I knew it wasn't that.
Things didn't get better as kindergarten went on.
Claire would get books that had words like sailboat and butterfly.
Big words she didn't know how to read yet, but there was a picture and she was supposed
to look at the first letter and guess.
And it just continued.
I would see these things that were doing my little dirty secrets and they were being
taught. These kids were being taught.
These kids are being taught to look like good readers.
My survival techniques, that was their bag of tricks.
Molly went to Claire's teacher and said she was concerned about the way kids were being
taught.
The teacher said she was teaching reading the way the curriculum told her to. From APM reports, this is, at a loss for words,
what's wrong with how schools teach reading?
I'm Emily Hanford.
The way Claire Woodworth was taught to read
is rooted in an idea about reading
that was debunked decades ago by cognitive scientists.
Yet this idea remains deeply embedded
in teaching practices and
curriculum materials that are commonly used in elementary schools. As a result,
in classrooms across the country, children are being taught to read the way that
poor readers read. In other words, the strategies that people with weak reading
skills used to get by are the very strategies that many beginning readers
are taught in school.
This makes it harder for many kids to learn to read.
And children who don't get off to a good start in reading,
find it difficult to ever master the process.
This can lead to a downward spiral
where behavior, vocabulary, knowledge,
and other cognitive skills are eventually affected by slow reading development.
A disproportionate number of poor readers become high school dropouts.
In the United States, a third of fourth graders can't read on a basic level.
Most students are still not proficient readers by the time they finish high school.
This hour, I'm going to show you how a disproven idea
about how people read is part of the problem
and how it is still widespread in curriculum materials
that school districts spend hundreds of millions
of dollars of taxpayer money on.
We're gonna begin with the idea itself.
For that, we need a little history.
People have been arguing for centuries
about how children should be taught to read.
There are basically two perspectives.
One view is that kids need to focus first on sounds and letters.
MacGuffee's Ecclectic Primmer, Lesson One.
The Sounds and Letters approach, also known as phonics, was popularized in the 1800s with
the MacGuffee readers.
This is from a MacGuffuffy audio book I found on YouTube.
C.
Er.
T.
Arat.
A cat.
The other view is that children shouldn't focus on sounds and letters.
They should focus instead on whole words.
This is Dick and Jane reading level two.
The whole word approach was perhaps best embodied in the Dick and Jane books that first appeared
in the 1930s.
This is a guy who grew up with Dick and Jane, reading one of the books on YouTube.
Come here, Dick.
Come and see Puff.
The Dick and Jane books rely on lots of repetition and pictures to support the meaning of the text.
C-Puff play! C-Puff jump! C-Puff jump and play!
In the whole word approach, the idea is that learning to read is a visual memory process.
C words enough and you eventually store them in your memory as visual images.
With phonics, the idea is that children learn to read words by sounding them out.
Reading instruction was basically a series of pendulum swings between whole word and phonics
until the late 1960s when a new idea came along. The basic theory was first presented in 1967
at the American Educational Research Association conference in New York.
There's no audio of the event, but here's what happened.
An education professor named Ken Goodman presented a paper called Reading, a Cycle-Linguistic
Guessing Game.
In the paper, Goodman rejected the idea that reading is a precise process that involves
exact or detailed
perception of letters or words.
Instead, he argued that as people read, they guess the words, using various kinds of information
or cues.
He grouped these cues into three categories.
One cue for figuring out a word is what kind of word would work. For example, is it a noun or a verb?
Goodman called these syntactic cues.
The noun, road, his.
The missing word must be a noun.
Maybe it's bicycle or motorcycle or horse.
Another cue for figuring out a word is to look at the meaning of the sentence.
What word would make sense here? The cowboy, road, his. Another cue for figuring out a word is to look at the meaning of the sentence.
What word would make sense here?
The cowboy rode his.
Ah, a cowboy rides a horse.
Or maybe the word is pony.
Goodman called these semantic cues.
A third cue to figure out a word is to look at the letters.
Goodman called those graphic or grapho-finemic cues.
The cowboy road is...
Oh, the word begins with pfft, it's probably pony. Goodman proposed that as people become better
readers, they rely less and less on graphic cues. Instead, they use context to predict the words,
and just sample from the letters to confirm their predictions.
context to predict the words and just sample from the letters to confirm their predictions. This was a new idea about how people read. It helped form the theoretical basis of an
approach to teaching reading known as whole language. In whole language, learning to read
is not about memorizing words as in the whole word approach. And it isn't about sounding out words as in phonics. Reading is coming up with words that make sense using what came to
be known among educators as the three-cuing system. For Goodman, accurate word
recognition was not necessarily the goal of reading. The goal is to comprehend
text. If the sentences are making sense, the reader must be getting the words right, or right enough.
I wish I had a garden that told here
it's some Pharisees.
This recording of a boy reading was part of a BBC radio program
about Goodman's work produced in 1986.
Plot them in your ground since broke. work produced in 1986. What the boy reads is not exactly what the text says. Here's the
announcer explaining to the radio audience. When the boy read is instead of R and you're instead of the, he made a couple of miscues.
That's what Ken Goodman called them.
Here he is in the BBC program.
And a miscue is very simply someplace where something unexpected happens in oral reading,
where the reader doesn't what we expected the reader to do.
That's a misqueue.
By no means is the goal to produce misqueue-free reading.
In fact, I keep saying that that's Ken's wife yet a good man.
They work together.
In my teaching, I want to help kids produce more higher quality misqueues.
A high quality misqueue is one that makes sense, where the meaning
of the sentences preserved, even if the exact words are not read. And what we have to
let the reader know is, it's perfectly all right when you come to words you don't know.
We all do that in our reading. And what we do, we have a lot of strategies as adults.
Legitimate strategies. We can skip it. We can read back. We can keep reading.
The Goodman's traveled all over the world in the 1970s and 80s,
talking to educators about their theory of how people read.
One person they met was a developmental psychologist from New Zealand.
My name is Marie Clay.
Marie Clay created a reading intervention program for struggling first graders
called Reading Recovery.
It became one of the most widely used reading intervention programs in the world.
This interview is from a video tribute to Clay, produced after she died.
The work that I do is not just practical work.
The work that I do is founded on pretty strong theory.
Clay's theory also relied on queuing.
Her queues became widely known among teachers as MSV.
M for using meaning to figure out what a word is, S for using sentence structure, and V for using visual information.
That is, the letters in the word. In the queuing theory of how reading works, when a child comes to a word, she doesn't know. The teacher encourages her to think of a word that makes sense,
and asks, does it look right?
Does it sound right?
If a word checks out on the basis of those questions,
the child is getting it.
She's on the path to skilled reading.
Mari Clay and Ken and Yeta Goodman were trying to understand what goes on in
people's minds as they read. They couldn't actually get inside anyone's head, so
they observed people as they read out loud. The theories they developed were good
guesses based on what it seems like people are doing when they read. But it
turns out skilled reading doesn't quite work the way they thought.
The laboratory for the neurodevelopment of reading and language.
That's your lab.
Okay.
Hi.
I'm at the University of Maryland to visit a reading lab.
I don't know quite what I was expecting, but this wasn't it.
There's no fancy equipment, no lab text working on experiments.
It's just an office with a couple of desks, an old couch,
and a graduate student's bike up against the wall.
This is our kind of humble facility.
That's Donald Bulger, also known as DJ.
He studies how reading works in the brain.
We have testing rooms upstairs.
We might get a little bit.
We're going upstairs in a bit to watch
a demonstration of one of DJ's experiments. But first, I want to give you some background.
Over the past 50 years or so,
scientists in labs and classrooms all over the world
have done thousands of studies about how skilled reading
works and how people learn to do it.
Something they were especially interested in early on
is whether skilled readers use context
to read words, or whether they rely on the letters in the words.
A couple of graduate students at the University of Michigan thought the context idea made sense.
It seemed likely that as people get better at reading, they would rely more on their
knowledge of vocabulary and language structure to recognize words, and wouldn't need to
pay as much attention
to the letters.
In 1975, graduate students' key Stanovich and Richard West set out to see if this was
the case in their lab.
They recruited readers of various ages and abilities and gave them a series of word-reading
tasks.
What they discovered surprised them.
It was the less skilled readers who were more
dependent on context for word recognition. The skilled readers were able to recognize
words without relying on context at all. Other researchers have done similar experiments
and it turns out the ability to read words in isolation instantly and accurately is the hallmark of being a skilled reader.
This is now one of the most well replicated findings in all of reading research. In addition,
experiments show skilled readers do not read words as visual images. Instead, they very
quickly recognize a word as a sequence of letters. That's how a reader knows the difference between house and horse, for example. To better understand how all this
works, we're going back to the reading lab at the University of Maryland. We're
heading up to one of the testing rooms, Professor DJ Bolger mentioned earlier,
to see a demonstration of an experiment he first did when he was a graduate
student. Two of DJ's students have volunteered to be the guinea pigs for today's demonstration.
We climb to the top floor of the building and enter a small room with low ceilings and
no windows.
There are two cubicles set up with computers.
It feels like a tiny call center in an attic.
Student Alyssa Cole runs through the experiment first.
Alyssa, you can start and hit number one.
Alyssa is going to learn how to read some English words that are spelled using Korean letters.
These are simple words like bud and duck.
But Alyssa doesn't know how to read words spelled with Korean letters.
So she's kind of like a typical kindergartener.
She knows the meaning and pronunciation of these words,
but she doesn't know how to read them. She's going to be taught using an approach that
calls her attention to how the sounds in each word are represented by letters.
As each sound in the word is articulated, the Korean letter that represents that sound is highlighted on the computer screen.
Alissa is learning in what DJ Bulger called the phonics condition.
The other student, Hannah Weisman, is learning the same words in a different way.
DJ called it the holistic condition.
But, duck, kin.
Hannah is seeing the whole word as it's red.
There's no sounding out, no highlights on the screen that help Hannah understand how
each sound is represented by letters.
This is the whole word method, the Dick and Jane approach.
That was so hard.
Oh my god.
When DJ first did this experiment in the early 2000s,
there was already lots of research that showed phonics
is more effective than whole word for teaching people to read.
What DJ wanted to know is, why?
In the real experiment, college students were trained and tested four times over four days.
The students who were taught whole word did better than the phonics students at first.
They were able to memorize some of the words and do better on the tests.
But by day four, the students who learned in the phonics condition were doing better.
Not only were they better at reading the words they'd been taught, they were better at
reading words they'd never seen before.
The big news of the experiment came when DJ and his colleagues got a peek inside the
brains of their subjects, using functional magnetic resonance imaging, or FMRI.
We wanted to kind of get an inside glimpse of what these brains would look like in these
two different learning methods. And I was expecting that those with holistic learning method would look almost like you're
round of the male dyslexic.
And that many children with dyslexia learn to memorize lots of whole words, but they actually
don't tend to activate the areas of the brain that are associated with phonology and pronunciation.
And this is exactly what he found. His experiment and other studies show that people who are
taught phonics learn better because focusing on letters and sounds increases activity in
the area of the brain that is best wired for reading.
So if you teach people through whole word method, you're teaching them to read like a dyslexic
reads.
That is correct.
However, just because a student is taught to read with the whole word method you're teaching them to read like a dyslexic reads. That is correct. However, just because a student is taught to read with the whole word method doesn't mean
he'll end up stuck reading words that way.
In DJ's study, about half the students in the whole word group were able to get beyond
memorizing words.
They figured out the relationships between the sounds and the letters, but half the students
in the whole word group weren't able to teach themselves to read.
That's in contrast to the phonics group
where everyone learned to read.
Phonics worked for everybody,
for all of the participants in the phonics group.
They were uniformly doing well.
What does all of this have to do with the idea
of using context to read?
After all, in DJ's study,
the students were reading isolated words. Wouldn't
putting those words in the context of a sentence help. Well, think about it. If you're a
beginning reader and you don't know any of the words in a sentence, context isn't going
to help you much. If you already know how to read a lot of words, it's a different story.
Even expert readers need context in some cases.
Take a word like match.
We can't even know what match means unless it's in context because it can mean a competition,
it could mean something you'd like to fire with, you know, it could mean two things that
look alike or the same.
This is David Kilpatrick.
He's a psychology professor at SUNY Courtland in upstate New York, and the author of a book
about preventing reading difficulties.
We need context for comprehension for understanding, nobody questions that. But the confusion is that
when you see the word match, the word match jumps out at you. You don't need context to figure out
that that's the word match. You need context to figure out the meaning. If you're a skilled reader,
you know the word match instantly. Whether that word is by itself or in a sentence.
In fact, your brain has gotten so good at reading words that you process the word match
faster than you process a picture of a match.
You know tens of thousands of words instantly on site.
How did you learn to do that?
It happens through a process called orthographic mapping.
Orthographic mapping occurs when you attend to the letters in a written word and link the
words pronunciation with its sequence of letters.
Orthographic mapping requires an awareness of the speech sounds in words and an understanding
of how those sounds are represented by letters.
In other words, you gotta have phonic skills.
Here's David Kilpatrick.
It's really sequential.
First you develop a mastery of the code and then that allows you to become better at the
orthographic mapping process.
So once you are good at phonics, you teach
yourself new words that could anchor it in your long-term memory. By the time a
typically developing reader gets to about second grade, he needs just a few
exposures to a word through both its pronunciation and its spelling. And bam, the
word is orthographically mapped to his memory. He doesn't recognize that word because he's memorized it
as a visual image.
He recognizes it because at some point,
he successfully sounded it out.
The more words he maps to his memory like this,
the more he can focus on the meaning of what he's reading.
He's not using his brain power to identify words.
He's using his brain power to comprehend what he's reading. He's not using his brain power to identify words. He's using his
brain power to comprehend what he's reading. That child is a skilled reader.
What's the process for people who are not skilled readers? They only sample from
the letters because they're not good at sounding them out and they use context.
This is David Kilpatrick again. So the three-cueing system is the way poor readers read.
And if teachers use the three-cueing system is the way poor readers read.
And if teachers use the three-cueing system to teach reading, they're not just teaching
kids the habits of poor readers.
David Kill Patrick says they are actually impeding the orthographic mapping process.
So the minute you ask them just to pay attention to the first slider or look at the picture,
look at the context, you're drawing their attention away from the very thing that they need to interact with in order for them to either read the word or to
remember the word. Some kids realize pretty quickly that sounding out a word is the most efficient and
reliable way to know what it is. They don't necessarily need to be taught this.
They figure it out.
Those kids tend to have good phonological skills.
It's not difficult for them to understand the ways
that sounds and letters work.
But if this doesn't come easily to you,
say your Molly Woodworth,
who we met at the beginning of the program.
If you're Molly and the sounds and letters thing
just doesn't make sense,
and no one teaches it to you, you're going to come sounds and letters thing just doesn't make sense and no one teaches
it to you, you're going to come up with a bunch of other strategies to try to get by.
Reading for you is kind of like being a detective.
You're hunting everywhere for clues.
Now consider a kid who's in the middle.
Okay phonological skills, not great.
Maybe he could eventually figure out reading on his own.
But then along comes his teacher who tells him,
being a good reader is like being a detective.
You need to search for clues and develop a bunch of strategies
to solve all those tricky words.
In the United States today,
this is how many children are being taught to read.
According to David Killpatrick, the three-cuing system is ubiquitous in American schools.
Coming up after the break, we're going to find out what three-cuing looks like in the classroom.
Why schools are still teaching it and how it harms children.
You're listening to, at a loss for words, from APM Reports.
There's more at our website, apmreports.org.
You can find an annotated version of this story with links to articles about the Cognitive Science Research.
Support for APM Reports comes from the Spencer Foundation and Luminof Foundation.
Back in a moment, this is APM American Public Media.
What you watch depends on what kind of majoring.
Sometimes you're craving comedies like friends or South Park and sometimes you're more into
dramas like HBO's Succession and House of the Dragon.
There's also cooking shows like Chopped and Beat Bobby Flay and even movies like The Lord
of the Rings and Shazam, Theory of the Gods.
Well, Max is the streaming destination that has the best of entertainment for whatever
mood you're in anytime.
And plant start at his little is $9.99 a month.
Max, the one to watch.
Subscription required.
Visit max.com
Sold a story comes from an investigative journalism team at American public media, that
is nonprofit and nonpartisan.
We only take on investigations that we think can prompt meaningful change.
Join us on this mission today by making a donation.
Go to soldastory.org slash donate or follow the link in our show notes.
Welcome back.
I'm Emily Hanford and this is Adelaoss for Words, a documentary from APM Reports. We're going to Manhattan now to meet Erica Meltzer. Until a few years ago,
she was an SAT tutor. This was $400 an hour tutoring. Kids who went to schools considered
among the best in New York City. Erica was startled, by the way, some of her students
read. They would get to an unfamiliar word, they would look at the beginning of the word,
and then they would just sort of guess for the rest of it.
They wouldn't even try to sound it out.
They would just plug in a word that looked like the word that was there,
and it wouldn't occur to them that they were misreading the word.
These were not students with diagnosed learning disabilities.
She says it was hard to raise their test scores.
Then I was like, what is this?
Erica started searching online
and came across an article about the three-cuing system.
It was written by a cognitive psychologist
named Marilyn Adams.
The article describes how the three-cuing system
conflicts with what researchers have figured
out about reading.
How Maryland came to write this article has an interesting backstory.
In 1991, Maryland had written a book summarizing the research on how children learned to read.
One big takeaway from the book is that becoming a skilled reader of English requires knowledge
of sound spelling correspondences. Another big takeaway is that many kids were not being taught
this in school. Soon after the book was published, Marilyn was describing her
findings to a group of teachers and state education officials in Sacramento
California. She was sensing discomfort and confusion in the room. And I just
stopped and said, what is it that I'm missing?
What is it that we need to talk about?
A woman raised her hand and asked, what does this have to do with the three-cueing system?
Maryland didn't know what the three-cueing system was.
I think I blew all of their fuses that I did not, since this was so fundamental to being
an elementary reading teacher.
The teachers explained the three-cuing system to her. They said readers use meaning, sentence
structure, and visual cues to read. Maryland thought this made perfect sense. We absolutely
use all of those things to comprehend what we're reading. But Maryland soon figured out
the disconnect. Teachers understood these cues as the way readers identify words.
In their minds, it was a reason not to do much phonics instruction.
Most important thing was for the children to understand and enjoy the text,
and from that understanding and joy of reading,
the words on the page would just pop out at them.
She would explain to teachers at every opportunity
that explicitly teaching children about sound spelling
correspondence is essential to ensure all kids
get off to a good start in reading.
But she got tons of pushback.
They didn't want to teach phonics.
They told her phonics instruction kills the joy of reading.
In the 1990s, there was a big national fight going on about how kids should be taught
to read.
On one side, phonics.
On the other side, the whole language approach rooted in the queuing theory.
Marilyn Adams wrote her article about encountering the three-cuing system in 1998.
She hoped the article would help put the idea to rest.
The scientific research on reading was gaining traction at this point.
A National Reading Panel report commissioned by Congress came out in 2000.
It documented overwhelming evidence that phonics instruction enhances children's success
in learning to read.
Many whole language supporters eventually accepted the weight of the scientific evidence
about the importance of phonics instruction.
They started adding phonics and renamed their approach balance literacy.
But they didn't get rid of the three-cuing system.
It's not hard to find classrooms where children are taught QA.
The power that we are going to learn today, it's called picture power.
Can we say picture power?
Picture power!
And I'm going to put...
This is a video posted on a website for teachers.
It's a kindergarten class in Oakland, California.
The lesson is part of the Units of study for teaching reading series, more commonly known
as readers' workshop.
Here's the teacher describing the goal of the lesson.
The learning goal would be that the children were able to use a picture to figure out an
unknown word, or that they would be able to use the picture in a first sound to determine
an unknown word in their book.
In the garden.
That's the book the class is reading.
On each page, there's a picture of something
you might find in a garden.
The sentence on every page is the same,
except for the last word.
Look at the caterpillar.
The kids have been taught to memorize the words look at and the.
The challenge is getting the last word in the sentence.
The lesson plan tells the teacher to cover up the word with a sticky note.
The wiggly kindergartners come to a page with a picture of a butterfly.
The teacher tells the kids she's guessing the word is going to be butterfly.
She uncovers the word.
Look at that. It starts with the book, book, book. So let's read it together.
Look at the butterfly.
As you can hear, this lesson includes some attention to sounds and letters.
In fact, the lesson plan says this lesson teaches phonics.
But the students were not taught to sound out words in this lesson.
They were taught the queuing system.
The author of the lesson Lucy Cockins refers often to queuing in her published work.
Queuing is foundational to another approach to teaching reading known as
Fountis and Penel Literacy.
Irene Fountis and Gaysu Penel are education professors who have written many
books for teachers, including a bestseller called Guided Reading.
They also sell a reading assessment system to schools that uses what are called
leveled books. Children start with
predictable books like in the garden and they move up levels as they're able to
read the words. But many of the words in those books, Butterfly, Caterpillar,
those are words beginning readers haven't been taught to sound out yet. One
purpose of the books is to teach kids that when they get to a word they don't know,
they can use context to figure it out.
Teacher Margaret Goldberg remembers a moment when she realized what a problem this was.
A first grader named Rodney came to a page with a picture of a girl looking an ice cream cone
and a dog looking a bone. The text said,
My little dog likes to eat with me.
But Rodney said,
My dog likes to lick his bone.
Rodney breathes right through it,
unaware that he hadn't read the sentence on the page.
And that's one of the things that I started noticing
with the students is that when they were given texts
that they couldn't read,
they would just make it up.
And a lot of times the making it up looked close enough
to the book that a teacher could think,
oh, they had just miscued on a word.
Margaret Goldberg is a literacy coach in Oakland, California.
She was hired a few years ago to teach something called LLI.
That stands for Leveled Literacy Intervention.
It's a found to some penel approach to help struggling readers and it teaches queuing.
Around the same time, Margaret went to a training in a program that uses a different approach.
The program is called systematic instruction in phonological awareness, phonics, and sight words,
otherwise known as SIPS.
And so I started teaching some groups with systematic instruction
and some groups I was still doing LLI because I felt like I had been hired to do it.
It was my responsibility to provide the instruction I was hired to give.
She began to notice differences in her two groups of students.
Not just in their abilities to read, but in the way that they approach their reading.
Margaret and a colleague recorded students talking about reading.
This is Margaret's colleague interviewing two first graders.
What makes you good readers?
Here's Mia, who was in Margaret Sips group learning phonics.
Looking at the words and sounding them out.
Mia's friend, Jabria, was taught some phonics.
She was also taught queuing.
What makes Jabria a good reader?
I look like the pictures in the reader.
There is a pre-reading stage where children identify words based on visual features and
context.
A child who knows the words stop on a stop sign, for example.
But the cognitive science shows that teachers need to be moving kids away from relying on
context.
Margaret Goldberg didn't know this yet.
What she knew is that the kids in her Sips phonics group were being taught that when you get to a word you don't know, you sound it out.
The kids in her L.L.I. group were being taught that when you get to a word you don't know, you have lots of strategies.
You can sound it out. You can also check the first letter, look at the picture, think of a word that makes sense and make a good guess.
It was clear to Margaret after just a few months that her Sips phonic students were doing better.
So she stopped teaching LLI and the queuing that goes with it. Just couldn't do it anymore.
And I think one of the things that I still struggle with is a lot of guilt.
It was a few months that I did it and I did lasting damage to these kids.
So the kids that I had in LLI, it was so hard to ever get them to stop looking at a picture
to guess what a word would be.
It was so hard to ever get them to slow down and sound a word out because they had had
this experience of reading as being easy.
They had had the experience of knowing that you predict what you're going to read before
you read it.
Learning to read is not easy for many kids.
That's one of the big takeaways from decades of scientific research.
We're not born wired to read.
But through connecting the pronunciation and meaning of words with their spelling, we
rewire our brains a bit.
Margaret Goldberg soon discovered this cognitive science research.
She hadn't learned about this research in her teacher preparation or on the job.
Once she knew about it, she wanted her colleagues to know about it too.
For the past two years, Margaret and another literacy coach, Lonnie Mennick, have been
leading a grant-funded pilot project to improve reading achievement in Oakland. Nearly half
the district's third graders are below grade level and reading. Margaret and Lonnie want to raise questions about how kids in Oakland are being taught to read.
They meet every couple of weeks with literacy coaches from the 10 elementary schools in the pilot project.
Today the coaches watch a video.
The power that we are going to learn today, it's called picture power.
Can we say picture power?
Picture power! It's the picture power? Picture power!
It's the video you heard of the kindergarten class.
Lonnie Bendick says the point of watching this video is not to criticize the teacher.
So this teacher meant well, and it seemed like she believed that this was a lesson that
would ensure her students would be on the road towards reading.
Lonnie wants the coaches to consider the beliefs about reading
that would lead to the creation of a lesson like picture power.
The coaches see right away that the lesson was designed
to get kids to use context.
But coach Soraya Sojouz Brooks says until this pilot,
she thought queuing was fine,
as long as kids were also getting some phonics instruction.
At first, I didn't see the problem if you were teaching phonics, right?
And I thought it would be okay.
And then I realized that one negates the other.
In other words, cueing sends the message to kids that they don't need to sound out words.
They can use other strategies instead.
But the scientific research is clear. Developing good sound spelling knowledge is critical
for figuring out unknown words
and eventually storing them in your memory.
Now that the coaches in the Oakland Pilot Project
know about this research, they're in an uncomfortable position.
Their jobs include observing and giving feedback
to teachers who deliver the picture power lesson.
Margaret Goldberg lays out the situation they're in.
This is a district adopted curriculum.
She was teaching a lesson that she was told to teach.
So she hasn't writing something that is telling her that this is the right thing to do.
And that makes this conversation a little bit more tricky because it's not like we're
asking a teacher to stop teaching something that she pulled off of Pinterest.
We're having a conversation about the core curriculum adoptive IR district.
So, what do we do?
No one in the room has a good answer to this question.
What they're doing instead is trying to show the district there's a better way to teach reading.
Schools in the pilot project use grant money to buy new materials that steer clear of the three-cueing idea.
Two charter school networks in Oakland
are working on similar projects to move their schools away from queuing.
Here's what it looks like in one first-grade classroom
at a charter school in Oakland called Achieve Academy.
Here we go. Ready? Isaac, ready?
Three students are sitting at a kidney-shaped table with their teacher, Andrea Ruiz.
It blends good.
I'm going to say the sounds, okay, guys,
I feel blend them together.
Sssk.
In blend.
Sssk.
This lesson is from the Sips phonics program.
You heard Margaret Goldberg talking about earlier.
Explicit phonics is one part of the reading instruction.
There are also vocabulary lessons.
Let's think about what chameleons pray are.
The first graders are now gathered on a rug at the front of the classroom, talking about
a book misreweezed, read out loud to them. One of the words in the book was pray.
P-R-E-Y.
What animals are chameleons pray?
Or we can also ask what animals do chameleons hunt for food.
The kids turn and talk to each other.
A chameleon's prey are a bugs and insects and other chameleons are mice and birds.
Other vocabulary words these first graders have learned are posted on cards around the classroom.
They include wander, persevere, squint, and scrumptious.
The kids aren't expected to be able to read those words yet.
The idea is to build their oral vocabulary so that when they can read those words, they know what the words mean.
This comes straight from the scientific research,
which shows that reading comprehension is the product of two things. First, a child needs
to be able to sound out a word. Second, the child needs to know the meaning of the word
she just sounded out. So, in a first grade classroom that's following the research, you
want to see explicit phonics instruction and also lessons that
build oral vocabulary and background knowledge.
And you want to see kids practicing what they've been taught.
This is Belinda, and she's reading what's known as a decodable book.
Almost all the words in this book contain spelling patterns Belinda has been taught
in her phonics lessons.
There are some common words she's learned to memorize as sight words because they don't
fit the spelling pattern she's been taught.
Words like is and have. The boy you heard is Steven, another first grader. He's Belinda's
reading buddy. His job is to help her if she misses a word or gets stuck, but that
doesn't happen much because Belinda's been taught how to read these words.
Are you looking at the pictures when you read them? No, she says.
Belinda doesn't need the pictures to read the words. Pictures are great to look at and
talk about and they can help a child comprehend the meaning of a story. Context, including
a picture of there is one, helps us understand what we're reading all the time. But if a
child is being taught to use context to identify words, she is being taught to
read the way that poor readers read.
In the long running debates about how to teach reading, the fight has mostly been about
whether to teach phonics.
That fight is pretty much over.
You'd be hard pressed to find a school today that doesn't do some kind of phonics. That fight is pretty much over. You'd be hard pressed to find a school today
that doesn't do some kind of phonics instruction. The question is, what else do schools teach?
The reading instruction you just heard in Oakland is not distinctive because there is phonics
instruction. It's distinctive because there isn't any queuing. The only strategy I teach my kids is, is it a cyborg, is it a sound outward?
This is the first grade teacher you just heard, Andrea Ruiz.
And all of these other strategies are teaching them to guess.
They're not teaching them to look at the entire word.
This is a big change in Andrea's approach to teaching.
When she started, she knew nothing about how kids learned to read.
It was a relief when she came to Oakland and the curriculum spelled out that kids use meaning,
structure, and visual cues to figure out words. Because I came from not having anything,
I was like, oh, there's a way we should teach this. I heard this from other educators.
Cueing was appealing because they didn't know what else to do.
Cueing was appealing because they didn't know what else to do. When I got into the classroom and someone told me to use this practice, I didn't question it.
Stacy Cherny is a former teacher who's now principal of an elementary school in Pennsylvania.
She says many teachers aren't taught what they need to know about the structure of the English language to be able to teach phonics well.
Phonics can be intimidating.
Three queuing isn't.
So a lot of times I think that these practices have popped up because teachers don't have
the background knowledge, and it's, oh yeah, I can do that, that's easy, I'll implement
that.
Another reason queuing holds on is that it seems to work for some children. But researchers estimate there's a percentage of kids, maybe around 40 percent, who will
learn to read no matter how they're taught.
Kids who learn to read with queuing are succeeding in spite of the instruction, not because of
it.
That's according to David Kelpatrick, the author of the book about preventing reading difficulties.
We heard from him in the first part of the program.
We have to get rid of ideas about reading that are faulty because those ideas result in
practices that make it harder for children to learn to read.
Margaret Goldberg, who's working on the pilot project in Oakland, thinks it's time for all
schools to take a close look at everything they use to teach reading.
We should look through the materials and search for evidence of queuing,
and if it's there, don't touch it.
Don't let it get near our kids, don't let it get near our classrooms or teachers.
Margaret wants the Oakland schools to get rid of all instructional materials that include queuing.
I reached out to the superintendent's office to ask about this.
A spokesperson said in a statement
that there's not yet enough evidence
from the pilot project to make curriculum changes
for the entire district.
The district remains committed to the curriculum materials
it has invested in.
Oakland's situation is no different
from many other school districts across the country
that have invested millions of dollars
in materials that include three queuing.
Here's Margaret Goldberg again.
It feels like everyone's trusting somebody else
to have done their due diligence.
So classroom teachers are trusting that the materials
that they're being handed will work.
And the people who purchased the materials
are trusting if they were on the market that they will work.
So we're all trusting, and it's a system that is broken.
I wanted to talk to the authors of the curriculum materials
I mentioned in this story.
Lucy Cawkins, Irene Fountis, and Gaysu Penel.
I wanted to know what they make of the cognitive science,
and I wanted to give them a chance
to explain the ideas behind their work. I wrote to them what they make of the cognitive science, and I wanted to give them a chance to explain the ideas behind their work.
I wrote to them and asked for interviews,
but they all declined.
Their publisher, Heinemann, said in a statement
that every product Heinemann sells
is informed by extensive research.
I also asked for an interview with Ken Goodman,
the education professor who laid out the three queuing theory
in that guessing game paper more than 50 years ago.
He said yes.
Hi.
Hi.
I visited Ken and his wife Yeta at their home in Tucson, Arizona.
Nice to meet you.
Ken is 91, but he's still working, just finished a new edition of one of his books.
I wanted to know what he makes of the cognitive science research.
He told me he thinks cognitive scientists focus too much on word recognition. The word recognition is a preoccupation. I don't teach word recognition. I teach people to make sense of
language and learning the words is incidental to that. He brings up the example of a child who comes to the word horse and says pony instead.
Well, that is a beautiful example of the fact that it's not the word that's important.
It's the meaning.
He says the child will still understand the meaning of the story, because horse and pony are the same concept.
I press him on this. I just want to stop you on pony and horse because they're not exactly the same thing,
though. A pony isn't quite the same thing as a horse.
But in a kid's story, they could be.
That's yet a good man.
But don't you want to make sure that a child is there learning how to read, understands
pony, that that says pony and something else says horse.
The purpose is not to learn words, the purpose is to make sense.
As far as I know, no one in the scientific community disputes the idea that the purpose
of reading is to make sense of text.
The question is, how does a little kid get there?
I ended up talking with the Goodman's for nearly four hours, and we could never quite
agree on the terms of the debate.
They rejected the idea that you can make a distinction between skilled readers and unskilled
readers.
They don't like the value judgment that implies.
They said dyslexia does not exist, despite lots of evidence that it does.
And they said the three-queuing theory is based on years of observational
research. In their view, three-queuing isn't invalid. It's drawn from a different kind of evidence
than what scientists collect in their labs. In case you missed it, he said, my science is different.
The idea that there are different kinds of evidence that lead to different conclusions about how reading works is one reason people continue to disagree about how children should be taught to read. It's important for educators to understand that three queuing is based on theory and observational research.
It's not based on controlled scientific experiments
or FMRI brain scanning.
The cognitive science does not provide all the answers
about how to teach children to read.
But on the question of how skilled readers read words,
scientists have amassed a huge body of evidence.
Many educators remain unaware of this evidence.
Are you ready? Okay, remember we're playing for the guessing months to sticker.
We're back at the tutoring center with first-grader Claire Woodworth.
Guessing words is such a problem among the kids who come here for reading help
that they earn stickers if they sound out all the words they don't know. Polly flew over the houses.
She...
R-A.
Claire's mom Molly came to this same tutoring center when she was in high school and couldn't
get through the ACT.
She learned some basic things about how to sound out words, and she was able to raise her score enough to be eligible for a college honors program.
Molly says she's still not a very good reader.
She tears up when she talks about it.
She's grateful her daughter Claire is learning a different way.
What's in your pocket?
Cousin, Marissa, Jessica?
Claire earned her sticker today.
She takes it out of her pocket and reads what it says.
Clare's sticker says she slayed the guessing monster.
I slayed the guessing monsters.
You've been listening to an APM reports documentary at a loss for words. It was produced by me Emily Hanford and edited by Catherine Winter.
Research and production assistance from John Hernandez.
Our associate producer is Alex Baumhart.
Web editors are Dave Mann and Andy Cruz.
The final mix was by Chris Juleen and Craig Thorsten,
fact-checking by Betsy Towner Levine.
The APM reports team includes Sasha Aslanian, Shelley Langford, Executive Editor Steven
Smith, and Editor-in-Chief Chris Warlington.
If you go to our website, apmreports.org, you can find an annotated version of this story
with links to research and further reading.
You can also find our podcast, Educate, where we have more documentaries about how children
learn to read.
Support for APM reports comes from the Spencer Foundation and Lumina Foundation.
This is APM American Public Media.
You've been listening to, at a loss for words from 2019.
This is Emily again.
We'll have a bonus episode of Sold a Story coming soon, so keep this podcast in your
feeds.
If you want to find out more about the Sold a Story podcast, and all of our reporting
on reading, you can go to our website, it's soldaastory.org.
In 2012, a new charity bursts onto the scene. It's called Believe in Magic,
and it grants wishes to seriously ill children.
It has the support of the biggest boy band in the world,
one direction.
It's run by an inspirational 16 year old girl called Megan Barry,
who herself is battling a brain tumor.
I've been in and out of hospital and seen so many other very poor children.
But when questions arise about her story, Megan Barry, who herself is battling a brain tumor. I've been in and out of hospital and seen so many other very poorly children.
But when questions arise about her story, they reveal she could be facing another very different danger.
What is this girl going through?
It wasn't supposed to end like this.
Listen to Believe in Magic, with me, Jamie Bartlett.
me Bartlett.