Stuff You Should Know - How Eyewitness Testimony Works(?)
Episode Date: July 25, 2019Few things are more compelling than a witness pointing out a defendant in the courtroom as the perpetrator. But few things are also more unreliable than eyewitness testimony. Our memories can be prett...y terrible, which matters when you’re facing death row. Learn more about your ad-choices at https://www.iheartpodcastnetwork.comSee omnystudio.com/listener for privacy information.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
On the podcast, Hey Dude, the 90s called,
David Lasher and Christine Taylor,
stars of the cult classic show, Hey Dude,
bring you back to the days of slip dresses
and choker necklaces.
We're gonna use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
but we are going to unpack and dive back
into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it, and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
Listen to Hey Dude, the 90s called
on the iHeart radio app, Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you get your podcasts.
Hey, I'm Lance Bass, host of the new iHeart podcast,
Frosted Tips with Lance Bass.
Do you ever think to yourself, what advice would Lance Bass
and my favorite boy bands give me in this situation?
If you do, you've come to the right place
because I'm here to help.
And a different hot, sexy teen crush boy bander
each week to guide you through life.
Tell everybody, ya everybody, about my new podcast
and make sure to listen so we'll never, ever have to say.
Bye, bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass
on the iHeart radio app, Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you listen to podcasts.
Hey, everybody, it's Josh and Chuck,
and we're coming to see you guys.
Some of you, some cities, just listen up.
That's right, because we just did Chicago and Toronto,
and it went great.
And I think our topic of bleep went really well.
Sure did.
And everyone loved hearing about bleep.
That's right.
So if you're in Boston, you can come see us
on August 29th at the Wilbur.
Portland, Maine, Maine?
At the State Theater on August 30th, I can't wait.
I'm gonna it's Labor Day weekend.
I'm gonna stay the whole weekend.
I'll be all over Maine.
That's great, man.
Where else?
We're gonna be in Orlando on October 9th,
and then on October 10th, we're gonna be in New Orleans, man.
And then later on that month,
we're doing a three-night stand,
the 23rd, 24th, and 25th at the Bell House in Brooklyn.
That's right, 25th is sold out,
but you can still get tickets for the 23rd and 24th,
and we will see you then.
Check it out at sysklive.com.
Welcome to Stuff You Should Know,
a production of iHeartRadio's How Stuff Works.
Hey, and welcome to the podcast.
I'm Josh Clark, and there's Charles W. Chuck Bryant,
and there's Jerry over there.
And this is Stuff You Should Know,
the continuing courtroom drama edition.
Yeah, this one, I think if you take our podcast on memory
and our podcast on police lineups,
and they made love, then they would have this baby.
I just came a little aroused, Chuck.
Can you tell?
Yeah, this is, I guess 2011 was the memory one,
so that's been a while.
Yeah, also you could sprinkle in a little photographic memory.
Maybe that one was just watching.
Sure, okay.
Jerry's back.
Yeah, I know, hey Jerry, how are you?
That's right, good answer.
So, Chuck, have you ever been wrongfully convicted
of a crime based on eyewitness testimony?
Not convicted, but you have been indicted on a crime.
Not indicted.
You have been accused of a crime.
I've been accused of crimes.
Really?
I think you should dish about that.
You know, crimes against humanity.
Okay, I'm gonna take all that as a no, you never have.
That's great, but it turns out there are plenty of people,
hundreds so far in the US alone,
who have been found to have been wrongfully convicted
of crimes, big crimes.
I mean, crimes that have put them on death row
based on eyewitness testimony.
And in the last few decades, it's become really apparent
that eyewitness testimony is really not great.
I mean, we've known it for a long time,
but thanks to DNA evidence coming along,
we can now go back and say, yeah,
this person is innocent, actually.
Yeah, you want to hear something, a little story?
Yes, please.
So we worked with a locations person, actually two people.
It's a couple on some of the stuff
you should know stuff back in the day.
I think some of the shorts.
Sure, I remember them.
And locations, and I'm not going to say their names
or anything to protect them, but they were riding their bikes
and were hit and run last week.
Oh, no.
And she's fine now, but she was in the hospital.
It was not great.
And they have, I've been following this on social media,
they have video from everyone has cameras now, businesses
and homes and stuff.
They have video of the incident.
They have the car's license plate is clear as day.
The car is clear as day.
The police have all the stuff, and the police
are like, nothing we can do about it
unless you have an eyewitness that
can say who the driver was.
What?
It's the same thing that happened to me in LA
when I told that story back in one of our shows.
When I got hit and run, and I couldn't identify this young
woman in a lineup card.
Oh, yeah.
And they're like, sorry, she said she didn't do it.
I'm like, that's all you got to do is say I didn't do it.
But the same thing is happening to them.
They have, you wouldn't believe the clarity of the videos that
show this car hitting them and leaving.
And they're like, nothing we can do about it.
Man, that is crazy.
So that's a pretty good example of the law
being slow to catch up to the current state of, I guess,
the world, basically.
Yeah, I mean, they're working the case,
and they're trying to find out who did it.
But they can't simply go to the person's house who
owns that car and arrest somebody.
I guess in a way, though, that it sounds stupid and dumb.
But at the same time, it is kind of reassuring,
especially with the rise of Deepfakes,
which we've talked about, too.
You can't just fabricate a video, especially convincing one
to be like, go arrest this person.
I guess so.
My thought, though, is like, go bring in the person who
owns the car, and you will probably very likely find out
who was driving it if it wasn't them.
Yeah, especially if you are really generous with the rubber
hose, you know what I mean?
And the D-Lousing.
I've met more of the beating with the rubber hose.
No, I know what you mean.
Oh, OK.
Well, you took it a different direction.
I was just thinking of Rambo and First Blood.
What, did they D-Lous Rambo?
Yeah, they D-Loused him and then hit him with the fire hose.
OK, huh.
But all this to say, eyewitness testimony
is what's needed in many cases to prove guilt.
But it's so unreliable.
It's like a joke, almost.
It's the gold standard in the American justice system.
And I would suspect just about every justice system
that if somebody comes into a court and points at them,
the suspect or the defendant, and says,
I saw them kill that person.
I saw them hit that couple with their car and drive off.
I saw them.
Other people who make up juries will be like, wow.
How are you going to argue with that?
You can't.
This person's swearing under oath that they saw them do it.
They don't strike me as a liar.
They don't seem to have anything to gain from lying about this.
So I'm going to go ahead and believe this person and convict.
But like we've kind of been toying with a little bit
and saying eyewitness testimony isn't great.
You don't have to have some sort of vested interest
in sending someone to prison.
You don't have to be outright lying
to basically send someone to jail who's actually innocent
based on your own testimony.
And while you're doing this, while you're testifying in court,
you might actually fully believe what you're saying,
even though what you're saying is fabricated.
And actually, you don't really recognize the person
that you're saying you saw commit this crime.
Yeah, and a jury is way more likely to convict
if you're like super, super sure.
And you're like, oh, no, that was the person.
I am 100% positive, but as we will learn
as we impact this topic, that confidence in court
is not there from the beginning necessarily.
Yeah, yeah, that's true.
But if you think that confidence sells it,
if you have a cocky witness,
they'll just kill the defendant on the spot.
So like, are you 100% sure?
And they go, what did I just say?
Exactly, that kind of witness will send you
to the electric chair or the lethal injection needle
every day of the week.
They're called, do I stutter, witnesses.
Right, oh my God.
All right, let's get into this.
We've been dancing around it quite a bit.
It's been a beautiful dance, but let's get into it, okay?
Yeah, I guess this 1959 paper kind of says it all.
Physiologist, I'm sorry, a psychologist,
an attorney named Robert Redmount,
said it has been suggested that the presumption
is probably warranted to the effect that a random person
give an accurate original perception.
Well, in the ordinary course of events,
reflect the memory competent to serve
most of the purposes for which it is demanded,
which that's sort of a long way of saying,
eh, memory's good enough, right?
Yeah, basically that the average person walking around
can serve as a reliable eyewitness to a crime.
Basically, and what this 1959 brief is basically saying
is like, this is the state of affairs
in the American justice system
that if you say you saw something
and you say you're pretty sure that what you're saying
or what you think you saw is accurate,
the court system can rely on you enough to convict somebody.
Yeah, but almost to the point where it's like,
can we all just get on the same page here
and agree that we'll just believe someone
when they say they're really sure?
Yeah, I mean, it smacks of that too, for sure.
It definitely does.
Like, I guess the guy was just trying to shore up
any opposition to it.
And I mean, that was 1959, but long before that,
there were chinks in the armor of eyewitness testimony
and just how reliable it was.
So, I mean, people have been using
eyewitness testimony for basically ever, right?
It's probably the oldest type of testimony
that there is in any kind of court
or proceedings or anything like that.
But starting in the early 20th century,
as psychology kind of developed,
one of the first things that psychology took on
was the reliability of memory and eyewitness testimony.
And one of the first people to take it on
was a psychologist named Hugo Munsterberg.
I got the umlaut, correct, thanks, man.
Nice job.
He wrote a book called On the Witness Stand in 1908.
And he's known still today
as the father of applied psychology.
He was a psychologist who said,
hey, here's how psychology can help you
in your day-to-day life, especially if your day-to-day life
is that you're being convicted of a crime
based on eyewitness testimony.
And he basically showed through a lot of experiments
and exercises when he was a lecturer at Harvard
that memory was definitely not essentially
just like a film strip or a videotape
or for today's kids an MP4 file, you know?
That we don't just sit there and record
the events going on around us in the world at all times
and can go back and replay those events in our lives.
And it's an accurate rendering of what we experienced.
That's just not the case.
Well, yeah, and this is with students
where they knew that they were doing memory tests
and quizzes and they knew that they were there to do that
and had to focus on this stuff.
And you really need to concentrate
and remember what I'm about to show you.
Yeah, maybe have a sandwich beforehand.
Yeah, and they were still inaccurate
and really demonstrated what we all now know
is the fact that human memory is very fallible.
Like forget about just happening down the street.
You got a million things on your mind.
You're right in the middle of texting someone
and you look up and you see a crime happen.
Like that has, after reading this stuff,
it seems like very little probability
of you getting that stuff right.
Right, yeah, I saw somewhere that smartphones in general
are they're good in that, you know,
they can help capture video of a crime or a photo of a crime.
But at the same time, they really make
a lot of witnesses unreliable
because everyone is so distracted by their smartphones
that they don't really see what's going on or don't,
you know, they might have otherwise
been a really good witness,
but they were kind of glued to their phone at the time.
I mean, that's true about everything
when it comes to smartphones.
Yes, man.
I wasn't paying attention, I was looking at the phone
or the people that, you know,
if I can complain about concerts again for a moment.
Go ahead, man.
The people that videotape the entire songs
are usually looking at it through their phone
and that's the worst possible way
to experience a live musical moment.
It really is, especially when you consider
that they will probably never go back and watch that video.
Yeah, Justin, my buddy, my tall British friend,
he yells from behind them so you can hear it on the video,
you're never gonna watch it as loud as he can,
which is great.
He's probably right like 80% of the time, I would say.
Yeah, but I would like to see the next day
where some of those people watch it
and they hear Justin in the background screaming,
you're never gonna watch it.
They're probably like, what is that guy talking about?
Or the person watching is like, I showed that guy.
Right.
So Hugo Munsterberg, he wrote this thing
on the witness stand basically saying,
we should not just, you know, take everyone's word for it
when an eyewitness comes forward in a criminal proceeding.
Like there's problems with memory
and I've just demonstrated it.
But his writings were largely overlooked
because during World War I, he was from Germany
but he became a German-American.
He wandered around vocally supporting Germany
during the First World War,
which is not something you wanted to do back then.
No, it's not a good way to get your book out there.
No, so he was basically just ignored for many, many years
even though he was one of the first psychologists
to take up this mantle.
And it wasn't until about the mid-70s
that psychology again took this up.
And there were two psychologists in particular,
a guy named Robert Buckout,
who basically was the first to be like,
memory is not a videotape is one way to put his research.
And then another psychologist,
a very famous psychologist named Elizabeth Loftus
in the later 70s, a few years after Buckout,
was really the one whose work
kind of captured the popular imagination
and made us all realize that we're just total frauds
when we're recalling a memory.
Well, yeah, and with the advent of DNA evidence
and when all it took was a building up of cases
being overturned because of DNA evidence
where eyewitness testimony that was 100% positive
was directly overturned.
You get enough of those mounting up.
And then all of a sudden,
the United States has a problem on their hands
and they have to say,
well, maybe we really need to look into this whole thing
about memory and eyewitness testimony
not being super reliable.
Right, let's take the next 30 years to mull it over basically.
Sure.
So yeah, that was the Innocence Project in particular.
There have been people working to exonerate people
based on faulty evidence,
which really got a punch in the arm
or shot in the arm after DNA evidence like you were saying,
but the Innocence Project in particular was started in 1992
and they've got like, I think 365 exonerations
under their belt.
One for each day of the year.
Exactly, they do it on the daily.
We did a show on that too, so.
Yeah, you remember?
Our world is closing in.
We talked to Paula's on.
That's right.
So I can tell by that sigh,
I think you're ready for a message break.
You wanna take one?
Yeah, I'm just gonna Google some, some Paula's on,
trying to remember who she was.
All right.
We'll be back right after this.
On the podcast, Hey Dude, the 90s called David Lasher
and Christine Taylor, stars of the cult classic show, Hey Dude,
bring you back to the days of slip dresses
and choker necklaces.
We're gonna use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
but we are going to unpack and dive back
into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it, and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
It's a podcast packed with interviews, co-stars,
friends, and nonstop references to the best decade ever.
Do you remember going to Blockbuster?
Do you remember Nintendo 64?
Do you remember getting Frosted Tips?
Was that a cereal?
No, it was hair.
Do you remember AOL Instant Messenger
and the dial-up sound like poltergeist?
So leave a code on your best friend's beeper,
because you'll wanna be there
when the nostalgia starts flowing.
Each episode will rival the feeling
of taking out the cartridge from your Game Boy,
blowing on it and popping it back in
as we take you back to the 90s.
Listen to Hey Dude, the 90s called
on the iHeart radio app, Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you get your podcasts.
Hey, I'm Lance Bass, host of the new iHeart podcast,
Frosted Tips with Lance Bass.
The hardest thing can be knowing who to turn to
when questions arise or times get tough,
or you're at the end of the road.
Ah, okay, I see what you're doing.
Do you ever think to yourself,
what advice would Lance Bass
and my favorite boy bands give me in this situation?
If you do, you've come to the right place,
because I'm here to help.
This, I promise you.
Oh, God.
Seriously, I swear.
And you won't have to send an SOS,
because I'll be there for you.
Oh, man.
And so, my husband, Michael.
Um, hey, that's me.
Yep, we know that, Michael.
And a different hot, sexy teen crush boy bander
each week to guide you through life, step by step.
Oh, not another one.
Kids, relationships, life in general can get messy.
You may be thinking, this is the story of my life.
Just stop now.
If so, tell everybody, yeah, everybody,
about my new podcast and make sure to listen,
so we'll never, ever have to say bye, bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass
on the iHeart Radio app, Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you listen to podcasts.
All right, so, Chuck, we've been talking a lot of smack
about the human memory.
Let's back it up with some facts and figures and stuff, OK?
Yeah, I mean, there have been, like you said,
the past 30 years is when the United States started doing,
I just said, United.
Started doing more and more studies on the human memory
and how accurate it is.
And it has really exposed the flaws and biases.
And it's really not even, I mean, it is memory,
but it's also perception.
Right.
And what we perceive is going on.
And there are a lot of, like, we don't all agree
on what perception even means.
And there are a lot of different theories
about how we can get through life.
And there are a lot of different theories about how
visual perception works.
Yeah, there's, like, a twofold issue with memory.
There's the formation of memory, and then there's the recall.
Right?
So with the formation of memory, it's
like, yeah, if you can't agree on what constitutes reality,
you know, it makes it really tough to perceive reality
in, like, a standard, uniform, objective way.
But you can form a wrong memory.
That's like, that should stop everyone cold in their tracks.
Right, exactly.
And there's basically two ways of looking
at how we perceive reality.
And it is either reality exists in some way
that we don't perceive, and we kind of paint this picture
that we think of as reality, but that's not actually
really reality.
Or reality is reality, but we just kind of perceive it
as a piecemeal in order to save energy, save time, save
storage space, whatever.
But the upshot of both of these, and I really
want to do an entire episode on the nature of reality
someday, OK?
Sure.
But the upshot of these theories on what reality
isn't how we perceive them is that we basically
take what we need from the environment, from whatever
scene we're observing, whatever, and then we kind of fill
in the blanks to create this complete picture.
And in doing so, if we're just kind of walking through a meadow
or something like that, enjoying the day,
that doesn't really matter, right?
We can kind of recall what the butterfly that flew by
looked like, what its colors were, what the trees looked like.
But if we really dig in, did we actually
look at the trees that kind of provided
the backdrop of this scene?
Or is it just kind of a conception of what trees
in general look like in that situation
that our minds filled in?
And when people started thinking about this stuff,
not just psychologists, but neurologists, philosophers,
all these, a lot of different people,
trying to figure out how we go through the life and reality
and perceive the world, it became really apparent
that we do a lot of shorthand construction
as we're kind of moving through life.
And when we're walking through a meadow, not that important.
When we're convicting a person of robbery and murder,
then it does become important.
And it is an issue that we just kind of fill in the blanks
to create a whole picture that didn't necessarily happen.
Yeah, I don't know if anyone listening has ever
seen the hollow mask illusion.
That has to do with Gestalt theory, basically,
that our perceptions are based on perceptual hypotheses.
So that's us making these educated guesses about
the sensory information that our eyeballs and our ear holes.
And we should point out that eyewitness testimony
can mean audio, like something you overheard as well.
I don't know how well other senses have performed in court.
I was thinking about that.
I guess, did you smell a chemical smell
or something like that?
That would be one, but I mean,
I don't know what else you would,
like, did you feel the murderers touch?
Taste, I don't know.
Did you lick the guy who was robbing the gas station?
But if you look at the great example of Gestalt theory
and that perceptual hypotheses is the hollow mask.
So if you go online, there's one very famous one
of Albert Einstein.
And it's basically someone will show you
what looks like a mask of Albert Einstein.
It's face, and then they start to turn it around.
And about halfway through, you realize
that you were looking at the inside of that mask
and not the outside of that mask.
And it's painted, of course, but it's still concave.
So it shouldn't look convex, but yet it does.
And it's a mind trick and it's really freaky.
It is, but it also just kind of goes to show
that our brains leap to conclusions, basically.
Yeah, absolutely.
Another thing is that the whole Darwinian approach
is basically if you're in a dangerous situation,
your brain is gonna quickly decide
what's most important to pay attention to in that scene.
And that will, of course, skew reality
depending on what's going on.
Plus also, so that's point one,
our brains fill in the blanks,
probably more than we realize
to create our idea of reality and memory, right?
Yes.
And even when we're actually actively
taking in information, just how good,
say, like our eyesight is, or our hearing is.
Or how good the lighting is on a street, or...
Exactly.
And that's one thing that defense attorneys,
in particular, will try to attack is things like that.
Like, do you wear glasses or contacts?
Have you ever had Lasik?
Was it raining out?
Was it nighttime?
How far away were you?
That streetlight was under repair.
We have records.
Exactly.
And the whole courtroom goes, oh.
Yeah.
Yeah.
It's the big moment.
And Perry Mason shoots a duck.
Perry Mason farts in court?
I didn't say that.
I said he shoots a duck.
All right.
It was his thing, at least at first in the early episodes.
And then the producers were like,
this isn't going anywhere.
We're gonna drop this as his thing.
Your honor, I object.
Huh?
Right.
That's right.
That was from episode three.
Yep.
Oh, boy.
So there has to be a standard here, though,
when it comes to court and how well people see.
I mean, it can't...
I mean, it is case by case,
in that every case is its own unique thing in court.
But there has to be some sort of standard,
as far as like, how well does somebody
with 2020 vision see, for instance?
Right.
And there's a guy named Jeffrey Loftus,
a researcher from, I believe, is it UW?
Yeah, UW.
And he kind of developed this formula
on 2020 vision over distance,
which basically says at 10 feet,
you might not be able to see eyelashes
on a person's face.
Right.
200 feet, you may not be able to see eyes.
And at 500 feet, you could see a person's head,
but it's just a big blur.
So like, this could be,
and is this the standard that they use in court?
I think he's trying to make it a standard.
And I'm sure he gets called on
as a professional witness and says all this,
but I don't believe it's an actual,
like it's been judged to be like the standard.
Like they don't whip out a chart in court.
No, but I think if you really wanted
to get the point across,
you could do worse than hiring Jeffrey Loftus.
Yeah, and I imagine don't they also do,
do they test these people?
I don't know.
I think if you have a really good defense attorney,
you could probably ask that a witness,
if not go to an optometrist,
at least have their optometrist records subpoenaed.
Or in the dramatic TV or film version,
you see the, your honor,
if I may step to the rear of the courtroom
and you do that move.
Right.
And then, you know, you hold up two fingers
and you say, how many fingers am I holding up?
And then what?
And then Mr. Brady crosses a briefcase
and the guy with the neck brace on turns his head.
What a chump that guy was, he was not committed.
No.
That's so great that you said that
because it was between that
or a Perry Mason joke for me if I was going to swoop in.
Okay, I don't know much Perry Mason.
Did he fart in court?
No, I just totally made that up.
I don't know anything about him either.
No, it was Raymond Burr.
Oh, sure.
Oh, no way.
He was Ironsides or was he both?
He was both, buddy.
But I mean, if there was ever somebody
that looked like he'd fart in court,
it's Raymond Burr, you know?
Even like put together clean shaven Raymond Burr
from Perry Mason.
Yeah, he does look gassy, doesn't he?
A little bit.
So, okay, moving on Chuck,
there's also the problem that researchers have found
that we humans have a finite amount of attention, right?
And if there's a bunch of stuff going on at once
or we have to pay attention to multiple things
in quick succession,
it's been found that there are a lot of problems with that.
That we don't really do real well
with fast-paced stuff coming at us,
especially when we're stressed out
or in a high stress situation.
Yeah, it's like this stuff is really neat.
There's something called attentional blink,
not intentional, attentional.
And that's when, like when you're just looking around
at things anywhere you are,
it feels like one big fluid thing
where you're taking in everything.
But that's not really happening.
When you, you know, if I'm looking at this coffee cup
and then I look up at your face,
there's something called attentional blink,
which is a little blip, less than a second,
where there is, I guess, just an interruption in input.
Yeah, in your attention.
Yeah. You know, you're shifting
from one thing to the other,
and it's not a fluid motion, it's kind of like a hiccup.
But you don't notice this.
No, you don't.
It all blends seamlessly
because your brain is filling in these little gaps.
But during that period, if something really vital happened
in that, say, half of a second span,
you might not notice it.
And because we've already seen that our brains
tend to fill in information
to create a smooth picture of reality,
that could be problematic for the person
who you're saying you saw do something or didn't see.
That's right.
The other thing about attentional blink too,
is that it really kind of points out
that if we are really focused on one thing,
we might miss another,
that our attention is very selective.
Like a smartphone.
Basically, yeah.
Yeah, you know, if you're like into your smartphone,
you're not paying attention to stuff going on around you.
Even if like you're driving,
like if you're driven up next to somebody
and they're driving like 30 miles an hour
under the speed limit, which supposedly is safe,
but you know, they're on their phone
and you honk at them and like flip them off
and like throw a rock at their windshield,
that kind of thing.
And they don't even look up.
Yeah, they don't know you're there.
No, they have no idea.
That's kind of the same thing.
But there's this really amazing video
that I hadn't heard of, but these two magicians I know,
Jared and John, who I hope are working on a podcast
about this kind of stuff.
They pointed it out, did you go see that video
that was linked in this article?
Which one?
The one that was created in 1999 by Daniel Simmons
and Christopher Shabris or Shabri,
where it's the ball passing video.
Oh, yeah, yeah.
Okay, so I don't want to give anything else out about it.
Yeah, totally.
Everybody just go look up 1999 Daniel Simmons
ball passing video and prepare to be amazed.
But it really drives home like what the,
just how focused we can become
at the expense of other information.
That's right.
What else, Chuck?
Well, there's something called
the psychological refractory period or the PRP.
And that's when if two things, if two cognitive tasks,
and this can include you seeing things,
if they arrive really closely together,
there's a bit of a lag time between when we process
these two things, that first thing
and then that second thing.
So if these things are coming in quick succession
or they are very intense
or there are a lot of different stimuli,
there is a little bottleneck,
a processing bottleneck that can occur.
And especially in like a scary experience,
like if someone sticks a gun in your face or something,
the big example that you always hear
is like what was the weapon?
Was it a gun or a knife?
And it's been kind of shown time again,
if you're, if you are,
if someone comes and sticks a gun in your face,
you're gonna have your attention on the gun
and more so than the face.
So you might not be able to recall
what your perpetrator looked like.
You may have more information about the gun,
which is a little helpful, but not as much as their face.
Yeah, you know, I think I told you before that time
that Yumi got mugged, she was not focused on the gun
and did not know that the guy had had a gun on her.
And her friends, when she was asked
if there was a gun at the cop station,
she's like, I actually don't know.
Like she didn't process the gun.
And her friends were like, yeah, there was a gun,
the guy had a gun, which is, I hadn't realized,
like I get that, you know,
not processing something because of a stressful situation,
but it's funny that's like the opposite,
apparently, of how it usually is.
Yeah, and I think you can sort of train yourself.
I mean, hopefully this kind of thing doesn't happen
over and over again to Yumi or anyone else,
but like I've sort of told myself,
like if anything ever happens,
try and keep your wits about you
and take in as much detail as you can
and like repeat it in your brain over and over.
That's just good advice for like daily living.
Sure.
That's mindfulness, I think.
Yeah, that's a good point.
I told you before, I think it was the police lineups one
that she was able to pick the guy out in the lineup.
So maybe she was focused on the guy's face
and was missing the gun rather than the opposite.
Exactly, was that a finger in your pocket?
Right.
Don't say the second part.
So I'm not going to, I'm just gonna leave it up
to the listeners, dirty, dirty listeners minds.
So there's also another one for forming memories
that has kind of confounded researchers for a while.
And it's called the own race bias or cross race effect.
Yeah, we talked about this in police lineups, didn't we?
I feel like we did,
but I think it's worth going over one more time.
Sure.
So it basically is, it says that if you are a witness
and you witness a crime that's carried out by somebody
from another race or ethnic group other than yours,
you're going to have a harder time recognizing that person
than you would if they were a member of your own race
or ethnic group.
And so it seems easy-peasy, well, that you're just a racist
and everybody of another race looks alike to you.
That's not the case.
They found that people who score low on questionnaires
about being prejudiced also are subject
to the cross race effect and that it's across the board
for everybody of any race, they're all equally subjected
or they're equally, what's the word?
Victims of it, I guess.
Mistaken.
Yeah, it's in there somewhere.
Misidentified?
No, they're equally susceptible.
Susceptible, we got there, we got there Chuck.
Yeah, that's true.
And that's really interesting.
Like you could test out as the least prejudice person
on the planet and still misidentify
someone from another race.
Yeah, and they think that different races
have different defining characteristics
and that you as a child and probably well into adulthood
are kind of trained to pick out the identifying characteristics
of people of your own race,
which doesn't necessarily apply to people of other races.
And so people really are bad at distinguishing
different members of different races,
not because they're racist and everybody looks alike,
but because they're looking for the wrong cues,
distinguishing cues.
Yeah, and sometimes people can look like other people.
Sure.
It's in the famous cases section,
I'm gonna go ahead and pick one out of there.
Oh yeah, I know which one.
Very famous case of Ronald Cotton in 1984,
he was identified as the perpetrator of a rape,
sentenced to life in prison.
And I went back and I looked at the person
who eventually was found out to be the,
he was exonerated, Cotton was,
but the real guy, Bobby Poole,
that I saw side by side images,
these guys look a lot alike.
They look a lot alike.
Like their noses are different,
but if you block out their nose,
the lower half of their face and their eyes and forehead
are really, really similar.
And I think that's just a case of really bad luck.
It was really bad luck,
it ultimately pan out to be really good luck,
but I mean, the victim, the eyewitness was the victim,
a woman named Jennifer Thompson.
And during the rape, she did, she took your advice
and like kept her wits about her as much as possible
and took the opportunity to study the guy's face.
But because Poole and Cotton look so much alike,
there was a case of mistaken identity of a witness
who actually, as we'll see, was kind of unsure at first,
but became more and more confident, which is a big problem.
But when Cotton was exonerated, Jennifer Thompson
and Ronald Cotton went on to write a book
about the whole thing together.
Yeah, they're friends, they got to be friends
because she experienced a tremendous amount of guilt
for identifying this man and him serving time
for something he didn't do.
And yeah, they wrote a book together,
which is now, I was like, oh man,
that's tell me that's gonna be a movie soon.
And as of like a couple of months ago,
it was optioned to be a film.
Nice.
It's called, the book's called Picking Cotton.
Oh my.
Yeah, I know.
Picking Cotton, Colin, our memoir
of injustice and redemption.
Semi-Colin.
Oh God.
So yeah, the good luck he had though
was that Bobby Poole and Ronald Cotton
were in the same jail together
and they were frequently mistaken for one another.
That's how much they really looked alike.
And I guess Bobby Poole blabbed to another inmate
that he was the one who had really raped Jennifer Thompson
and that cotton was in there wrongfully
and that word got around.
And then finally, thanks to DNA evidence,
Ronald Cotton was excluded from the crime.
Yeah.
Nice ending to that story.
It is.
That's the only one in the list
that does have a nice ending though.
That's true.
So here's the other thing with eyewitness testimony
or should we take a break?
That's to you, pal.
All right, let's take a break
and I'll tell you about that other thing right after this.
On the podcast, Pay Dude the Nineties called
David Lasher and Christine Taylor
stars of the cult classic show, Hey Dude,
bring you back to the days of slip dresses
and choker necklaces.
We're gonna use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
but we are going to unpack and dive back
into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it, and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
It's a podcast packed with interviews, co-stars, friends,
and non-stop references to the best decade ever.
Do you remember going to Blockbuster?
Do you remember Nintendo 64?
Do you remember getting Frosted Tips?
Was that a cereal?
No, it was hair.
Do you remember AOL Instant Messenger
and the dial-up sound like poltergeist?
So leave a code on your best friend's beeper,
because you'll want to be there
when the nostalgia starts flowing.
Each episode will rival the feeling
of taking out the cartridge from your Game Boy,
blowing on it and popping it back in
as we take you back to the 90s.
Listen to Hey Dude,
the 90s called on the iHeart radio app,
Apple Podcasts, or wherever you get your podcasts.
Hey, I'm Lance Bass,
host of the new iHeart Podcast,
Frosted Tips with Lance Bass.
The hardest thing can be knowing
who to turn to when questions arise
or times get tough,
or you're at the end of the road.
Ah, okay, I see what you're doing.
Do you ever think to yourself,
what advice would Lance Bass
and my favorite boy bands give me
in this situation?
If you do, you've come to the right place,
because I'm here to help.
This, I promise you.
Oh, God.
Seriously, I swear.
And you won't have to send an SOS,
because I'll be there for you.
Oh, man.
And so will my husband, Michael.
Um, hey, that's me.
Yep, we know that, Michael.
And a different hot, sexy,
teen crush boy bander
each week to guide you through life,
step by step.
Oh, not another one.
Kids, relationships,
life in general can get messy.
You may be thinking,
this is the story of my life.
Oh, just stop now.
If so, tell everybody,
ya everybody,
about my new podcast,
and make sure to listen,
so we'll never, ever have to say,
bye, bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass
on the iHeart radio app,
Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you listen to podcasts.
So here's the other thing
about eyewitness testimony.
All right.
Is that, uh,
like you have to do this a bunch of times.
It's not like you identify someone
in a police lineup,
and then you're in court the next day.
Right.
You identify someone in a lineup,
and then you're going to get
grilled by cops after that.
And then you're going to get
talked to by your attorney beforehand.
And you're going to be recalling this
and describing this scene
and this, uh,
who you think is a perpetrator
a lot of times.
And every time this happens,
um,
something can go wrong
with your recall, basically.
Yeah, yeah.
And so, I mean, like we've talked
about this so many times,
but every time you recall a memory,
you are adding to it.
You're, um,
you're adding more information to it.
Right.
And that information can be incorrect
or flawed.
And if we,
if our brains kind of strive
to create as complete a picture
as possible,
if the memory originally is incomplete,
the more we recall it,
the more we're going to round it
out to create this,
this,
this picture.
And since part of the process,
like you're saying of going
through the criminal justice system
as an eyewitnesses to recall
over and over and over again,
by definition,
that process,
um,
leads to contaminated evidence.
In this case,
the evidence of an eyewitnesses,
um, testimony.
Yeah.
And, you know,
not to mention when cops get in there
and they ask leading questions
a lot of times
and even this one example
is really great.
Even swapping out one word.
One.
That you might not think matters
if, if you hear the questions,
did you see the broken headlight
as opposed to
did you see a broken headlight?
That takes on a whole different meaning
because in that first one,
the cop is basically saying
there was a broken headlight
and did you see it?
Not was there one?
Yep.
That's called the misinformation effect
and it can be as innocuous as that.
It can be purposeful.
Like if a cop believes
that the suspect is the one,
um,
cops have been known to
ask leading questions.
And when you have, uh,
an eyewitness
who's kind of
so, so on something,
after a few leading questions
and they're answering,
they can become more and more confident
in their, um,
in their, their memory,
their recall of the event.
And then that coupled with the fact that,
well,
this is the right person,
obviously,
because the cops wouldn't be
prosecuting,
um,
or arresting somebody.
If it wasn't the right person,
that just gives the whole thing
even more confidence.
And studies have found over time,
the more confidence,
um,
or the longer,
um,
more often a memory is recalled,
the more confidence grows associated with it.
And the less accurate it may be.
Right.
Right.
So there's this,
there's like a, um,
a negative correlation
over time
between confidence
and accuracy
of a,
of a memory.
Over time,
that's a big distinction
that we'll get into later.
But the longer it goes on,
so say like a,
from the time a crime occurs
to the time
the court date comes
or the trial starts,
it could be a year
and you,
the eyewitness
might have had to recall this
for half a dozen people,
at least,
not to mention all the friends and family
that you've shared the story with.
And so what a lot of people say is
by the time,
maybe the second,
third,
fourth time you're recalling this,
you're not recalling your original memory
any longer.
You're recalling the story
that was helped
to be fabricated by the cops
and the prosecutors.
And in some part by yourself,
just from telling this,
you're recalling the story.
You're not recalling the actual memory.
And that's a real problem
because that's how people
get wrongly convicted
by eyewitnesses
who go into court
and say,
I'm a hundred percent certain
that that was the person
that I saw commit that crime.
Yeah.
And the thing,
I mean,
if you just think about
in your own lives,
not,
I mean,
forget crimes
and forget courtrooms.
Like just think about stories
that you like great stories
from your life
that you've told
a bunch of different times
about this one time when like,
these become so burned
in your brain
as these great stories
that like I'm always curious to
like I wish I had video
of these stories
as they happened
because it'd be kind of fun
to go back and see this funny story
about when like my friend
and I got shaken down
by the Texas Highway Patrol.
Like I tell the story
all the time.
But it turns out,
I wonder what really happened
that day though.
Right.
And like by the,
by the end of the story,
by the time the story is told,
it's like Chuck Norris
himself is Walker,
Texas Rangers doing the search
or my ghost story in Athens.
Like to me,
I tell that story exactly
as it happened.
But who knows?
Well, yeah,
it's kind of like you're playing
a game of telephone
with yourself over time.
You know,
like stuff just gets kind of muddled
and and again,
normally this doesn't matter,
you know, unless you happen to be
telling a bit of a fish tail
to somebody who can't stand fish
tails and calls you out on it.
It doesn't really matter,
right?
Like it does matter
in a court of law
and the fact that the courts
have continued to pretend
like this,
this isn't an actual implication
of the human memory.
The human memory is actually
infallible and just continued
on with eyewitness testimony
has been a problem in the past.
I'm not sure if we've gotten
that across or not yet.
And consider this too,
that juries,
I mean,
we talked about that confidence
building over time.
By the time you get to that jury
and you are super confident,
that's going to have a huge impact.
Juries are going to be far more
influenced by a confident
witness than someone's like,
hey,
I'm pretty sure,
but you know,
if I'm really being honest
because I'm on the witness stand,
I can't be a hundred percent sure.
Yeah,
that is a rare eyewitness
from what I can tell
that that by the time
the trial comes along,
they have been so prepped
and guided
and have become so confident
that from what I can tell,
it would be really rare
to hear an eyewitness
be like,
I'm not so sure,
maybe they probably wouldn't
make it to the witness stand
because the prosecutor
doesn't want somebody
like that on the stand.
So what you're going to hear
in court is,
yes,
I'm absolutely sure.
And you know,
juries are just normal people.
They're not doing the research
on,
you know,
the possible infallibility
or the possible fallibility
of eyewitness testimony.
So it's up to the the defense
attorneys to kind of poke holes
in this stuff.
And so they will.
But for a long time,
this was really surprising to me.
I had no idea.
Courts wouldn't allow expert testimony
that basically taught jurors
how many problems
there are with human memory
and that eyewitness testimony
is not all it's cracked up to be
and that not only
should you not be wowed
by the confidence of somebody
who comes into court
a year after the crime
is 100% certain,
you should probably discount
that testimony altogether.
Yeah.
And the reason that they weren't allowed
is that they claimed
that was common sense.
Like everybody knows
that our memories aren't great
and eyewitness testimony
probably isn't great.
Whereas it seems to me obvious
that you would want to get
an expert in there
to at least explain this stuff.
Especially in like a capital case.
Yeah.
I mean,
you can still make up your own mind,
but at least know the facts
and the science behind
eyewitness identification.
So you can like take that
into consideration as a juror.
Right.
That's just not the case.
That wasn't it.
But then apparently
they started turning
overturning convictions
because the expert witness
on eyewitness testimony
was disallowed.
Right.
And once that started happening
they started allowing them
in the actual trials.
But that's kind of like
if you have a defense attorney
and you're being tried
for a really important crime
that you could get
some serious time for.
You want that attorney
to bring in a witness
an expert witness
on eyewitness testimony for sure.
Yeah, the Supreme Court
themselves in 1977
ruled 72
that eyewitness testimony
is constitutional.
It does not violate
the 14th Amendment.
Even if it's suggestive
but they said it was subject
to five factors
that just depends.
It's a case by case thing.
But the witness's degree
of attention.
Right.
So you have to determine that.
The opportunity of the witness
to view the criminal
at the time of the crime.
Right.
So I guess just literally
physically were you able
to see this happen, right?
Or smell or hear or lick
the criminal.
The accuracy
of the prior description
of the criminal.
That's a big one
and that still holds up today.
Yeah.
Like if you told the cops
initially that the guy had
a mustache
and you didn't have a mustache
you're going to hear about
that from the defense
during the trial.
And we'll get into that
later on.
But that you know
that sort of virgin description
is that the right word to use?
I love it.
Is the one that really should count.
Sure.
All right.
And then what were the last two
factors of the five?
The level of certainty
demonstrated at the confrontation.
And by confrontation
they mean that that's the thing
that you always see
on like courtroom dramas
where the witness
they say, you know,
do you see the perpetrator
here today?
And the witness says,
yes, that man there.
And then they say,
let the record show
that the witness is pointing
at the defendant.
And then Perry Mason farts.
Right.
That's the particular one
that's under attack today
because they're saying like
how certain does that witness
seem when they confront
the defendant in court?
Right.
And then the last one is
the time between the crime
and the lineup.
Like, you know, was it
if the witness
saw the crime
and then the cops don't catch
the person for, you know,
three months.
Is that too long?
Yeah.
Like, does the witness become
unusable at that point?
And those five,
those were the tests
for constitutionality
of an eyewitnesses testimony.
Yeah.
I think those are five pretty
decent factors to consider.
Yeah, except for the one,
the one about the certainty
demonstrated at confrontation.
And that's the big battle today
because some people are like,
look, man,
if human memory is that fallible,
maybe we should just get rid
of eyewitness testimony all together.
Right.
But now I think is the approach
like, hey, why don't we just treat
it like anything that can be contaminated
from, from like physical evidence?
Like, why don't we just treat this
like physical evidence and say,
you know, it was that again,
that virgin identification
is the one that counts
and everything after that is tainted.
Yeah.
And so everybody on this,
there's a, there's kind of a battle
over just how much confidence
relates to accuracy and memory.
Yeah.
That's the crux.
Right.
But both sides say everything
after that first recall,
whether it's telling the cop
on the scene of the crime,
what you saw,
or whether it's the lineup,
wherever it is,
the first memory test is what it's called.
The first time you do that,
that is the only evidence
that should be admissible.
And everybody can talk
about that evidence
and you can come to court
and describe that evidence.
But every other time
you recall it after that,
it should be considered contaminated evidence
just as much as you would consider
somebody dropping a blood on,
a blood sample as contaminated
or smearing a fingerprint
as contaminated.
Same thing.
That's the big,
the big crux.
Everybody says,
disregard everything after that.
Where they disagree though, Chuck,
is just how much during that first,
that first memory test,
how much confidence is correlated to accuracy.
Right.
And some people say
it's very highly correlated.
Like one guy said that in,
I think 15 different experiments,
they found that the,
that accuracy was 91%,
97% accurate.
Confidence indicated a 97% accuracy.
And other people are like,
that's flim flam.
Don't listen to that guy.
But that's the battle
that's going on right now.
But everybody agrees
that whole courtroom,
that's the man right there,
that that shouldn't hold any water whatsoever.
The problem is that holds the most water
because that's what's done
in front of a jury.
These are human beings, you know,
someone might carry that kind of confidence
in every area of their life.
Whereas someone else
might be very unsure
about everything in their life
and that wouldn't be a time for them to be like,
you know, someone who's not very confident,
it's probably going to have a hard time
being super confident
about something this important.
Sure.
You know.
But you also could imagine
that that person would maybe be more easily coached
than somebody who does have a lot of self confidence.
Coach them up.
Yeah.
Isn't that the phrase?
That's what it says.
The t-shirts say.
Coach them up.
So you want to go over any of these other ones?
I guess we can.
I mean, this is all sort of innocence project stuff.
I mean, there's certainly been plenty of examples
over the years.
I think of the 300 and well, how many?
You said there's 352 now.
365 that I saw.
365 convictions.
At this time, let's just say it was 349
that they had overturned.
70% of them were based on the testimony of an eyewitness
and this is just death row.
Like forget muggings.
Yeah.
I don't know if it's all just death row or not,
but some of these two were not just single witnesses,
multiple eyewitnesses, which if there's one thing
that basically says there was a cop or a prosecutor
who coached everybody to basically share the same story,
that's it.
It's an overturned conviction with multiple eyewitnesses
that DNA evidence shows were all incorrect.
Yeah.
This one right here was especially maddening Jerome White
in 1979 was convicted of rape and robbery
and he was exonerated 12 years later,
but the real guy who did the crime
was in the actual same lineup where White was identified.
Yeah.
So that one's especially tough pill to swallow.
The one that gets me, do you remember Troy Davis?
Oh, yeah.
Like back in 2011, Georgia executed Troy Davis
for the murder of a cop, Mark McPhail.
Savannah, right?
Yeah, Don and Savannah.
And there was no physical evidence and no weapon.
Nothing tied Troy Davis to the crime except for nine eyewitnesses,
seven of which recanted their testimony.
And there was a big deal because a lot of people are like,
it looks like Georgia is going to execute an innocent man.
We need to get this commuted to a life sentence
so we can try to figure this out.
And there was a petition that went around.
I remember signing it.
There was 660,000 signatures on this petition
and it still didn't get his sentence commuted
and Georgia executed what was almost certainly an innocent man
for the murder of Mark McPhail.
Yeah, that was tough.
Which also means that the murderer of Mark McPhail
is still out there somewhere.
Yeah, I think that's not mentioned enough in these cases.
Like it's obviously we should think about the victim
and the second victim, which is the person falsely accused.
And then there's also a murderer out there, maybe.
Yeah, that's another episode I want to do is times
when almost certainly innocent people were executed.
Okay, that'll be a fun one.
That's a good title for the episode.
You've got to put the um in there too.
Well, that's it for eyewitness testimony
unless you have something else.
I got nothing else.
Well, Chuck says nothing else.
I got nothing else.
So that means everybody, it's time for listener mail.
This is a very sweet email.
Hey guys, on Father's Day in 2015,
our son Aaron died from cancer at the age of 40.
One of his last wishes was for his beloved
Australian shepherd dog, Scully,
to live on a family farm with some wonderful friends
he knew from Pennsylvania.
Scully was with us in Southern California at the time.
So I began looking at options to send her back
and it became obvious that driving Scully to Gettysburg
was the only true way to say goodbye
and carry out Aaron's wish.
I announced to the family I was taking her back
and our daughter, who had come home to be by his side
while he was in hospice, quickly said she wanted to come with me.
Without any further delay,
the three of us took off across the country.
After a few hours of listening to the radio,
our daughter, Brandy, said,
do you want to listen to some podcast?
Sure was my response.
What's a podcast?
She plugged in her phone and started an episode of stuff
you should know.
And from that moment on, for the next four days,
we listened to an endless stream of you guys.
I wanted to thank you for helping us cope
with the pain and heartache we were dealing with.
Your banter and fun were very therapeutic
as my daughter and I traveled across country
with our thoughts and Scully.
Being with my daughter and sharing all this time together
with you by our sides was one of the best experiences
of my life given the circumstances.
I now listen to you guys often and my daughter
even bought me a Jerry quote blank t-shirt.
That's a good one.
As a reminder of our time together.
And that is from Doug and Brandy Bell.
Thanks a lot, Doug, and thanks a lot, Brandy.
I like the cut of your jib for suggesting stuff you should know.
Yeah, terrible circumstance,
but I'm glad Scully's on that farm in Pennsylvania.
Thanks, Doug.
Thank you, Brandy.
And on behalf of all of us, our deepest condolences,
we're glad that we could have some small part
in making it a little better for you.
Absolutely.
If you want to get in touch with us like Doug did,
you can go on to stuffyshouldknow.com
and check out our social links.
You can also send us an email to stuffpodcast
at iHeartRadio.com.
Stuff You Should Know is a production
of iHeartRadio's How Stuff Works.
For more podcasts from iHeartRadio,
visit the iHeartRadio app.
A couple podcasts are wherever you listen to your favorite shows.
On the podcast, Hey Dude, the 90s called
David Lasher and Christine Taylor,
stars of the cult classic show Hey Dude,
bring you back to the days of slip dresses
and choker necklaces.
We're going to use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
but we are going to unpack and dive back
into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it, and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
Listen to Hey Dude, the 90s called
on the iHeartRadio app, apple podcasts,
or wherever you get your podcasts.
Bye, bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass
on the iHeartRadio app, apple podcasts,
or wherever you listen to podcasts.