Stuff You Should Know - Mulititasking: Working slower with worse results
Episode Date: March 5, 2024Multitasking isn't really possible, because of the way the human brain works. We know you think you're getting more done, but you're really working slower and with worse results than working sequentia...lly. We promise. See omnystudio.com/listener for privacy information.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Get ready for our 2024 I Heart Podcast Awards presented by the Hartford live at South by Southwest
Celebrating the best of the best
We'll honor the very best in podcasting from the past year and
Celebrate the most innovative talent and creators in the industry podcasts have always reflected our culture watch live Monday
March 11th on I heart radio's YouTube channel and listen on I heart radio stations across America
See all of the nominees now at IHeartPodcastAwards.com.
The Hartford Small Business Insurance is the presenting partner of the 2024 I Heart Podcast
Awards live at South by Southwest.
To learn more or start a quote, visit the Hartford.com slash Small Business.
With insurance designed for your small business, the bucks got your back.
It's Rachel Leviss, host of the new podcast, Rachel Goes Rogue.
This podcast is about choosing my own path
and standing in my power.
I have been involved with one of the biggest scandals
in reality television history.
We're gonna get into all of it.
The good, the bad, the ugly.
I've been keeping secrets for far too long and I just want to come clean.
Listen to Rachel Goes Rogue on the iHeart Radio app, Apple podcasts, or wherever you listen to podcasts.
The second season of El Flow is here. Step into the ever-evolving world of Réadon and get up close with both legendary figures
and emerging talents in the industry.
Part of the enormous significance of Réadon is really the way in which personal narratives
connect to larger things going on historically and socially.
Listen to El Fló on the iHeart radio app, Apple podcasts, or wherever you listen to your
podcasts.
Welcome to Stuff You Should Know, a production of iHeart Radio.
Hey and welcome to the podcast. I'm Josh. There's Chuck and we're doing this on our own, flying high like an eagle.
To the sea?
No, no, no.
Uh, and this is stuff you should know.
No, no, no.
I'm so annoyed at this.
The, the, the episode from 2011 that we somehow recorded in 2024.
It feels that way a little bit, doesn't it?
Yes, it does.
It really does.
I think I was annoyed because, and I put this one together so it's my fault,
but like, I feel like it was just like,
well, this study says this about multitasking,
and this study says this, and this study says this.
I think you feel that way
because that's exactly what it does.
There's just no good story here.
I think that's my problem.
There is, there is.
I think the story is, is that we're gonna like flip
everybody's wig
because it turns out that multitasking is a myth, a fraud.
You shouldn't even try to do it
because not only is multitasking impossible
for you to do puny human,
it actually makes you worse at what you are doing.
Yeah, I think there are people,
plenty of people who think they're great multitaskers.
Yeah.
They may be among the very small, small percentage of people who are super-taskers, which we'll
talk about.
But more than likely, they probably just think they're getting a lot more done by switching
back and forth between a bunch of different things when they're really, really not.
And that's really what we're talking about.
Multitasking isn't even really multitasking.
It's just, it's task switching very fast.
That's a much, much better description of a task switching
because when you're trying to do multiple things at once,
even something as simple as say like baking a cake,
like stirring like the batter while you're talking
on the phone, you're not actually doing those two things
at the same time as far as your brain is concerned.
It's basically flipping back and forth
to make sure you're doing them both adequately.
And we actually can do something as simple as that fairly well,
but the more complex the tasks get
and the more numerous the tasks get,
everything just starts a short circuit
and you sit there and wake up every morning
and wonder why you're still so tired
and stressed out all the time.
It's multitasking.
Yeah, since you said short circuit,
we should mention the origin of the term
is from a computer
ad in 1965, IBM report basically talking about their new IBM System 360 and talking about
a computer's ability to multitask or process tasks at the same time.
And since then, since the 1960s, that really took hold as far as a catchphrase and psychologists have gone wild
with doing studies and experiments to see
what are the limits of the human brain
as far as taking on multiple things at once.
Which again, isn't exactly at once.
Like you can do things like at the same time
like stirring batter and talking on the phone. once. Like you can do things like at the same time, like,
bake or stirring batter and talking on the phone. But I think generally when people are talking about multitasking
today, they think more about, I'm sitting at my computer and
I've got like five different tabs that are sort of doing
different things. And I'm also emailing and I'm researching
this and I'm doing that at the same time.
And somebody's texting me and I'm getting push notifications.
Yeah, it's a thing for sure. That's one reason why it's worth talking about.
Because if it's not actually effective and productive, people should know that because
it's becoming such a huge part of our modern world.
Yeah, for sure. So let's talk about it, eh?
Okay. So sadly, you already hit on the fact of the podcast that multitasking comes from an IBM ad.
But you said that psychologists had gone... I thought that joke would have gone over better.
You said that psychologists had gone wild, like trying to figure out the computational processing power of humans.
And I think that's actually one of the first problems
that IBM had kind of set it up for us to view
the human brain as a computer.
And that's not an exact analogy,
which is why I think we thought we could multitask for a while
because we even thought computers could,
but it wasn't until the advent of multi-core processors
that computers themselves could actually process
more than one thing at the same time.
At the outset, including for the IBM System 360,
the computer with a single CPU
was not actually multitasking.
It was doing the same thing that our brains do.
It was jumping back and forth between tasks really fast to make sure it was doing the same thing that our brains do. It was jumping back and forth between tasks really fast
to make sure it was doing them both adequately.
So when we kind of figured out
that there was a problem with processing,
that there were limits to it,
we established very quickly something
called a processing bottleneck.
That there is, it's documented, humans do not multitask to begin with,
and when we try to multitask, the results are terrible, and it seems to be because there's
like too much stuff is trying to get through too narrow a passageway.
Yeah, and some people think that, you know, this bottleneck happens most severely when you're trying to plan an action
that you're going to do at the same time as you're trying to retrieve something from your
memory bank.
Memory plays a big part in all of this.
What we're doing, like we said, we're task switching.
We're performing things in sequence.
You're switching back and forth very quickly, you think.
But there is something called a switching cost.
So every time that you're, like you and I are researching
or something, and we're like, oh wait, I forgot we needed
to email Jerry about something.
And so I'll stop real quick and email Jerry.
And it seems very seamless.
And then I go back to my work.
But there is a cost to that switch there
where it takes a second or two for your brain
to kind of ramp back up into what you were on previously.
May not seem like a lot,
but when you add that up over a lifetime of work,
there's a lot of inefficiencies going on.
Yeah, for sure.
So that switching cost is just a time lag,
a loss of productivity
when you go from one task to another, right? And especially if we're trying to do two tasks
at the same time, which we can't, but we're switching back and forth over and over again,
that switching cost becomes more and more dramatic. And so those two tasks, neither one of them gets done very well.
And there's some theories about what's going on here.
There's some rival ones.
The first one that was established, I think, as far back as the 60s,
is called the psychological refractory period effect.
And the psychological refractory period paradigm basically says that
when we're presented with two tasks that are presented
in quick succession, the response to task two is delayed, right?
That's documented.
We know that, but here's why.
They're saying that it's because the brain is still processing the response to the first
stimulus, task one.
So response two, the response to task two has to wait.
And so the difference in time between task two,
when we're presented with that task,
and response one, when we complete the first task,
that period is called that refractory period.
And then it's followed by response two.
So there's a gap, there's a chunk of time
where we just can't do anything for task two
because our brains are still working on task one.
And then finally, when we complete task one,
we can start on task two and that time in between,
that's the refractory period.
And that's what accounts for that delay in response time
because our response to task one is typically shorter than our response to task one
is typically shorter than our response to task two.
So task two is completed in a longer amount of time
than task one was because it was given to us
while we were still trying to complete task one.
Right, that's a good way to put it.
Like there's no progress being made
in that downtime, right?
No, not at least on task two, from what I can tell.
Yeah, and I don't even think you're,
I mean, I think guess you're putting to bed task one.
I don't think there's real progress being made at all.
Yeah, that's why some people, so that's a new version.
That's part of the rival interpretation.
They're like, no, this actually seems like
you're not still completing task one
during that refractory period while you're waiting on task two.
There seems to be a blank spot where you're not able
to do anything when you're switching from one to the other.
Oh, okay, I got you.
Yeah, so that's the new interpretation.
Well, what they're talking about though is the brain.
And when you look at the parts of the brain,
the prefrontal cortex is the one that is,
you know, if you're doing something,
you're paying attention to something,
it's your prefrontal cortex that is at work.
And it spans, as we know,
the left and right sides of the brain
and it's coordinating with each other
when you're doing one thing at a time.
If all of a sudden you have two things,
then the left side of your brain is doing something, the right side of the brain is doing something, and it's split
up and that's where that inefficiency really kicks in, right?
Yeah, that's based on a French FMRI study from 2010, back in the era where we used to
call the MRI the wonder machine, you remember that?
That's right.
So this is like prime vintage wonder machine type study where they found that you can still do a couple of tasks
but it does seem like there's a certain amount
of parallel processing the brain can do
when it splits the two lobes, the two frontal lobes up
and says, here you do this and you do this.
We can still kind of do it
but neither one is as good as or as fast as if
we just did one after the other. That's the big joke for multitasking. But if you add a
third one, the brain just goes, hey, why are you just completely goes kaput.
Yeah, like two, one is ideal. Two is possible. And again, we're not talking about super
taskers, which we'll get to. And three is just don't even try.
What are you doing?
What are you even trying to do that for?
Or as the French researcher said, forget about it.
Oh man, that got me.
I thought you were about to hit me with a good French phrase.
No.
I love it.
There was another study conducted at Vanderbilt University.
Go Commodores, right?
Yeah.
Yeah.
They're easy like Sunday morning.
They are.
Oh boy, you're on a roll.
Where they talk about the brain exhibiting what's called a response selection bottleneck.
Another bottleneck, but this one's a little different.
If you're tasked with doing several things at one time
the brain says what's more important and
so it sort of
Chooses for you, but I also saw some other studies that said that
What also might be going on is?
you are
Maybe subconsciously if you're trying to juggle two things the one that you're really paying the most attention to is either the one that gives you the most
pleasure or the one that maybe
Can be completed as like a sub goal
Yes, so that's kind of a different interpretation too
So that's these are kind of rivals to the refractory period paradigm and they're basically saying like our brains actually
Period paradigm and they're basically saying like our brains actually require a moment to switch and like we've kind of established In that moment. We're not actually doing anything
But these two these two different ones that these two kind of interpretations that you just mentioned
They strike me as kind of tomato tomato
Because they're essentially saying the same thing one that, you know, the brain has to decide
which activity is important and that it takes more time.
The other one from David Meyer of University of Michigan
is saying, well, the brain has this thing called
adaptive executive control that says,
this priority is more important than this priority.
So I need to work on this one first.
It's the same thing.
Like I genuinely could not find what the distinction is.
And there's even some bad reporting on some of this stuff
where I saw David Meyer in his study
from University of Michigan was essentially described
to suggest that it's showing like,
oh yeah, we can do multitasking.
We can process in parallel.
And that's not at all correct.
Like he's saying, like you can't do that at all.
I don't know how it got all kind of messed up like that,
but there doesn't seem to be anybody who says,
no, we actually can.
Everyone, every single study is, it shows that we can't.
The difference between the studies
is trying to interpret the results
in different ways to explain why we can't or what difference between the studies is trying to interpret the results in different
ways to explain why we can't or what happens when we try.
All right.
So we're going to multitask now.
Take a break. Get ready for our 2024 I Heart Podcast Awards presented by the Hartford live at South by
Southwest celebrating the best of the best. We'll honor the very best in podcasting from
the past year and celebrate the most innovative talent and creators in the industry. Podcasts have always reflected our culture.
Watch live Monday, March 11th on iHeartRadio's YouTube channel
and listen on iHeartRadio stations across America.
And the winner is?
The winner!
See all of the nominees now at iHeartPodcastAwards.com.
The Hartford Small Business Insurance is the presenting partner of the 2024 iHeart Podcast
Awards live at South by Southwest. To learn more or start a quote, visit
theheartford.com slash small business. With insurance designed for your small
business, the bucks got your back.
Snakes, zombies, public speaking, the list of fears is endless, but the real danger
is in your hand when you're behind the wheel. Distracted driving is what's
really scary and even deadly. Eyes forward, don't drive distracted. Brought to you by NHTSA
and the Ad Council.
Hi, I'm Martha Stewart and we're back with a new season of my podcast. This season will
be even more revealing and more personal with more entrepreneurs, more trailblazers, more
live events, more Martha and more questions from you.
I'm talking to my cosmetic dermatologist, Dr. Dan Belkin, about the secrets behind my
skincare.
Walter Isaacson about the geniuses who change the world.
Angkor Jain about creating a billion-dollar startup.
Dr. Elisa Pressman about the five basic strategies
to help parents raise good humans. Florence Fabrikant about the authenticity in the world of
food writing. Be sure to tune in to season two of the Martha Stewart podcast. Listen and subscribe
to the Martha Stewart podcast on the iHeart Radio app, Apple podcasts,
or wherever you get your podcasts.
All right, so we're back with multitasking.
I mentioned early on that memory recall when you're trying to do something else can really
suffer.
And so we should talk about memory a little bit.
There's a guy named George Miller who was a psychologist at Harvard who basically says
the human brain as far as recall goes is centered around the number seven with a little variation
up and down by two.
So he's done studies where they basically just say, hey, repeat these numbers after me.
And what they found is the average number of numbers that someone can repeat back to
somebody is seven.
And again, it goes up and down a little bit.
Some people are inherently just gonna be much better at that
and maybe can rattle off 15.
But the average came down to seven.
So what people do when they're trying to learn things
or make something memorable is parse things out
and separate them into smaller bits for storage.
Yeah, and so we do that with telephone numbers,
social security numbers, any string of 10 numbers,
we almost always put hyphens or dots
or something together, dates.
It's just almost like an inherent thing that we do,
an intuitive thing, but we do that
to make it easier on our working memory.
Remembering three sets of three numbers is easier
for our brains to keep in our working memory than oneing three sets of three numbers is easier for our brains to keep
in our working memory than one long set of nine numbers. Yeah, I mean, I guess there's no reason
a social security number should have those dashes, right? I think that's exactly why it has dashes.
Yeah, so you can remember. So I can just rattle out 287969473. Oh man, I hope that's not legit.
287-969-473. Oh man, I hope that's not legit.
What?
Like, what could someone do with that?
Are you the life lock guy now?
Of course that's not.
I'm surprised I was able to even do that successfully
without saying my real social security number.
It was really impressive actually.
I just had a bunch of numbers.
So just kind of as a side note,
we've been talking a lot about working memory
and that's a huge part of multitasking
because when you multitask,
you are, you're not, by definition,
not completing one thing before moving on to the other.
You're stopping mid task to move on to something else
which you probably stop mid task
to go back to the first one or go to a third thing.
That's what multitasking is. Like there's no, if you do it sequentially rather than
at the same time concurrently, then you're not multitasking. So it requires working memory, it requires attention,
it requires self-regulation, it requires an ability to keep goals
directed in the back of your head and also to be able to place those goals on pause
while you move on to another goal.
And if you have ADHD,
you have a challenge with every single one of those things,
memory, attention, self-regulation, goal direction.
So it's multitasking is really, really hard for people with ADHD because it
requires a lot of functions that a lot of people with ADHD struggle with. The big difference is
in a lot of, I think I read at least one study, it's like, well, if you have ADHD, you know,
you're just completely distracted all the time. You should be really good at multitasking because
in that what multitasking is, it's like, absolutely not. That's just not distracted all the time. You should be really good at multitasking because isn't that what multitasking is?
It was like absolutely not. That's just not it also. I mean you yes, you're more distracted, but people with ADHD
They they're slightly deficient in a really important ability in multitasking, which is quickly
Diminishing the the size of that switching cost.
So where you go from one task to the other,
if you don't have ADHD, you're a lot better
at picking up that second task.
That gap between switching is shorter with ADHD, it's longer.
And then the other problem with multitasking in ADHD is that
if you become hyper aroused when you multitask and hyper arousal leads to much more elevated
levels of stress in people with ADHD, which makes them even more prone to error and to
frustration and all sorts of stuff that makes multitasking that much harder.
stuff that makes multitasking that much harder. Yeah, and good setup for our eventual,
hopefully coming soonish ADHD episode.
We've already got the stuff kind of culled,
but it's a lot, it's a big episode.
Yeah.
So I know people have been asking for it for a long time.
So anyway, that's coming down the pike.
But one thing that we have roundly seen in study after study
is when you try to multitask or task switch back and forth,
is that your work suffers.
Not only are you taking longer to get things done
than you would if you did it sequentially,
and by sequentially we mean completing a task
and then moving on to the next.
Not only are you taking more time when you think you're not, you think you're actually
being more efficient maybe if you're living a lie, like most of us are, but you're actually
doing less, the work is less good and you're making more mistakes along the way as well.
Yeah.
Did you say that it can take up to double the time or more?
No.
Yeah. When you try to multitask, let's say you're doing your
test with making a paper airplane and then shaving a bunny.
If you try to do those two things at the same time, it will
actually take sometimes twice as long.
When you try to do them at the same time, then it will if you
make the paper airplane and then shave the bunny.
That sounds like- You try to do them at the same time, twice as long, and then you're
probably gonna give the bunny a bad haircut and your paper airplane's gonna have a wonky
wing and it's not gonna fly.
Sorry, shaving the bunny sounds like a very dirty euphemism.
That didn't even occur to me. You're a dirty old man these days, you know that?
Hey, I'm just a regular guy who likes to shave the bunny, you know what I mean?
Oh my God.
Oh God.
See?
It's dirty, right?
Yeah, I feel like I'm blushing right now.
When I put it that way.
So, in addition to it taking longer, didn't you also say that the work suffers as well?
Yeah, they've shown on, in work studies and stuff like that,
that you can have up to a 40% loss in productivity
if you're at your computer at work
and you're checking social media
and you're doing quick email checks
and a quick browse to look up where, you know,
Tom Petty was what high school he went to.
Like all that stuff takes a couple of seconds,
but it compounds throughout the day
and you end up like 40% less efficient.
And those are just things that can happen at work.
Like there are actual real dangers
when you start putting people in automobiles, let's say.
Right.
And Tom Petty went to Gainesville High School
in Gainesville, Florida.
Was it Gainesville High?
Yeah.
Okay.
I knew he grew up there.
I just didn't know if it was, you know,
the actual Gainesville High School.
Yeah, the one.
Or a rival school.
No.
So driving a car is just dangerous enough,
just even if you're doing it right.
But since the advent of smartphones, the ubiquity of
smartphones, people have gotten really, really
careless.
Yeah, dangerous.
Yeah.
I mean, like there's nothing more shocking and angering to me
and driving past somebody who's just staring at their phone.
Like they're not even like, up every once in a while.
They're just staring, watching a video on their phone.
It drives me crazy.
And so I'm like, of course crashes have gone up a million percent.
There's no actual good studies or data on how many crashes
are caused by people distracted by their phones.
The best I could find was a study from 2022.
And it estimated that only two point,
there's only been a 2.7% increase
since the advent of smartphones in crashes.
An extra 3,500 crashes each year.
None of those 3,500 is justified
or something to sneeze at,
but I would have guessed it was way more than that.
And apparently there are more dangerous things
you could do than be distracted by your phone,
some of which we've been doing ever since we started driving
like eating while driving.
Yeah, or writing a letter longhand
or reading the newspaper.
Shaving a bunny?
Shaving.
Ha ha ha ha ha ha. Yeah, of course eating while driving is very dangerous. to reading the newspaper. Shaving a bunny?
Yeah, of course, eating while driving is very dangerous. There was a study in 2021
where 50% of drivers say they eat while they drive. And I think a lot of people
eat and drink while they drive. And they think, well, that's not really that big of a deal.
But the NHTSA says that 65% of near-miss accidents
are due to eating and drinking behind the wheel. And if you're doing that,
if you're eating or drinking behind the wheel,
it increases your chances of being in an accident by 80%.
That's shocking.
That is shocking.
Coffee, they said, is the most dangerous thing
to consume while driving.
And this is one of the favorite things I've read
in a long time.
Other dangerous foods that they listed are soups,
tacos, chili, hamburgers, barbecue,
fried chicken and donuts, especially jelly filled donuts.
Yeah.
Soft drink and chocolate.
But who's eating soup while they drive?
I've not tried to eat soup,
but I am guilty of trying to eat chili while I was driving before.
You move like this is ridiculous.
You can't get chili on road trips any longer.
Right.
Would you stop by Wendy's?
Well, yeah, it was.
Was it?
Yeah.
Oh man, I've had Wendy's chili in so long.
Yeah, this wasn't any time recent, but it was a big moment
in our relationship.
Yeah, I mean, I've had moments where I've been
on a road trip and eating while I'm driving
and been like, oh crap, like I've swerved
into the wrong lane or something.
I'm like, wait, what am I doing?
This is completely, especially if you're like,
you know, if you're trying to mannish up a hamburger
or something like that.
None of that stuff is good to do while you're driving.
No, or like dunk some nuggets in sauce
or something like that.
That's a small target when you're going 80 miles an hour,
you know?
Yeah, if you got a churro and some chocolate sauce,
you're in big trouble.
Some of these foods, they actually qualified
like why they were on the report, like fried chicken.
They said a lot of people lick their fingers afterward.
Just depending on what you've done with your fingers recently,
you do not wanna lick them, especially while driving.
But that's why fried chicken's on there.
Jelly filled donuts or powdered
because you can very easily drip on yourself
and all of a sudden you're looking down
like trying to clean powdered sugar off of you.
I think coffee was on there just because
so many people drink coffee while they're driving
that of course, just proportionately speaking,
it's going to be one of the highest food related
or crash related foods.
You're probably right.
And fried chicken, I'm an expert.
Unless it's like a nugget or a finger,
it's a two-handed affair generally, unless it's like a nugget or a finger. It's a two-handed affair generally,
unless you have like a chicken leg.
But when you're eating something with fried chicken
with bones, it's a two-handed affair.
And you're also, it's not something you just bite
into like a cheeseburger, because there are bones.
It's a little more, it bears more concentration.
Yeah, you got a big old breast or a thigh.
You have to basically palm it if you're eating it one-handed.
And that's not how you're supposed to eat fried chicken.
Oh man.
We should talk about schooling a little bit
because there's obviously been a lot of studies
about multitasking and its effect on learning
and stuff like that.
And there are studies that have found
that it will really affect your academic success.
And if you have high levels of multitasking in class
or like why you're trying to do homework,
it's gonna, just like your job,
it's gonna have serious deleterious effects on your grades.
It's true, but depending on what you do, I thought this was interesting, but also kind of intuitive
as well.
It depends on what you're doing while you're multitasking.
Yeah, of course.
This study found that Facebook and text messaging were related to lower or poorer academic performances
while searching online and emailing
were less related to poor academic performance.
And that makes sense.
Like if you're on Facebook or you're texting,
you're probably not getting to the bottom
of the research paper you're writing.
Right.
Yeah, if you're searching online or you're emailing,
there's a much higher chance that you are
trying to find an answer.
You are engaged in research.
So that makes sense.
And this is a problem when you're talking about multitasking,
like studies, they very easily conflate distractions and multitasking.
Yeah, interruptions.
Yes, we do multitask on a lot of, on a lot of occasions because we are being distracted,
right?
And so we have to like kind of do something while we're still doing the thing we were
meant to be doing.
But they're not exactly the same thing, although they do seem to kind of be related.
We'll just call them cousins, kissing cousins even.
I saw step sibling, but I like cousin better.
Okay.
Yeah, either one works, frankly.
I fall into this trap a lot because not generally
when I'm researching for our show,
I tend to focus pretty well, except for maybe email
because that stupid ding comes through on my computer,
which I need to shut down
And we'll get to actually we can talk about it now one of the things that you very much should do if you have a problem with
with this kind of distraction is is
Getting those notifications turned off on your phone. Yeah, I don't like I did they annoy me
So I've always had my phone ringer off and I get zero notifications
just because it bugs me.
But especially if you have issues trying to focus, like, don't get notified when someone
comments on your Instagram post or whatever.
There's notifications for everything it seems like.
Yeah, no, for sure.
That's like one of the first best steps you can do is start turning off notifications,
turning your ringer off when you're trying to concentrate.
Like just really basic stuff that people are like,
no, thank you.
I'd rather suffer in every way, shape, and form.
Yeah, but where I was going with that is I'm pretty good
when I'm doing this job, but when I just,
when we have one of our non-research or recording days,
and it's sort of, I call them admin days, when we have one of our non-research or recording days, and it's sort
of, I call them admin days, when we're just doing all the other stuff that goes along
with the job, I really bounce around on those days between tasks.
For me, I can pay attention generally while I'm researching.
The difficulty or the challenge is I run into, remember that article from maybe the Atlantic
in 2009 by Nicholas Carr as Google making us stupid?
Oh yeah, yeah.
Where he talks about we don't read deeply any longer,
we're just superficial readers.
I can fall into that a lot.
Like I have to force myself to not just get the information
I'm looking for from an article,
but to ingest it, because there's always more stuff
that ties into other parts of whatever topic
we're researching in that article.
And so rather than just going in,
harvesting what I'm looking for, and then moving on,
or going in, reading until I find something
I need to go look up,
because I don't know what they're talking about.
So I open another tab, and then I just move on down the line
like that.
If I can just take the time to ingest like each article,
I get so much more out of it than I do when I'm just kind
of reading superficially from tab to tab.
How do you read for pleasure?
Is that easier for you?
I forced myself to relearn how to read for pleasure
because I realized like I was reading nothing
but nonfiction and it was always for work all the time.
And so I started reading short horror fiction again
like buying anthologies of horror fiction.
And it's been a great, a huge wonderful change in my life
because I forgot how much I like reading fiction again.
Same here, man.
I'm reading a novel for the first time in a long time.
Wow.
And I used to be nothing, but I used to be NBN.
Oh yeah.
Nothing but novels.
And then went through a she's 12 year period
where it was NBM almost, nothing but memoirs and autobiographies and biographies,
but now I'm reading a novel again and I'm just having so much fun.
I was like, oh man, I used to love this and I'm glad I'm getting back into it.
And I can focus like we have family reading time because you know, we're encouraging Ruby
just to read more for pleasure on our own.
So the whole, it's hard to say like, hey, go read.
And I'm just gonna sit here and scroll on Instagram.
So we have our like our family reading times
where like 30 minutes every night,
we all sit in the same room and read our book together,
which is really good.
That's awesome.
Yeah, it's fun.
Very nice.
I think that's a great place for a break.
We can let everybody just sit there
and think about the wholesomeness
of the scene you just described.
That's right.
Okay, we'll be right back. Get ready for our 2024 I Heart Podcast Awards presented by the Hartford live at South by
Southwest. Celebrating the best of the best. We'll honor the very best in podcasting from
the past year and celebrate the most innovative talent and creators in the industry. Podcasts
have always reflected our culture. Watch live Monday, March 11th on iHeartRadio's YouTube channel and listen on iHeartRadio stations across America. And
the winner is? The winner! See all of the nominees now at iHeartPodcastAwards.com. The
Hartford Small Business Insurance is the presenting partner of the 2024 iHeart Podcast
Awards live at South by Southwest. To learn more or start a quote, visit the Hartford.com
slash Small Business.
With insurance designed for your small business, the bucks got your back.
Snakes, zombies, public speaking, the list of fears is endless. But the real danger is
in your hand when you're behind the wheel. Distracted driving is what's really scary
and even deadly. Eyes forward, don't drive distracted. Brought
to you by NHTSSA and the ad council. And more questions from you. I'm talking to my cosmetic dermatologist, Dr. Dan Belkin, about the secrets behind my
skincare.
Walter Isaacson, about the geniuses who changed the world.
Angkor Jain, about creating a billion-dollar startup.
Dr. Elisa Pressman, about the five basic strategies to help parents raise good humans.
Florence Fabricant about the authenticity in the world of food writing.
Be sure to tune in to season two of the Martha Stewart podcast.
Listen and subscribe to the Martha Stewart podcast on the I Heart Radio app,
Apple podcasts, or wherever you get your podcasts.
So I don't know if you picked up on it yet, everybody, but there are, there's some low
quality studies and even lower quality reporting when it comes to something like multitasking.
It's a bit of a wild west still as far as our understanding of what the brain's doing
and why we can't do it very well.
We've got a general idea.
I think we've kind of gotten that across, but there's enough leeway that people can
come along with stupid, stupid studies and the media will report on it breathlessly and it'll turn out to just
be not really right at all when you dive into the study. Yeah and I think a lot of
those at least in terms of this research I thought came down to well who's
better at this men or women. Right. Is that what you were thinking? Yeah that's
a huge part of it for sure. Yeah, because they've done all
kinds of studies, of course, because, you know, what better time to pour money into research than
to pit men and women versus one another in a challenge in a competition of multitasking and
task switching. And there have been a lot of studies that do show some differences.
A lot of them have been very inconsistent.
A lot of the studies haven't been great.
There have been studies that say that like men are better at this.
There have been studies say that women are better at this.
So I don't even know what to think.
I think the answer is we don't really know yet.
Probably so, yeah.
Yeah, there hasn't been like a really good study or series of studies on it.
I also don't think it matters, but who knows if we're on a quest to understand everything,
including ourselves, and maybe it is worth investigating.
The problem with the studies that have been done so far is they jumped to massive conclusions based on really poor data sometimes.
There was this one that the media was like, this guy did it, proved it, women are better at multitasking than men. And when you read into it, you find that the response time of women was about 69% compared to 77% of men.
So men had a 8% slower response time at a multitasking task.
And then to put the icing on the cake, they were worse at a lost key task.
And get this, this is the lost key task.
You take a blank piece of paper and you
show the researcher how you would go find a key imagining the blank piece of
paper as a field and the keys in there somewhere. It's one of the most objectively
interpreted tests I've ever heard of in my life and men were apparently not as
good at it or drew fewer lines than women did, hence further supporting the idea that women were better at multitasking than men,
because they could find a key in a field better, imaginarily speaking.
Is it like a maze?
No, it's a blank piece of paper.
And you show how you would cross this field
to look everywhere for the key.
I saw an example.
Oh, okay, okay.
What an example for a woman.
It was just a series of lines back and forth,
crossing the entire paper.
Okay. That's what I would do.
For some reason, a man who had completely taken leave
of his senses made a circle design, like a swirl,
starting from the inside out,
and then like missed some corners of the paper.
So clearly women are better at multitasking than men.
Yeah, that is very interesting and so subjective.
You're right, because like, I don't know,
I feel like if you got a hundred people,
there would be a hundred different scribbles.
Yeah, exactly.
And then who are you, the researcher, to be like,
that's a pretty good strategy for finding a key.
You pass.
I have seen studies that was one from Penn Medicine
that found that men on average are better at performing
and learning a single task,
whereas women are better with their memory,
they have a better memory and better social cognition skills
that suggest that they may be like more apt
to be better at multitasking.
So that's another study that you can poke a bunch of holes in.
The study didn't do anything with multitasking? So that's another study that you can poke a bunch of holes in. The study didn't do anything with multitasking.
It was a brain imaging study that looked at the connectome, how the brains of men and
women are connected and found some differences in the directions of the connections.
Men had more connections from front to rear and rear to front.
Women had more connections from left to right hemisphere.
And that's it.
That's what it found.
And for some reason, in the press releases
and what the media picked up,
that was immediately translated and extrapolated
into women being better at memory and social cognition
and men are better at navigating directions.
That's the one I'm talking about. Like you, yeah, yeah.
Like I feel like this is devolved into a like be careful what you read kind of lesson, but that's always a good lesson to include.
Yeah, as far as media goes, you know, we talked a lot about smartphones and stuff like that.
It's sort of a different deal these days because in the old days there would usually be a media,
it's called an exchange of media.
So TV comes along and replaces radio,
something might replace print,
new equipment replaces the old equipment.
But now things are,
technology has moved such that things are being stacked
since the advent of the internet and smartphones and TV and gaming and texting and emailing
like things have stacked upon one another such that they've done studies where they
found in the late 90s about 16% of time, media time, was concurrent or combined basically,
and in two, I've tried to get something a little more
up to date than this.
But just six years later in 2005,
that went up to 26% of media being used together.
And I guarantee you that number is way, way bigger now.
So the most recent I could find was in 2011,
and it was up to 30%.
I mean, think about the difference between
what technology you have in your house in 2024
and what you had in 2011.
Yeah, I remember our old buddy, Luke Ryan.
Remember Luke?
Yeah.
He was working in, he was like an executive
in TV and film and stuff.
And I remember this is very early on
where he talked about the multi-screen experience
and that they were all searching, you know,
how to crack that.
And I was like, what do you mean?
He's like, you know, like if you're looking at your iPad
while you watch TV, and it was so early in that technology
that I was like,
what?
Like, that's so weird.
Right.
And now I just feel like even I fall into the trap,
even when I'm watching things I like of being distracted by my phone.
Yeah.
Which I hate doing.
I really try to make an effort if it's not just some throwaway dumb thing I'm watching
to really like, all right, pretend like I'm in a movie theater and like put that thing away.
Yeah, but it does take effort, doesn't it?
It takes conscious effort.
It does.
So one of the weird things about media multitasking
was what that's called when you have multiple screens
that you're looking at at the same time
getting multiple inputs.
Makes you dumber.
Well, there's no study that shows that necessarily.
There has been a study that it's related to a lower density
of gray matter in the back of your brain,
but that hasn't necessarily been shown to be a bad thing.
And is that like the result of it?
We don't know.
I think the thing that interested me about it is that
there's a group of people who were accidentally discovered
in just a few years back from some University of Utah researchers. interested me about it is that there's a group of people who are accidentally discovered in
just a few years back from some University of Utah researchers that are considered super-taskers
to where they were part of a study of just finding out differences in multitasking and cost-switching, just a general study on multitasking.
And they accidentally discovered this small,
small group of people who there was no effect
on performance when they were doing,
like performing multitasking.
Like they were just as good doing it at the same time
as they would have been doing it sequentially.
And with this one article I read about it,
they were kind of positing like,
you know, why is this such a small population?
I think they found like 2.5% out of 400 people,
just a handful, do the math yourself, because I'm done.
We're super taskers.
And they were wondering if like,
there's such a small population
because it's a newly developing trait among humans.
Like a new benefit.
Yeah, like because we're doing things
like exposing ourselves to so many different inputs
of information at once,
some of us are getting better at it sooner than others.
And that maybe we'll all be super taskers
in 100 or 200 years or something like that.
Yeah, that makes a lot of sense, I think.
I thought so too.
So, you know, to finish up earlier,
we said obviously to turn off those notifications
and things like that, as far as like how to focus more
or how to get a little bit better at this,
try and, you know, turn off the email,
certainly turn off social media and things like that.
If you have to sit down and accomplish like a,
well, not even at the computer.
If you wanna sit down and read a book or do anything
and you're getting notifications that can redirect you,
there is a method that you found called the Pomodoro Technique
created by Francisco Cerrillo. And basically there are these Pomodoro technique, created by Francisco Cerrero.
And basically there are these Pomodoro's timer,
but you can find them online if you want.
They're very kind of clean and simple.
But you decide at a task,
you set your little timer for 25 minutes,
and you work on that task until the little timer goes off.
You record a little X there and take a break for five minutes
and then go back to your work.
And then every time you have four of those,
so 25 times four, however many minutes that is.
Well, plus the breaks too.
Well, yeah, plus the breaks.
Then you take a longer break,
you take a 10 minute break before you go back.
And apparently it's a pretty good technique.
Yeah, so during that time, like all your emails off,
all your notifications are off,
your ringers are off during that 25 minute period,
you are heads down focusing on that task.
Yeah, during the time management stuff,
that's a good way to go.
Totally, but some people are like, no, it cares.
I love being distracted with all that stuff.
Yeah, those are women Totally, but some people they're like no it cares. I love being distracted with all that stuff. Yep
Those those are women because they're better super taskers than men apparently
That sounds like it If you want to know more about multitasking
Then you can go read all about it on the internet and see what you think of the quality of studies that have been produced on it
So far, maybe you'll agree. Maybe you won't that's okay because we're all people
And since I said that baffling thing, it's time for Listener Man.
That's right.
This is pretty delightful email.
Sometimes when we talk about stuff, we hear from actual people that were involved in that
stuff, which is great.
And this one was an actual student of the Spruce Elementary School in San Francisco that was a part of that pioneering program
in the 1960s that we talked about in the Pygmalion show. So, hey guys, I'm a 66-year-old man,
retired elementary music teacher from California, city councilman in
Sareana, Italy, and an alum of Spruce Elementary School in South San Francisco.
My husband and I usually fall asleep to your show and replay the show in the morning to
catch what we missed when the Sandman arrived.
Last night I was on the brink of slumber and I heard Spruce school.
I woke my snoring partner and made him replay the podcast from the beginning and needless
to say I was wide awake the entire show and stayed up late into the night in the morning
hours scouring the internet for Pygmalion in the classroom.
I was flabbergasted to say the least to learn that I was part of that experiment.
It's amazing.
Also surprised to learn Dr. Lenora Jacobson is still with us at 101 years old.
Well.
I entered the Spruce School as a kindergartner in the fall of 1962.
The same year, actually mid-year, when Dr. Lenore Jacobson
became principal. Dr. Jacobson left after my fifth grade year in 68. My head is reeling
at this point, guys. I've ordered the book, first edition coming from the UK. I made a
mental list of my classmates who I'm going to send to your podcast. I remember the testing.
We thought they were fun, actually. I was a bit of a class clown and always one of my teachers pets
My memory has always been sharp and can remember several personal interactions with dr. Jacobson in her office
You boys have given me a wonderful assignment as I trip down memory lane and that is from the city councilman of
is from the city councilman of Siriana, Italy, none other than Bob Chiorsi. Very nice, Bob. I love that email.
He didn't realize what he'd been through until he heard the podcast.
It's just nuts to me. I love it.
Amazing. Great to hear from you, Bob.
Yeah, for sure, Bob.
And if you want to be like Bob and just knock our socks off, give it a shot.
Do it via email, though.
Wrap it up, spank it on the bottom, and send off to StuffPodcast at iHeartRadio.com.
Stuff you should know is a production of iHeartRadio. For more podcasts on my heart radio, visit
the iHeartRadio app. Apple podcasts are wherever you listen to your favorite shows. and celebrate the most innovative talent and creators in the industry. Podcasts have always reflected our culture.
Watch live Monday, March 11th on iHeartRadio's YouTube channel
and listen on iHeartRadio stations across America.
And the winner is...
The winner!
See all of the nominees now at iHeartPodcastAwards.com.
The Hartford Small Business Insurance is the presenting partner of the 2024
iHeart Podcast Awards live at South by Southwest.
To learn more or start a quote, visit theheartford.com
slash Small Business.
With insurance designed for your small business,
the bucks got your back.
Buenas, mis amores.
This is Vico Ortiz, host of Dave MyAualita First each week.
Myself, alongside our resident Abuelita Liliana Montenegro,
es aso yo. Play matchmaker for a group of hopeful romantics alongside our resident Abuelita Liliana Montenegro. Esa soy yo!
Play matchmaker for a group of hopeful romantics
in this fun, flirty, and hilarious game show.
Let's see if Cheesepuss will fly,
or if these singles will be sent back to the dating apps.
Listen to Dave my Abuelita first on the iHeartRadio app,
Apple Podcasts, or wherever you get your podcasts.
It's Rachel Levis, host of the new podcast, Rachel Goes Rogue.
This podcast is about choosing my own path
and standing in my power.
I have been involved with one of the biggest scandals
in reality television history.
We're gonna get into all of it.
The good, the bad, the ugly.
I've been keeping secrets for far too long
and I just
want to come clean. Listen to Rachel Goes Rogue on the iHeart Radio app, Apple podcasts, or wherever
you listen to podcasts.