Stuff You Should Know - SYSK Selects: How the Scientific Method Works
Episode Date: January 25, 2020It evolved over centuries to become the gold standard for conducting scientific inquiry. Yet many people - including some scientists - don't fully understand it. Learn about the basis of how we explor...e our world in this classic episode. Learn more about your ad-choices at https://www.iheartpodcastnetwork.comSee omnystudio.com/listener for privacy information.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
On the podcast, Hey Dude, the 90s called,
David Lasher and Christine Taylor,
stars of the cult classic show, Hey Dude,
bring you back to the days of slip dresses
and choker necklaces.
We're gonna use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
but we are going to unpack and dive back
into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it, and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
Listen to Hey Dude, the 90s called
on the iHeart radio app, Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you get your podcasts.
Hey, I'm Lance Bass, host of the new iHeart podcast,
Frosted Tips with Lance Bass.
Do you ever think to yourself, what advice would Lance Bass
and my favorite boy bands give me in this situation?
If you do, you've come to the right place
because I'm here to help.
And a different hot, sexy teen crush boy bander
each week to guide you through life.
Tell everybody, ya everybody, about my new podcast
and make sure to listen so we'll never, ever have to say.
Bye, bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass
on the iHeart radio app, Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you listen to podcasts.
Hi everyone, happy Saturday.
This is Charles W. Chuck Bryant here.
Hope you slept well, hope you're feeling good
because you're about to listen to
how the scientific method works.
This is from January of 2015.
And boy, this was a good one, I really loved it
because we love science around here
and we love the scientific method and proving stuff out.
So, check it out right now.
Welcome to Step You Should Know,
a production of iHeart Radio's How Stuff Works.
Hey, and welcome to the podcast.
I'm Josh Clark, there's Charles W. Chuck Bryant,
there's Jerry, Step You Should Know.
Why are you grinning?
It's been a while, man.
I know.
It's funny, like those words come pouring out of my mouth
and it's cool.
You wake up in the middle of the night saying that
and Yumi like slugs you in the face.
Right, she's like, go back to sleep.
She has to dry my brow.
Yes, we pre-recorded some for December
as we like to do to take a little time off
at the end of the year and not explain things
for a few weeks in our real lives.
It's nice.
Like people ask me things like...
What happened to that stick of butter?
Yeah, I don't know.
Don't ask.
Don't even ask me.
I could tell you, but I'm not gonna.
Exactly.
That's how it goes in my house.
Find your own butter.
All right.
December was find your own butter month.
Yeah.
That's a good one.
That should be a T-shirt for stuff you should know,
find your own butter.
Or December is find your own butter month.
Yeah, that's right.
Maybe a stick of butter or some garland on it.
Yeah, I like that.
So it's good to see you again, man.
Good to be back in here.
Yeah, it is nice to be back, isn't it?
As much as the break was great,
I'm happy to be explaining things again.
Well, that's good because if we got in here
and you're like, I can't do this, I can't do it again.
We'd be in trouble.
Yeah.
I'm glad we're all feeling good.
Jerry, you feeling good?
Jerry's got two thumbs up in a big goofy smile.
Wow.
Two of her three thumbs.
She looks like Bob from that male enhancement pill ad.
Oh, is he the guy, the old man that's like super buff?
I would call him old, he was middle aged.
He looked like kind of a Bob Dobbs typey dude.
I think that's kind of who he was modeled after.
You see the guy that's super muscly now?
I'm thinking of someone different, I think.
Are you thinking of Jack LaLaine?
No, no, no, no.
Just there's some ad, there's some old man
that looks like really creepy because from the neck down.
Because he's super buff?
He looks like a 25 year old.
No, remember there was like a male enhancement pill
and I'm making air quotes here.
For erectile dysfunction.
Oh, well, there go the air quotes, but yes.
And it was like in the early 2000s, I think,
maybe late 90s, but I think early 2000s,
and these ads were everywhere and there was Bob
and all these great things happened to him
because he started taking this pill.
I can't remember the name of the pill,
but the company got into a lot of trouble
because it was basically like a subscription service.
And you gave him your credit card
and you got this free trial,
but then they started sending it to you
and it was like next to impossible to cut off service.
Interesting.
They were like, no, we want your maleness to be enhanced.
So you've seen these ads.
Yeah, I was gonna start asking questions,
but why bother?
I will find it on YouTube.
I'll be like, oh, Bob.
Yeah, you will.
You'll go, oh, I won't have to come back in
and record an insert.
The guy that's on the back of all those pill bottles
in my bathroom.
So, Chuck.
Yes.
I don't even remember how we got,
oh yeah, Jerry did that.
That was Jerry's fault,
but you remember we did the Enlightenment episode?
Yeah.
Okay.
We talked a lot about how there's this kind of tug of war
over the human psyche
between rationalism and mysticism, I guess you could put it.
Yeah.
Well, I feel like we're talking today
about the scientific method.
Yeah, great idea, by the way.
Thank you very much.
Kudos.
It's been a long time coming,
because I realized I don't understand it as fully as,
I don't understand science.
I understand the scientific method
because it's pretty cut and dry
and it's beautiful and elegant and simple,
but then you just take this thing
and it came out of the birth of rationalism.
And when you place it into the world and make it function,
there's a lot of implications.
Is it being used properly?
Is it being used responsibly?
Like, are we putting what constitutes faith into that?
You know, like it just raises all this other stuff
and it made me realize like,
I don't understand science as much as I want to.
So researching this, it was awesome.
Yeah, and this is a cool episode I think
because not only are we gonna talk about the scientific
method, but we're gonna talk about just science.
Like, what is science in general?
And some of the rock stars along the way,
who really laid out the path remarkably
in like many, many years ago,
like coming up with these amazing discoveries
that still like hold, you know,
you can like hold their feet to the fire
for a lot of this stuff.
Yeah, because if you come upon a universal truth,
you know, it is what it is.
Like you got to be the person who discovered it
because, you know, you saw it,
you realized it a certain way,
but ultimately it was there already.
Yeah, like Newton.
I mean, we'll talk about all this stuff,
but it's not like now we're like,
oh, Newton, most of what he said was wrong,
but that's understandable because it was a long time ago.
Like his stuff holds up really, really well.
I was wondering if he on his deathbed was just like,
oh man, I contributed so much to humanity.
It's mind boggling.
But I couldn't enhance my malehood.
Well, Bob hadn't come along yet.
So Chuck, let's just quit stalling
and talk about science.
Like what is science?
Well, I hate the old elementary school defined as,
but it's a pretty good place to start here,
to get a base definition of science.
Yeah, old William Harris did a great job with this.
Yes, William Harris did a great job on it.
Yeah, he did.
Science, the intellectual and practical activity
encompassing the structure and behavior
of the physical and natural world
through observation and experimentation.
Boom, end of podcast.
So the first part of that is science is practical.
And it is, you know, they make a good,
he makes, Bill Harris makes a great point in here.
It's not just stuff you do in a lab
and it's not just for scientists.
It is all about being hands-on and active
and it's all about discovery and asking questions about,
I mean, that's how everything is ultimately solved
is by someone looking at something
and having a question about it.
Exactly, and then the scientific method comes in
when you say, and this is how you properly get
to that answer.
Exactly.
And he makes another good point too,
that the idea that there is a method,
a scientific method makes it seem like it's secreted away
among the fraternity of scientists.
And like you said, anybody can use it.
It's just kind of part of being a curious human.
It's not even anyone can use it.
Everyone does use it.
Nice.
You can't even know that you're using it.
Like if you, I mean, one of the examples
that you use later is if like your car overheats.
Right.
When you figure it out why and fix it,
that's the scientific method playing out.
Exactly, based on reasoning.
Yeah, okay.
And deduction and induction.
Right.
Man, there's so much to talk about.
Okay, so let's talk about that definition that you had.
So the first part is that science is,
it's a practical activity.
So science is practical, right?
Yeah.
It's this, the basis of the whole thing is discovery, right?
You see something, you see birds in flight
and you say, where are those birds going?
And if you just went and laid down on the ground
and went to sleep after that,
then you're not carrying out science.
But if you went, I want to find out
where those birds are going and you follow them
and you start taking notes,
that is the basis of science is discovery.
Yeah, and that's the observational part as well.
Sometimes you're using a microscope or a telescope.
Sometimes you're using your eyeballs,
but no matter what your tool is,
you're going to be watching something
and recording what's called data or data,
depending on, I don't know, what kind of person you are.
Yeah.
What do you say?
I think I say both.
I think I say data, yeah.
I don't think I say data, data, I say data.
Data, yeah.
All right, we'll go with data.
You say both?
I feel like it just comes out of my mouth
one way or the other and I don't really think about it.
I think that's like being ambidextrous.
Yeah.
I'm a data data.
Yeah.
So once you are observing this data,
well, there are a couple of kinds.
There's quantitative data, which are numbers,
like your body temperature is 98.6,
although I think that's changed slightly now, isn't it?
Yeah.
Yeah, there used to be like if you were a human being,
your body temperature is 98.6 and then you was like,
no, there's a little more variation than that.
But any kind of just numerical representation
is quantitative, whereas qualitative is behavioral.
Like I'm going to watch that bird eat and poop
for the next week.
Right, or what happens if I, what will the slug do
if I put a bunch of salt on it, you know?
Don't do that.
No, you really should not do that.
No, that's awful.
But the reaction of the slug is gathering qualitative data.
And depending on who you talk to,
there isn't qualitative data in science
that it should all just be quantitative because,
yeah, because quantitative data is reproducible.
Qualitative data is, it's not necessarily reproducible.
You can observe the same phenomenon,
but you're not necessarily controlling it.
Okay, well, I guess I get that,
but I agree with Bill here in that they are both,
they go hand in hand,
and neither one is more important than the other.
You need to have both.
Well, a lot of people do,
and we'll talk more about it later,
because without the idea that qualitative data
is acceptable and scientific,
you don't have the social sciences,
like they don't exist.
Yeah, that's a good point.
You know?
But yes, we have quantitative data and qualitative data.
I agree with you, they're both useful.
Okay.
It is an intellectual pursuit.
So you can make observations on data all day long,
but until you bring reason,
in this case, inductive reasoning,
which is driving a generalization based on your observations,
then it's just data sitting there on a piece of paper,
like it's supposed to lead you somewhere.
Right, exactly.
And so we should talk about inductive
and deductive reasoning.
Depending, again, it's really weird.
One of the things I came across is that there's not
a universal agreement on how science is carried out.
Like I saw some places where there's like,
there's no place for inductive reasoning in science.
Then other places are saying,
well, you have to have science using inductive reasoning.
Everybody seems to agree that deductive reasoning
is the basis of science,
but that you also have to have inductive.
So deductive is basically taking a big, broad generalization
and saying that it applies to something.
Specific, more specific.
Yes, inductive is the opposite,
where you say, I've noticed these different data points
and that means that this broad generalization is true.
You go from specific small observations
to a broad generalization.
And the reason that a lot of people say,
well, inductive reasoning doesn't have any place in science
is because you're saying those birds over there
are all brown, therefore all birds of that type are brown.
Even though I haven't seen every single bird
of that type in the world,
I'm saying that all those birds are brown.
And a lot of people say there's no place for that in science.
Well, if you wanna go out and prove that then,
that's your business, you know?
You can't just say that and be like, and I'm done.
Right, exactly.
I guess you could, but you wouldn't be much of a scientist.
Right, but you can use it to formulate hypotheses, right?
So you can say, I've generated all these data points.
I'm gonna put them together and see
if this broad generalization is true.
So there is a place for inductive reasoning science,
but everybody says deductive reasoning
is the basis of science.
Well, Bill Harris does, he offers a great example
for inductive reasoning with Edwin Hubble
of the Hubble telescope.
He was looking through the Hooker telescope
at the time at California's Mount Wilson.
Is that the one from Rebel without a cause?
No, that's Griffith Park Observatory,
which has been redesigned and is really cool now.
Yeah, I mean, it was kind of cool before,
but it was definitely like sort of the base museum
that Time forgot.
Oh, really?
So they've updated it.
I'll bet that was cool though in its own way.
Yeah, it was neat.
I used to live near there, so it was kind of...
But that's like the famous one, at least in the movies.
Yeah, it's where they have the big knife fight.
Yeah.
And there's this James Dean statue there too.
Oh, I didn't know.
Like a bust.
So, yes, Edwin Hubble, he's at Mount Wilson
and he's looking through the Hooker telescope,
which was the biggest one.
And at the time, everyone said the Milky Way galaxy is it.
That's what we've got going on.
Yeah, did you know this?
Yeah, I knew that.
Because we're talking 1919.
Yeah, not that long ago.
I did not realize this.
And he started looking through this telescope
and said, you know what, these nebula
that everyone says are part of our galaxy
look to me like they're beyond our galaxy.
And not only that, they look like
they're moving away from us.
So he made this, through inductive reasoning,
made this observation that, you know what,
I think there are many, many galaxies out there.
And not only that, I think they are expanding.
Yeah.
And through technological advancement
with telescopes over the years,
scientists, you know, it proved to be true.
Yeah, pretty cool.
So this is a really good example of him saying,
like I've made some observations
and now I'm going to say this broad generalization, right?
So these galaxies appear to be moving away from another.
So the whole universe is expanding, right?
That's inductive reasoning.
Yeah, it's a pretty brave thing, especially back then,
because you're really putting your reputation at stake.
It really is, you know?
So what Hubble did was what we've come to see as science.
He made some observations.
He came up with a hypothesis.
And then it was tested later on.
It's not, you don't necessarily, as a scientist,
you're a part of a larger collective of scientists, right?
And every scientist needs one another.
It's why there's journals and conferences
and things like that to share information, right?
And to party.
Right, and to party.
And Hubble came up with his own observations.
And rather than just experimenting,
experimenting, experimenting himself,
which I'm sure he continued to do,
he created this basis of work that he probably realized
is going to survive him, right?
And then later on, scientists came down the road
and they tested his hypothesis.
And they found it was correct.
And so his hypothesis became a theory.
It eventually became part of the basis
of the Big Bang Theory that the universe
started as a huge explosion.
And it's expanding still
because it exploded at one point, right?
And they did that by carrying out other tests.
Or experiments.
Exactly.
So this is how science works.
Like some guy back in 1919
makes some observations in California.
In 1925, he proposes this big broad generalization
and over the next like ensuing half a century,
more and more scientists all around the world
start testing his hypothesis and find it to be true
so it becomes a theory.
Yeah, well, let's finish up here with science.
The last part of the definition is that it's systematic
and it's methodical and it requires testing and experiments
and it requires those experiments and tests
to be repeated and verified.
And it's a system, it's a way of working things out.
It's a way of working.
And that is the scientific method basically.
You have your idea, you pose a question,
you theorize or you put a hypothesis out there
and then you go about trying to either prove it
or disprove it.
Yeah, exactly.
And then the way that you go about proving
or disproving it, that's the scientific method.
Everything else is just scientific inquiry.
The way you go about the standardized way
of going about scientific inquiry is the scientific method.
And we, friend, we'll talk about the scientific method
right after this.
On the podcast, Hey Dude, the 90s called
David Lasher and Christine Taylor,
stars of the cult classic show, Hey Dude,
bring you back to the days of slip dresses
and choker necklaces.
We're gonna use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
we are going to unpack and dive back
into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
It's a podcast packed with interviews,
co-stars, friends, and non-stop references
to the best decade ever.
Do you remember going to Blockbuster?
Do you remember Nintendo 64?
Do you remember getting Frosted Tips?
Was that a cereal?
No, it was hair.
Do you remember AOL Instant Messenger
and the dial-up sound like poltergeist?
So leave a code on your best friend's beeper
because you'll want to be there when the nostalgia
starts flowing.
Each episode will rival the feeling
of taking out the cartridge from your Game Boy,
blowing on it and popping it back in
as we take you back to the 90s.
Listen to Hey Dude, the 90s called
on the iHeart radio app, Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you get your podcasts.
Hey, I'm Lance Bass, host of the new iHeart podcast,
Frosted Tips with Lance Bass.
The hardest thing can be knowing who to turn to
when questions arise or times get tough
or you're at the end of the road.
Ah, okay, I see what you're doing.
Do you ever think to yourself,
what advice would Lance Bass and my favorite boy bands
give me in this situation?
If you do, you've come to the right place
because I'm here to help.
This, I promise you.
Oh, God.
Seriously, I swear.
And you won't have to send an SOS
because I'll be there for you.
Oh, man.
And so will my husband, Michael.
Um, hey, that's me.
Yep, we know that, Michael.
And a different hot, sexy teen crush boy bander
each week to guide you through life step by step.
Oh, not another one.
Kids, relationships, life in general can get messy.
You may be thinking, this is the story of my life.
Just stop now.
If so, tell everybody, yeah, everybody
about my new podcast and make sure to listen
so we'll never, ever have to say bye, bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass
on the I Heart Radio app, Apple podcast,
or wherever you listen to podcasts.
All right, you brought up a point I think we should go ahead
and just get right to, my friend.
Let's do it.
Hypotheses and theories.
That's tough to say together.
No, you did it.
One thing that really chafes my hide
is when you hear poo pooers of whatever scientific theory say,
well, it's just a theory.
And where was this thing that you found that poo pooed that?
Do you remember what website that was?
No.
No, although I do want to give a shout out now
that you mentioned it to Explorables.
It's like an online university, basically, of free courses.
And there is one on scientific reasoning
that is just amazing.
It's like a huge rabbit hole you go down,
and you start clicking on the embedded links,
and you end up understanding all sorts of stuff.
So go check that one out.
If you like understanding stuff.
Right.
So that's one of the things that bug me if someone says
it's just a theory.
And this does a great job of throwing that out the window
because it's basically mixing up the two definitions of theory.
Yeah, there's a colloquial definition
that people use every day that doesn't really
have much to do with the scientific use of it.
I got a theory that Jerry in a one-hour bathroom
breaks every day is really playing words with friends
in the lobby.
I think your theory is correct.
So that's a theory in the colloquial meaning.
Right.
As far as science goes, a theory is not just something
you postulate.
Say, this may or may not be true.
A theory is beyond the hypothesis,
and it's something that is strongly supported
in many different ways.
There's all kinds of evidence to support something that
eventually becomes a theory.
Right.
So your theory about Jerry's bathroom breaks,
in the scientific world would be a hypothesis.
What?
Fact?
Well, it'd be a scientific law.
But it ultimately would begin as a hypothesis, a hunch,
based on intuition, based on the data you've collected,
observations, that kind of stuff,
where you've seen that Jerry goes to the bathroom
for an hour to stretch.
Frequently, when she comes back, she's
finishing up a game of words with friends.
You've heard that she's been spotted in the lobby
during these times.
So your hypothesis is that while she
is gone for these hour-long bathroom breaks,
she's actually down the lobby playing words with friends.
Right?
Yeah, based on knowledge, observation, and logic.
Right.
So let's say that you decided to set up an experiment,
and you experimented, and you went,
and you found Jerry playing words with friends five
different times, and you told me about it.
Right.
And I was like, I'm going to run that same experiment exactly
the way you did.
Yeah.
Right?
I would test that same hypothesis.
If I found the same results to be true,
then what you would have come up with, your hypothesis,
would move to basically a theory that
is this widely accepted thing, this explanation,
that Jerry is not actually in the bathroom.
She's downstairs playing with friends.
It'd be the Jerry bathroom break theory.
That's right.
And then if it turns out that you find that Jerry spending
an hour a day pretending to be in the bathroom,
but actually being downstairs playing words with friends,
if the universe couldn't exist without her doing that every day,
you would have a scientific law.
That's right.
Yeah.
I think that was a good example you came up with.
That's a great example, as it turns out.
I guess the point here is when you hear someone say,
in an argument, well, that's just a theory,
just punch them in the head and then tell them
what we just said about the bathroom breaks.
And they'll say, who's Jerry?
Or just queue up that whole bit and stand outside
of their window wearing a trench coat
and holding a boombox over your head
with the smug look on your face.
All right, so should we go back in the old wayback machine
a little bit and just talk a little bit about how
the scientific method came to be?
Yes.
Man, this thing, where are you running this on these days?
What do you mean?
It's a straight kerosene.
The fumes in here are killing me.
Sorry about that.
I'm trying to go green, you know?
Kerosene is not green.
Diesel, maybe?
I'm choking.
Biodiesel, how about that?
OK.
The wayback machine will run French fry grease.
That would be fine.
All right, I'll get to work on that.
I could handle this fumes.
So you tease this with the Renaissance,
and the reason the Renaissance was so awesome and necessary
was because of something else we've talked about,
which was the Dark Ages, when?
Which, remember, that's a rationalist's disparaging term
for this era.
That's right.
But I think sort of rightfully so.
Because right before the Dark Ages,
until about a century after, there
was not much advancement at all in the realm
of scientific advancement.
No, it's true.
That's hard to argue with that.
And the reason why is, again, science wasn't really born yet.
And there is a huge struggle between rationalism and mysticism.
And ultimately, we're living in the age of rationalism now.
Yeah, and we should point out, too,
that this was mainly in Europe, over in the Islamic world,
as I think we had a listener mail point out,
there were a lot of advancements being made,
just sort of flying under the European radar at the time,
because some say the Catholic Church kind of
kept science under its thumb for a while.
Yeah, well, it was a pretty big threat.
Said, you can't do this stuff.
You can't experiment like this.
And don't ask these questions, because here are your answers.
But eventually, the Renaissance came about in the 12th century.
And people woke up and saw some of the work in the Islamic world
and said, you know what, maybe let's
start reading up on Aristotle and Ptolemy and Euclid once again.
Yeah, they're like, we forgot about these guys.
Yeah, I mean, it literally kind of vanished for a while.
It did, from the West.
Yes.
Fortunately, it was still around in its home places.
But yes, in the West, they were lost.
The Roman stuff was almost entirely lost,
because it was being suppressed by the locals.
And I think the Greek knowledge was completely vanished.
Yes, somehow they got, we got another listener mail
after the Enlightenment one.
They said that it was an Islamic scholar, who
was the one who translated Aristotle into Latin
or something like that.
And that without this guy, the West
wouldn't have had much to start with.
Because that's where that birth of rationalism
came from, was this rediscovery of Greek and Roman
classical thought.
And this was the basis of scientific inquiry
of rationalism of saying, OK, there's set rules to things.
And we need to discover these rules
and how the principles of how the universe works.
Like, there has to be principles.
And we need to find this in a rational, methodical way.
And right out of the gate, Europe said, oh, OK.
Well, whatever you say is right then, Aristotle.
We're used to just believing everything without questioning it.
And luckily, Albert Magnus, I think, is who it was.
Albertus.
Was it Albertus Magnus or Roger Bacon who said, no, it was Bacon.
Roger Bacon, who just has this great name, Raj Bacon.
The Bacon brothers?
Yeah.
Francis and Roger?
Right.
Well, they weren't brothers, though.
But were they related at all?
You know, I look that up, and I don't
think people know either way.
I don't think there's any proof, but a lot of people
think because of their names and the way things went back then
that they may very well have been related.
And I mean, they were separated by 300 or so years.
Although Roger was a monk, so he would not
have had children.
So if they were related, it wasn't necessarily
through his line.
Gotcha.
Yeah, it could have been a nephew or something.
Yeah, or his brother Kevin might have had the line that matched.
So Roger was the one who said, everybody stop.
Just because Aristotle wrote something doesn't mean
it's fact, especially when we find contradictions to it.
That doesn't, Aristotle's not automatically right.
And this is a huge advancement.
Yeah, and Albertus Magnus was the one,
I believe, who said this thing called revealed truth, which
is basically God says this instead of a truth found
by experimenting is maybe we should experiment instead
and not take this revealed truth as the truth.
Right, and we mentioned in the Enlightenment episode
as well about scholasticism, about using scientific inquiry
to explain theology, which was, you know,
you're still working from a theological standpoint,
but you're starting to use scientific inquiry.
And the idea that you shouldn't just accept things as truth,
that was, again, a huge breakthrough.
Yeah.
Francis Bacon, the other Bacon brother.
He's one of the heroes of the story.
Yeah, he was an attorney and philosopher.
And possibly Shakespeare.
Oh, really?
I never heard that.
Oh, yeah.
Interesting.
So what do you mean?
Like wrote those under the pseudonym?
Yeah.
And the Shakespeare sister was the other theory, too, right?
That it was a woman.
I've heard that, yeah.
And she couldn't, like, women couldn't be the playwright.
So her dumb brother, William, took credit.
That's a good, was it her brother?
I think that was one of the theories.
It was a good Smith song, too.
Shakespeare sister, was that the name of it?
Yeah.
Wouldn't it a band, too?
I think it would.
Was it?
Maybe.
So anyway, he was a philosopher and a lawyer.
And he said, you know what, the Baconian method basically
became the scientific method.
He was the first dude who really said,
this is how the steps that you should take to investigate
science.
Right.
There has to be a framework.
And the whole point of this, that we take this so for granted
now, because it's so intuitively and on its face, right,
as far as scientific inquiry goes.
But this is an enormous breakthrough to say,
follow this step, these steps, this framework.
And if everybody who carries out science
follows the same framework, then science
will be universal and interchangeable.
And anyone in the world, and not just now, but any time,
will be able to carry out the same experiment
and will be able to verify or disprove it.
And that is amazing that that happened.
That's why Francis Bacon is one of the heroes of the story.
And he didn't come up with this entirely on his own,
but he was the one who said, this is what we're going to do.
I'm going to give it a name.
I'm going to spell it out.
And from now on, you can call me the dad
of the scientific method.
Yeah, and that's why Newton was such a rock star,
because he's so rigorously stuck to the scientific method
that all these centuries later, his systems of laws
are they have stood the test of time.
And I think it's a good point to bring up
to that the collaboration of scientists
is really the hallmark of advancement and moving forward.
It's not working in a vacuum.
It's sharing your ideas and working with one another.
And the whole little sidebar here on cell theory,
I thought was pretty cool, which was when science quit,
or not quit, but started looking at small things instead
of looking at the universe around them and at the stars.
And said, basically, through the advancement of lens
grinding, Antonio van Leeuwenhoek, specifically,
a Dutch tradesman, was pretty good at making
simple microscopes.
And all of a sudden, contemporaries like Robert Hooke said,
you know what, let's start looking at tiny things,
because therein might lie the answer to many, many things.
Yeah, and they're right.
Robert Hooke found cork, or he discovered cells
by looking at cork through an early microscope.
So in this story, science is hastened by technological
advancement, lens grinding, to make microscopes.
And then this new technology is used to further science, right?
Yeah, it's like mutual inspiration between Leeuwenhoek
and Hooke.
Leeuwenhoek.
Yeah, it was neat.
Because Hooke heard about Leeuwenhoek's microscopes,
got his hands on one, or a microscope, looked at them,
the cork, and said, oh, there's such a thing as cells.
Leeuwenhoek said, oh, that's pretty neat.
Let me try.
And he said, oh, there's such a thing as, quote,
little animals, which we call protoinbacteria.
And one of the royal societies, after Leeuwenhoek presented
his findings, turned back to Hooke,
and said, hey, Hooke, we know you're
pretty handy with the microscope.
Can you confirm Leeuwenhoek's findings?
Are there little animals?
Hooke said, there are indeed.
I can see them with my microscope.
That's right, and that inspired a German botanist name,
Matthias Schleiden, to look at a lot of plants.
And he was the first guy to say, you know what?
Plants are composed of cells.
And he was having dinner one night
with his zoologist buddy.
Yeah, and this is about 100 years later.
Yeah, Theodore Schwan, and said, you know what, dude?
Order the wine, and order the steak.
Trust me, because this place is fantastic.
And also, plants are made of cells.
Don't tell anyone.
And he went, you know what, dude?
I have been investigating animals with microscopes,
and they're made of cells, too.
And so they figured out at this dinner
that everything is made of cells.
All living things are made of cells.
Boom.
OK, so this is huge.
This is a big advancement that we're hitting upon right now.
Huge.
But it laid the further foundation, right?
So initial scientific inquiry led
to further scientific inquiry, and further scientific
conclusions and generalizations, all living things
are made of cells.
And then it was extrapolated elsewhere, right?
Yeah, like 20 years later, Rudolph Virchow said,
you know what, not only is everything made of living
cells, but they all come from pre-existing cells, which
was a huge deal at the time, because people believed
in spontaneous generation at the time.
Like if you left some wheat seed in a sweaty shirt,
it would spawn mice, I think, was one of them.
Gross.
There's a lot of weird ones.
Press basil between some bricks,
and you'll get a scorpion was one.
Like they were really out there.
Yeah, well, the one that is, well, not true,
but the one that you could actually see was rotten meat
would eventually spawn maggots.
Right.
How did they possibly get there?
Yeah, spontaneous generation.
That's the obvious explanation.
And if you think about it, they're working from Occam's razor.
And Occam's razor says the simplest explanation
is usually the right one, all other things given.
Well, the thing is, is spontaneous generation
has never been shown to be possible.
Right.
If we get the cell thing over here,
let's investigate that.
So what was the guy's name, Virchow?
Yes.
He's saying, OK, well, wait a minute.
I've got this cell theory I'm working on that's
been around for a couple of decades.
Cell hypothesis, probably.
Cell hypothesis at the nice catch.
Don't feel bad, though, because this article that you sent
said that scientists today still confuse those terms,
just colloquially.
And the House of Works article makes a good point
in saying that science and everything that
has to do with it in the scientific method
is very fluid and open to interpretation
and experimentation, obviously.
But so he says, OK, this cell hypothesis,
this is a pretty good explanation for what we now
call spontaneous generation.
He didn't do anything about it.
He just put it out there.
And then along comes Louis Pasteur,
who does do something about it.
He figures out a great experiment
to try to disprove spontaneous generation.
Yeah, it's pretty simple, too.
He basically took a broth, put equal amounts
in two different beakers.
One had a straight neck and one had an S-shaped neck.
He boiled it just to make sure everything in it was killed.
And then just let it sit there in the same conditions,
open to the world or open to the room,
like it wasn't corked, in other words.
No corked.
Notice that the one with the straight neck
eventually became cloudy and discolored,
meaning there was some junk growing in there.
And the one in the S-shaped neck did not do anything.
It remained the same.
So that led him to think, what?
Well, he thought that germs, that there
were such things as germs, which leaving hook and hook
had already shown, and that in the S-shaped flask,
they had gotten trapped in the open neck.
They had been able to just enter unobstructed
and had generated there.
The reason that the S-shaped flask was still sterile
was because there is no such thing as spontaneous generation.
If there were, then no S-shaped neck
would impede anything like that.
And boom, there you have it.
So he disproved that spontaneous generation is a thing, right?
That's right, through the scientific method.
Exactly.
Here's the leap that a lot of people make.
Scientists included that really is a great disservice
to science.
He didn't prove cell theory.
Right.
What he did was take that cell hypothesis
and present some really persuasive evidence
that it's probably right.
Yeah, but this article he sent points out disproving something
is just as important as proving something.
So here's the thing.
That's the most you can hope for as science is disproving.
With science, unless you're talking about math,
with science, there's no such thing as proof.
A theory, even a law, a universal law,
still has the potential for being undermined
by one single experiment, one single observation.
And therefore, there is no real, ultimate proof in science.
There's just theories and support for theories.
And then ultimately, laws aim further and further support
for laws, right?
But they're not proven.
What science does, ultimately, is disprove things
or lend support for existing theories or existing
interpretations of why things happen the way they do.
And that's what Pasteur did.
So if you look at the experiment,
he disproved spontaneous generation,
but he lent support to the cell theory.
And probably, with his experiment,
it went from the cell hypothesis to the cell theory.
Because it was just so persuasive.
And that's what a theory is.
It means that a lot of people out there
who are reasonable say this explanation is probably
the right one.
Yeah, it's predictive.
If you do it over and over, you're probably
going to get the same result.
Right.
But that's not to say that Pasteur
showed that if you do this a million and one times,
that the S-shaped flask won't turn cloudy.
He didn't prove that.
You can't prove that, which is, again,
science can disprove and lend support can't prove.
Very good point.
So right after this message break,
we're going to get into the actual steps
of the scientific method.
Woo-hoo.
We're going to use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
but we are going to unpack and dive back
into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it, and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
It's a podcast packed with interviews, co-stars,
friends, and nonstop references to the best decade ever.
Do you remember going to Blockbuster?
Do you remember Nintendo 64?
Do you remember getting Frosted Tips?
Was that a cereal?
No, it was hair.
Do you remember AOL Instant Messenger and the dial-up sound
like poltergeist?
So leave a code on your best friend's beeper,
because you'll want to be there when the nostalgia starts
flowing.
Each episode will rival the feeling
of taking out the cartridge from your Game Boy,
blowing on it and popping it back in as we take you back
to the 90s.
Listen to Hey Dude, the 90s, called on the iHeart radio app,
Apple Podcasts, or wherever you get your podcasts.
Hey, I'm Lance Bass, host of the new iHeart podcast, Frosted
Tips with Lance Bass.
The hardest thing can be knowing who to turn to when
questions arise or times get tough,
or you're at the end of the road.
OK, I see what you're doing.
Do you ever think to yourself, what advice would Lance Bass
and my favorite boy bands give me in this situation?
If you do, you've come to the right place,
because I'm here to help, this I promise you.
Oh, god.
Seriously, I swear.
And you won't have to send an SOS,
because I'll be there for you.
Oh, man.
And so will my husband, Michael.
Um, hey, that's me.
Yep, we know that, Michael, and a different hot, sexy teen
crush boy bander each week to guide you through life,
step by step.
Oh, not another one.
Kids, relationships, life in general, can get messy.
You may be thinking, this is the story of my life.
Oh, just stop now.
If so, tell everybody, yeah, everybody, about my new podcast
and make sure to listen so we'll never, ever have to say bye,
bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass on the iHeart Radio
App, Apple Podcast, or wherever you listen to podcasts.
All right, dude.
I guess, at long last, we're there.
Like you mentioned before, the scientific method is fluid.
And it's not like when you get your science degree,
they hand you a little laminated card,
like the Miranda rights that cops carry,
that list out all the different steps you have to take.
But generally.
Maybe, yeah.
I would.
We should carry those around.
We should make little wallet cards of the scientific method
just to carry.
Make the stuff you should know logo on it.
Oh, yeah.
We'll make a million bucks.
We could brand them and sell them.
Generally speaking, though, it follows these steps.
The first thing you do, like we mentioned earlier,
is you observe something.
You ask a question.
Next, like Darwin was known, I think
when we did our podcast on him, he
would spend like a week on three square feet of ground
on his property.
It was like even longer than that.
Remember?
Yeah, it was, wasn't it?
He said that he wasn't going to mow his lawn for like three
years because he wanted to see what happened.
Yeah, so he's the ultimate and qualitative data
of just observing, writing things down, and asking questions.
And the reason you ask your question
is so you can narrow something down.
Like I think the example they use in here
is on Galapagos, like the beaks of what bird was it?
The finches?
Yeah, the finch bird.
You notice a bunch of different beaks.
So he finally posed a question.
I think these beaks are different for a very specific
reason, and I aim to find out why.
Yes.
He said, what caused the diversification
of finches on Galapagos?
Ew.
You should have done that with an accent.
Yeah, he would have had a British accent, huh?
Yeah, unless he was pretending to be someone else.
I always think of him as sounding like Hemingway
or something.
Oh yeah, drunk and violent.
But he was, and he was like the opposite of that.
Yeah, well, I saw the movie.
So I picture his voice as the dude
that played him, who I can't remember right now.
Ed Norton.
No.
I finally saw Birdman, though.
Did you see that?
Yeah?
Yeah.
Great movie.
I disagree.
Oh, you didn't like it?
What?
Wow, that surprises me.
We'll get into that off here.
So sorry, you just threw me with that.
Make an observation.
Yes.
He's on Galapagos, and he's like,
what the heck's with all these different finches?
It's one small island.
Why would there be different species of finch?
So ask the question.
And why are they all seeming to survive and coexist so well?
What's what make, yeah, then he leads to the question.
What's making all of these species of finches so diverse?
Right, or Bill Harris uses a pretty good example
that's something everyone can understand.
Like, what car body shape is the best for air resistance?
Like, one that's shaped like a box,
or one that's shaped like aerodynamic like a bird?
Right.
And he carries that out.
And the next step, you formulate your hypothesis
based on your foreknowledge and maybe observations.
Like, so you know what?
I think that a car shaped like a bird
is probably more aerodynamic than one shaped like a box.
Yeah, if you're thinking, if you're the type of person
who's sitting around asking questions about aerodynamics,
you probably already have some sort of sense
that a box is less aerodynamic than a bird.
That's right.
Boxes rarely fly unless they're carried
by one of those delightful Amazon delivery drones.
They don't have those yet, right?
They're not going to do that, are they?
There's like a pizza delivery drone service, I think,
where you have no pizza grilled cheese in New York,
and you go stand on an X after you order,
and it like comes and drops it.
That is the dumbest thing I've ever heard.
And I can't wait to do it.
Oh, but they're making a lot of money.
That's pretty funny.
Yet we can't get food to the homeless somehow.
Exactly.
We can drop a grilled cheese on someone's head.
Right.
They're like, you homeless guy, get off of that X.
Exactly.
All right, so your hypothesis, I don't think we ever mentioned,
is typically represented as an if, then statement.
Yeah, if you're doing good science.
Yeah, like if the car's profile, well, the example he uses,
if the body's profile related to the amount of air,
it produces, which is the more general statement.
Yeah, that's like based on a theory.
Yeah, and it's going to get more specific.
Then the car design, like the body of a bird,
will be more aerodynamic than one like a box.
So that's inductive reasoning, starting with the broad statement
and going to something narrow.
And it's if, then, at the same time.
Yeah, and now you have a test.
You have a question that can be answered.
You can figure out a way to answer it.
Yeah, and he points out, too, this is pretty important
that your hypothesis, if it's formulated correctly,
means that it's testable and it's falsifiable.
Which are often one in the same.
True, yeah.
And that's, again, we go to the people
who say that their soft sciences aren't real science.
They're pseudoscience, because a lot of the data
that they come up with, a lot of the hypotheses they come up
with, aren't falsifiable.
They're not testable.
It's a thing.
It's an issue.
It's a thing.
So next up in the steps, you're going to experiment.
And when you experiment, you can't just go in there willy-nilly
and do whatever you want.
You have to set up specific conditions,
and they must be controlled.
That's the key.
And you want to, everything that's
supposed to be identical needs to be identical.
So basically, you have two variables, at least.
You have an independent variable,
and you have a dependent variable.
And if you're talking about car shape,
that is the independent variable in this study.
Yeah, that's the one that's manipulated.
Exactly.
It's the one you're controlling.
The independent variable is the one you, the researcher,
is controlling.
So in this case, you're controlling the shape of the car.
You have yourself a bird-shaped car,
and you have yourself a box-shaped car.
So the shape of the car changed because you made it change.
Now, when you blast a bunch of air over it
during your experiment, what you're measuring
is the dependent variable.
So you're measuring what happens based on the change
that you made.
That's right.
And you want to study one single variable at a time,
basically.
Yeah, don't get fancy.
Just do good science, step-by-step methodical.
You also have to have your control group in any experiment
and an experimental group.
And the control group is what's going
to allow you to compare the test results to that baseline
measurement, and you need that baseline measurement.
So it's not just like chance, basically.
Exactly.
Like if Pasteur had just done the S-shaped neck
and nothing happened, he wouldn't have necessarily
been able to say that he was right, even though he was right.
He needed that control, which was the open flask.
Right.
So with the cars, you need two cars, like you said,
one bird-shaped and one box-shaped.
Right, or maybe in this case, since the bird-shaped and the box-shaped
both show up in the hypothesis, you'd
need a third egg-shaped one or something like that.
Ooh, I bet that would be pretty streamlined.
Yeah.
Yeah.
But the key, though, is all of those variables
have to be, all the other variables have to be the same.
Like you have to have them, they have to be the same weight.
They have to be painted the same.
The tires, everything, the windows,
one can't have an antenna and the other not.
They've got to be identical other than the one variable.
Right, the independent variable, that's
the one you want different, everything else you want the same,
or else it's possible that, oh, well, this one had bigger tires.
So that actually made it more aerodynamic.
Yeah, and you're just doing yourself a favor
by doing all that stuff.
Yeah.
You want to rule out everything else but that one variable.
After that, you want to analyze your data
so you can draw your conclusion.
And sometimes it's kind of straightforward and easy.
Sometimes it takes a lot of work and a lot of various tools
to draw it out.
Let's say you're just blasting a car in a wind tunnel.
You're measuring the wind resistance using certain awesome
instruments and that kind of stuff
and you're taking that data.
And then afterward, you're going to analyze,
you're going to compare the data that you gathered
from the bird-shaped car, the box-shaped car,
and then the control, the egg-shaped car.
Right.
You're going to compare them and you're going to say,
well, the wind resistance was less for the bird-shaped car
than the box-shaped car, which means that my hypothesis was
correct.
Right, and here are all the data points,
whereas Louis Pasteur could just say, look at the beakers.
Exactly.
Don't be an idiot.
I'm a scientist.
That one's got gross stuff.
You can see it.
Right, but the other thing about science too, Chuck, ideally,
is let's say that egg-shaped one turned out the control group,
turned out to have better wind resistance than anything.
Well, just by virtue of carrying out this experiment
correctly, you would have stumbled upon an even better
aerodynamic design.
That's right.
And you would have come up with that little egg-shaped
Mercedes SUV that was so huge just like 10 years ago.
The Mercedes egg coming to a store near you.
So that's a big, big part of the scientific method
is carrying out an experiment, controlling
the variables, analyzing the data,
and then there's a step that he missed
that is very rarely part of a scientific method list.
That is to share your data.
Oh, sure.
And this is a huge problem with science right now.
Yeah, that article you sent was really eye-opening.
Scientific research has changed the world.
Now it needs to change itself.
Yeah, it's an economist article.
It's up on the internet.
Yeah, it was kind of scary that, I mean,
here's some of the data he points out
is one rule of thumb among biotech venture capitalists
is about half 50% of published research
can't even be replicated.
And a biotech firm, Amgen, found that they could reproduce
only six of their 53 landmark studies in cancer research.
So you can't repeat these things.
It's like everyone's fighting for dollars in fame,
and maybe not fame, but career advancement,
such that they're kind of not doing
that final step any longer.
No, and it's not necessarily just them.
It's the other scientists aren't going back and saying,
well, let me see if your results are reproducible.
People are just taking it on faith.
We need another Roger Bacon to come along and be like,
dude, we can't just blindly accept
that one person carried out this one study,
and then just go do clinical trials on it
without anybody reproducing it to see
if the results can be verified independently.
Yeah, because, and this is a good time to mention bias,
there is such a thing as bias, and it still happens.
A scientist is usually out to prove something,
or disprove something,
that they want a specific result.
Even if you're super open-minded,
you're probably hoping to disprove or prove something
one way or the other, and your confirmation bias might,
even if you don't think you're doing it,
you might nudge out some results
that don't support your hypothesis,
and so you won't make it into that awesome journal,
which this author points out that journals need to start
putting in what he calls uninteresting results
in experiments, like the stuff that's not sexy.
Right, or studies that failed to show
that their hypothesis was correct.
Yeah, stuff that's disproved.
Those things still need to, well, not even disproved,
well, yeah, I guess it is disproved,
but yes, the guy set out to say a red balloon
uses less helium than a silver balloon,
and it turns out that, no, they use the same amount of helium.
Well, if that study gets published
and put out there into the scientific literature
on helium and balloons,
then it's gonna prevent some other scientists down the road
from wasting time, money, and helium,
which as you'll remember is an increasingly needed commodity,
by carrying out the same experiment.
Whether the results are positive or negative or what,
the study's meant to be shared,
and that's the point of the scientific method
is to reduce bias, and if you follow it all the way through,
ideally, and do all of the steps,
including share your research,
whether it's happy or sad,
then science benefits, the world benefits,
and by not doing that, the world does not benefit.
Yeah, he points out that these days,
only 14% of published papers are, quote, unquote,
negative results, and it used to be like 30% or more,
and he says, because a lot of it has to do
with this sort of, you know, getting in these journals,
and you're the rock star scientist,
and this study is super sexy,
like if they kind of quit going that route
and made it what it should be,
then research dollars would be better spent,
and people could, you know,
he said the peer reviewed thing
isn't even all it's cracked up to be anymore.
I know, he mentions a study from a medical journal
that gave a bunch of peer reviewers
some stuff with deliberate errors
inserted into the research, into the studies,
and even when they were told
that they were being tested to find this,
they still missed a lot of it.
Yeah.
So yeah, science needs to kind of reevaluate
the way it's carrying out science.
It's not science, the problem isn't science itself,
the problem isn't the scientific method,
it's the way that it's being used or not followed through,
and a lot of it has to do with academia
and the people funding science.
Yeah, and he said, you know,
these days there's up seven million researchers,
and back in the day, even in like the 1950s,
there were like a few thousand maybe.
Right.
So there's just a lot of career competition,
he calls it careerism,
and so you fake a result or two,
or you just nudge out some results
that don't support your hypothesis,
because you want the bigger paycheck
or the famer notoriety,
and all of a sudden science is not science.
Yeah.
You know, it's pseudoscience.
Exactly.
And speaking of pseudoscience,
I think we've reached the point where
we should talk about the limitations
of the scientific method,
because it does have its limits, right?
Yeah.
Like the way that the scientific method is set up,
especially if you go through,
if you include falsification,
which most scientists now say is a thing.
Like falsifiability of your hypothesis
means that you have a real scientific hypothesis there.
If it can be disproven by some observation
or some measurement or whatever,
then it's falsifiable.
And if it's not falsifiable,
then it's not really science.
So the thing is,
for something to be falsifiable,
and it was actually a philosopher
that came up with the concept of falsification,
a guy named Karl Popper in the 1930s,
and he was the one that said like,
you have to be able to falsify something
for it to be disproven or supported.
And if not, then it's pseudoscience.
Well, part and parcel of that
is that what you're saying has to be able
to be detected empirically.
There's some way that has to,
the presence of it has to be measured or inferred.
And so a lot of people say,
well, then with the scientific method,
it reaches the limits of its current usefulness
when it tries to explain the supernatural.
When somebody says like...
Ghosts are real.
Exactly.
You can't prove that.
Well, you also can't disprove it either, right?
And so if you are a scientist who says,
because the scientific method can't prove
or disprove the existence of ghosts or God,
there is no such thing as ghosts or God.
You're making a leap of faith
just as much as the same person who says,
science can't prove or disprove
the existence of ghosts or God.
Therefore, gods and ghosts are real.
They're both leaps of faith.
And that really the most scientific approach
to the existence of the supernatural,
whether it is ghost or God,
is that we simply don't know
and that we cannot know scientifically.
But that doesn't mean that it does exist or doesn't exist.
And that saying that science shows that it does
or doesn't exist is, by definition,
the opposite of what science shows.
Science shows neither.
It's not capable of showing
or showing that something doesn't exist.
That's a good point.
The other place where science can get corrupted
is when it blurs the lines
or when people blur the lines between moral judgments
and science value judgments.
Like you can study global warming.
You can study cause and effect.
You can report data.
But when you make that, secondly, to say,
and this is a scientist,
I mean, someone can come along and say,
global warming is bad.
You shouldn't drive your SUV.
That's fine.
But a scientist can't do a study and say that
because that's a value judgment.
And that's where science can get corrupted pretty much.
You can study global warming and results
till the cows come home.
But you can't assert that
if you use this light bulb, you're a bad person.
Right, or ocean acidification is bad.
It's not good for humans,
but if you're a jellyfish, it's awesome.
Right.
So yes, again, you made a great point.
It's not science, it's people using science
to make value judgments.
So ultimately, the scientific method,
although it does have its limitations in that
it needs empirical data to prove or disprove something,
it's not flawed.
That's not a flaw, that's a limitation.
And it's when it's misused,
then its results become flawed or skewed.
And that's the people doing it, man, not science.
That's right.
It's pretty interesting stuff.
Yeah, man, this is a good one.
I thought so too, man.
Way to start out with a bang.
Boom.
It's all downhill from here.
If you want to know more about the scientific method,
check out that article on The Economist,
check out Explorables,
and then of course, check out the scientific method
in the search bar at HowStuffWorks.com.
And since I said that, it's time for Listener Mail.
That's right, but quickly, before Listener Mail,
we get asked by listeners all the time,
what can we do, since you have a free podcast,
we can't pay for it, what can we do to help you guys?
And one thing you can do that we would appreciate
is go to iTunes and leave a rating and a review for us.
That makes a big, big difference
in keeping us up there in the rankings,
which means more people find stuff you should know.
After they listen to Serial, they'll just say,
well, geez, there's other podcasts in the world.
What is this podcast?
So ratings and reviews really help us out
and it doesn't cost you anything but a few minutes.
Be honest, we're not saying go leave us some great review,
but go leave us a great review.
Oh, you said it.
And tell one person about stuff you should know,
we would appreciate that too.
Turn somebody onto the show and that's it.
That's our version of a pledge drive.
Wow, we do that once every three years now?
Not very obnoxious.
And it lasts 40 seconds.
All right, so on to listener mail.
This is from my sister-in-law, actually.
Oh, yeah, this is some nepotism.
Yeah, Jenny Bryant.
She mentioned in the homeschool episode,
homeschooled her kids for a little while
and she sort of corrected me.
Love the homeschooling episode, guys.
One very big trend these days in the homeschooling community
is what Abby, my niece, does, which is hybrid homeschooling.
So two to three days a week, she is at school
and then the rest of the time, she's at home.
She's a plant.
She's not a plant.
The rest of the time, she's at home.
So she says it's a great option with curriculum provided
and new topics taught at school and then worked out at home.
Many of these schools are accredited,
making getting into college, including Ivy League schools,
hassle free.
And Abby's school has sports teams, homecoming.
Abby's actually an excellent volleyball player.
Beta Club, newspaper staff, all the good stuff.
The flexibility is great for families
and we are huge fans of how the hybrid approach
prepares students for college
by allowing them time outside of class
to manage their work and life schedules.
So that's from Jenny.
Nice.
Thanks, Jenny.
It's actually via text.
Oh, really?
Our first listener mail via text.
How did you print that out?
Did you retype it and print it?
No, dude.
Are you serious?
You can print from text?
No, you just copy pasted to an email.
Oh, oh, yeah, yeah.
Forgot about that method.
How in the world did you print a text?
Did you do that with your thoughts?
I have a niece who is excellent at volleyball too.
Oh, yeah?
Yeah, we should get them together.
Jeeves, I don't know, 10, 11, 12, something like that.
Abby just turned 13, so they're.
Oh, maybe they face off against one another.
Yeah, is she in Atlanta?
Yeah, she's up in Canton.
You never know.
Where's Abby?
She's in Roswell, but they, I think with volleyball,
they kind of have played all over the state.
It'd be bizarre if they play each other.
Yeah, we'll just see each other at a match one day
on opposite sides of the court, with our arms folded.
Yeah.
What else?
I got nothing else.
Well, like Chuck said, go leave us a review.
And if you want to get in touch with us,
you can tweet to us at S-Y-S-K podcast.
You can join us on facebook.com slash stuff you should know.
You can.
Email us.
We still do that?
Yeah, you can't text me.
Stuffpodcast.howstuffworks.com.
And as always, join us at our home on the web,
stuffyoushouldknow.com.
Stuff You Should Know is a production of iHeartRadio's
How Stuff Works.
For more podcasts from iHeartRadio,
visit the iHeartRadio app.
Apple podcasts are wherever you listen to your favorite shows.
On the podcast, hey dude, the 90s called David Lacher
and Christine Taylor, stars of the cult classic show,
Hey Dude, bring you back to the days of slipdresses
and choker necklaces.
We're going to use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
but we are going to unpack and dive back
into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it, and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
Listen to Hey Dude, the 90s, called on the iHeartRadio app,
Apple Podcasts, or wherever you get your podcasts.
Hey, I'm Lance Bass, host of the new iHeart podcast,
Frosted Tips with Lance Bass.
Do you ever think to yourself, what advice would Lance Bass
and my favorite boy bands give me in this situation?
If you do, you've come to the right place
because I'm here to help.
And a different hot, sexy teen crush boy bander
each week to guide you through life.
Tell everybody, ya everybody, about my new podcast
and make sure to listen so we'll never, ever have to say.
Bye, bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass
on the iHeartRadio app, Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you listen to podcasts.