The Peter Attia Drive - #286 ‒ Journal club with Andrew Huberman: the impact of light exposure on mental health and an immunotherapy breakthrough for cancer treatment

Episode Date: January 22, 2024

View the Show Notes Page for This Episode Become a Member to Receive Exclusive Content Sign Up to Receive Peter’s Weekly Newsletter Andrew Huberman, professor of neurobiology at Stanford Universi...ty and host of the Huberman Lab podcast, returns for another special journal club episode. Andrew introduces an observational study investigating the influence of light exposure on circadian clock regulation and its link to mental health, while Peter covers a phase III clinical trial employing immune checkpoint inhibitors for the treatment of metastatic cancer. They delve into the essential findings of their respective papers, elucidate the reasons for their enthusiasm, and tackle potential limitations and unanswered questions. Additionally, they provide valuable insights into their approaches for comprehending research studies, aiding listeners in independently navigating this process. We discuss: The intricate relationship between light exposure, circadian rhythms, and mental health [3:30]; The importance of low solar angle sunlight, and other types of light needed for optimal mental and physical health [12:00]; Promising new lightbulb technology that simulates low solar angle sunlight [17:45]; The significance of both darkness and the need for direct light exposure to the eyes, specifically [20:00]; Some tips and advice regarding optimizing light exposure, blue blockers, and effects on circadian rhythm [22:15]; Andrew presents a paper which suggests avoiding light at night and seeking light during the day is associated with better mental health [25:45]; Examining the data: the negative impact of increasing nighttime light exposure and the positive effects of daytime light exposure [34:30]; Statistical analysis: the importance of focusing not only on statistical significance but also clinical relevance, power analysis, error bar range, and more [45:45]; Takeaways from the study of daytime and nighttime light exposure [49:45]; The practicalities of minimizing light exposure and screen time at night, the use of sleep trackers, and overall challenge of modern, indoor lifestyles [55:15]; Potential limitations of the light exposure study, reverse causality, and the complex interplay of variables in epidemiological studies [1:06:00]; A tangent on diet soda and sugar substitutes as an example of reverse causality [1:13:15]; Andrew and Peter’s take on the causality vs. correlation of light exposure to mental health, the damage of circadian disruption, and the interpretation of observational data [1:17:30]; A primer on the immune system as background for the paper Peter chose [1:25:00]; Background on cancer: causes, how it evades the immune system, and the logic behind immune checkpoint inhibitor therapy [1:35:45]; Peter presents a paper on immune checkpoint inhibitor therapy in cancer patients [1:50:15]; Unpacking the results of the checkpoint inhibitor trial [1:59:45]; Other noteworthy observations, including the differing results between males and females [2:15:30]; Adverse effects resulting from treatment with an immune checkpoint inhibitor targeting CTLA-4 [2:20:00]; Why melanoma is especially responsive to immunotherapy, and the remarkable success story of immunotherapy for pancreatic cancer [2:25:15]; Why immunotherapy may be the most important hope we have for treating cancer [2:35:30]; Avoiding melanoma: the sunscreen debate, sunburn as the biggest risk factor, and more [2:38:45]; and More. Connect With Peter on Twitter, Instagram, Facebook and YouTube

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Hey everyone, welcome to the Drive Podcast. I'm your host, Peter Atia. This podcast, my website, and my weekly newsletter all focus on the goal of translating the science of longevity into something accessible for everyone. Our goal is to provide the best content in health and wellness, and we've established a great team of analysts to make this happen. It is extremely important to me to provide all of this content without relying on paid ads. To do this, our work is made entirely possible by our members, and in return, we offer exclusive member-only content and benefits above and beyond what is available for free.
Starting point is 00:00:46 If you want to take your knowledge of this space to the next level, it's our goal to ensure members get back much more than the price of a subscription. If you want to learn more about the benefits of our premium membership, head over to peteratea.com.com. Welcome to another special episode of The Drive. This episode is actually a dual episode with my good friend, Andrew Huberman, where we are going to be releasing our conversation on both the Huberman Lab podcast and The Drive.
Starting point is 00:01:18 In this special episode, Andrew and I team up again for another round of Journal Club, and you may recall this is the second time we've done it, having done it back in September of 2023. We enjoy this so much that I suspect we're going to continue to do this, potentially at the cadence of about once a quarter, but of course, we'll see. In today's Journal Club, we start by looking at a paper that Andrew highlighted, which looks at how light exposure and dark exposure can affect mental health.
Starting point is 00:01:46 After that, I present a paper which is kind of a landmark study on a class of drugs that I believe are some of the most relevant classes of drugs in cancer therapy over the past 20 years, the so-called checkpoint inhibitors. The hope here is not only that this conversation gives you insights in the specific papers that we're discussing, both of which I think are highly fascinating, but equally importantly that you can learn something about how to read scientific papers, what to look for, and what the papers say and what's being reported and how that doesn't necessarily match with what the news is telling you.
Starting point is 00:02:21 That's a really common issue as many of you know, and I certainly rail against this where I'll comment on a paper that the media has picked up on and completely misrepresented. And again, there's really only one antidote to this, and the antidote is learning how to read the papers yourself. And unfortunately, there really is no better way to do that than practice. And so what we really hope is that people will sit with us and maybe take a look at the papers before they watch the podcast or listen to the podcast and try to get a sense of what they notice about these papers, what questions arise for them, and see if we touch on similar topics. As a brief reminder to anyone who's been up in the Himalayas hunting Yeti for the past six years and doesn't know who Andrew is, he is an associate
Starting point is 00:03:04 professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at the Stanford University School of Medicine and the host of the very popular Huberman Lab podcast. He's also a former podcast guest on episodes 249 and 270. So, without further delay, please enjoy my conversation with Andrew Huberman. Andrew, great to have you here for Journal Club number two. I'm already confident this is going to become a regular for us. I'm excited. I really enjoy this because I get to pick papers. I'm really excited about it. I get to hear papers that you're excited about and we get to sharpen our skills at reading and sharing data. And people listening can do that as well. So last time I went first, so I think I'm going to put
Starting point is 00:03:52 you on the hot seat first and have you go first and I'll follow you. Okay. Well, I'm really excited about this paper for a number of reasons. First of all, it, at least by my read, is a very powerful paper in the sense that it examined light exposure behavior as well as dark exposure behavior. And that's going to be an important point in more than 85,000 people as part of this cohort in the UK. I'll just mention a couple of things to give people background and I'll keep this relatively brief. First of all, there's a long standing interest in the relationship between light and mental health and physical health. And we can throw up some very well agreed upon bullet points.
Starting point is 00:04:35 First of all, there is such a thing as seasonal effective disorder. It doesn't just impact people living at really northern locations. But basically there's a correlation between day length and mood and mental health, such that for many people, not all, but for many people, when days are longer in the spring and summer, they feel better. They report fewer depressive symptoms. And conversely, when days are shorter, significantly more people report feeling lower mood and affect. There's a long-standing treatment for seasonal effective disorder, which is to give people exposure to very bright light, especially in the morning. The way that that's normally accomplished
Starting point is 00:05:17 is with these sad lamps, seasonal effective disorder lamps, and those lamps are basically bright, meaning more than 10,000 lux lights that they place on their kitchen counter or at their table in the morning or in their office. So they're getting a lot of bright light. That has proven to be fairly effective for the treatment of seasonal effective disorder. What's less understood is how light exposure in the middle of the night can negatively impact mood and health. And so where we are headed with this is that there seems to be, based on the conclusions of this new study, a powerful and independent role of both daytime light exposure and nighttime dark
Starting point is 00:06:00 exposure for mental health. Now a couple of other key points. The biological mechanisms for all this are really well established. There's a set of cells in the neural retina, which aligns the back of your eye. They're sometimes called intrinsically photosensitive. Retinal ganglion cells are sometimes called melanopsin retinal ganglion cells. We'll talk about those in a bit of detail in a moment. It's well known that those cells are the ones that respond to two different types of light input, not one, but two different types of light input and send information to the hypothalamus where your master circadian clock resides
Starting point is 00:06:35 and then your master circadian clock sends out secretory signals, so peptides, hormones, but also neural signals to the brain and body and say, hey, now it's daytime, now it's nighttime. Be awake, be asleep. But it goes way beyond that. These melanopsin, intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells, we know also project areas of the brain like the habanula, which can trigger negative affect, negative mood. They can trigger the release of dopamine or the suppression of dopamine, the release of serotonin,
Starting point is 00:07:05 the suppression of serotonin. And so they're not just cells for setting your circadian clock. They also have a direct line, literally one synapse away into the structures of the brain that we know powerfully control mood. So the mechanistic basis for all this is there. So there's just a couple of other key points to understand for people to really be able to digest the data in this paper fully. There are basically two types of stimuli that these cells respond to. One is very bright light as we just talked about. That's why getting a lot of daytime sunlight is correlated with elevated mood. That's why looking at a 10,000 lux, artificial lamp can offset seasonal effective disorder. By the way, just a couple questions on that. How many lux
Starting point is 00:07:48 does the sun provide on a sunny day at noon? Okay, great question. So if you're out in the sun with no cloud cover or minimal cloud cover in the middle of the day at noon, chances are it's over 100,000 lux. On a really bright day, could be 300,000 lux. Most indoor environments, even though they might seem very bright, department store with the bright lights, believe it or not, that's probably only closer to 6,000 lux maximum and probably more like 4,000 lux.
Starting point is 00:08:23 Most brightly lit indoor environments are not that bright when it comes down to total photon energy. Now, here's the interesting thing. On a cloudy day, when you're outside, it can be as bright as an average of 100,000 lux. But it won't seem that bright because you don't quote unquote see the sun. But it's also because when there's cloud cover, a lot of those long wavelengths of light such as orange and red light aren't coming through. However, and this is so important, the circadian clock, the super charismatic nucleus, it sums photons, it's a photon summing system. So basically, if you're outside in 8,000 lux, very overcast UK winter day, and you're walking around, hopefully without sunglasses, because
Starting point is 00:09:14 sunglasses are going to filter a lot of those photons out, your circadian clock is summing the photons. So it's an integration mechanism. It's not triggered in a moment. And actually, the experiments of recording from these cells first done by David Berson at Brown were historic in the field of visual neuroscience when you show bright light on these intrinsically photosensitive cells, you could crank up the intensity of the light and the neurons would ramp up their membrane potential and then start spiking, firing action potentials or long trains of action potentials that have been shown to go on for hours. And so that's this signal that's propagating into the whole brain and body. So the important thing to understand
Starting point is 00:09:54 is this is not a quick switch. That's why I suggest on non-cloudy days, we'll call them, that people get 10 minutes or so of sunlight in their eyes in the early part of the day, another 10 minimum in the later part of 10 minutes or so of sunlight in their eyes in the early part of the day, another 10 minimum in the later part of the day, as much sunlight in their eyes as they safely can throughout the day. But since you're a physician and you had a guest on talking about this recently, when the sun is low in the sky, low solar angle sunlight, that's really the key time for reasons we'll talk about in a moment. And when the sun is low in the sky, you run very, very little risk of inducing cataract by looking in the general direction when the sun is low in the sky, you run very, very little risk of inducing cataract by looking in the general direction of the sun. You should still blink as needed
Starting point is 00:10:28 to protect the eyes. It's when the sun is overhead, there's all those photons coming in quickly in a short period of time. You do have to be concerned about cataract and macular degeneration if you're getting too much daytime sunlight. So the idea is sunglasses in the middle of the day are fine, but you really should avoid using them in the early and later part of the day unless you're driving into the sun for safety reasons. Another question, Andrew. If a person is indoors, but they have large windows, they're getting tons of sunlight into their space. They don't even need ambient indoor light. How much of the photons are making it through the glass and how does
Starting point is 00:11:05 that compare to this effect? In general, unless the light is coming directly through the window, most of the relevant wavelengths are filtered out. In other words, if you can't see the sun through the window, even if sufficient light is being provided, that's insufficient to trigger this phenomenon? That's right. However, if you have windows on your roof, which some people do, skylights, that makes the situation much, much better. In fact, the neurons in the eye that signal to the circadian clock and these mood centers in the brain reside mainly in the
Starting point is 00:11:38 bottom two thirds of the neural retina and are responsible for looking up. Basically, they're gathering light from above. These cells are also very low resolution. Think of them as big pixels. They're not interested in patterns and edges and movement. They're interested in how much ambient light there happens to be. Now, keep in mind that this mechanism is perhaps the most well-conserved mechanism in cellular organisms. And I'll use it as a way to frame up the four types of light that one needs to see every 24 hours for optimal health. And when I say optimal health, I really mean mental health and physical health, but we're going to talk about mental health mainly
Starting point is 00:12:14 today in this paper. There's an absolutely beautiful evolutionary story whereby single cell organisms all the way to humans, Dogs, rabbits and everything in between have at least two cone options. One that responds to short wavelength light, aka blue light, and another one that responds to longer wavelength light, orange and red. So your dogs have this, we have this, and it's a comparison mechanism in these cells of the eye, these neurons of the eye. They compare contrast between blues and orange or sometimes blues and reds and pinks, which are also all long wavelength light. There are two times a day when the sky is enriched with blues, oranges, pinks, and reds.
Starting point is 00:12:58 And that's low solar angle sunlight at sunrise and in the evening. These cells are uniquely available to trigger the existence of those wavelengths of light early in the day and in the evening, not in the middle of the day. So these cells have these two cone photopigments and they say, how much blue light is there? How much red light is there or orange light? And the subtraction between those two triggers the signal for them to fire the signal off to the circadian clock of the brain and that's why I say look at low solar angle sunlight early in the day. What that does is it what causes phase advances the clock. This can get a little technical and we don't want to get too technical here but think about pushing your kid on a swing. The period of that
Starting point is 00:13:40 swing, the duration of that swing is a little bit longer than 12 hours. So when you stand closer to the kid, so your kid swings back and you give it a push, you're shortening the period. You're not allowing the swing to come all the way up. That's what happens when you look at morning sunlight. You're advancing your circadian clock, translate to English or non-nerd speak. You're making it such that you will want to go
Starting point is 00:14:05 to bed a little bit earlier and wake up a little bit earlier the next day. In the evening, when you view low solar angle sunlight, the afternoon setting sun or evening setting sun, you do the exact opposite. You're face delaying the clock. It's the equivalent of your kid being at the very top of the arc. And so it's gone, you know, maybe let's say 12 and a half hours is the duration of that swing and you run up and you push them from behind and give them a little more push. That's the equivalent of making yourself stay up a little later and wake up a little later. These two signals average so that your clock stays stable. You don't drift, meaning you're not waking up earlier every single day or going to sleep later every single day. This is why it's important to view low-cell angle sunlight in the morning
Starting point is 00:14:50 and again in the evening as often as possible. And it's done by that readout of those two photo pigments. Now midday sun, it's bright light, but you see it as white light contains all of those wavelengths that equal intensity. So the middle of the day is the so-called circadian dead zone. In the middle of the day, bright light triggers the activation of the other ops and the melanopsin, which increases mood, increases feelings of well-being, has some other consequences, but you can't shift your circadian clock by viewing the sun in the middle of the day because it's in the circadian dead zone.
Starting point is 00:15:24 It's the equivalent of pushing your kid on the swing when they're at the bottom of the arc. You can get a little bit more but not much and in biological terms you get nothing. So this is why looking at sunlight in the middle of the day is great but it's not going to help anchor your sleep wake cycle. And if you think about it, this is incredible, right? Every organism from single cells to us has this mechanism to know when the sun is rising and when the sun is setting, and it's a color comparison mechanism, which tells us that actually color vision evolved first, not for pattern vision, not for seeing beautiful sunsets and recognizing that's beautiful or paintings or things of that sort, but rather
Starting point is 00:16:02 for setting the circadian clock. Now, what if you only do one of these enders? So what if you've got constant exposure to low morning light, but your job prevents you from doing the same in the evening or vice versa? Better to get the morning light because if you have to pick between low solar angle light earlier later in the day. And keep in mind, if you miss a day, no big deal. It's a slow integrative mechanism averaging across the previous two or three days. But if you
Starting point is 00:16:29 miss a day, you'll want to get twice as much light in your eyes that next morning. The reason it's better to do in the morning as opposed to the evening, although best would be to do both, is that most people are getting some artificial light exposure in the evening anyway. And here's the diabolical thing. Your retina is very insensitive to light exposure in the evening anyway. And here's the diabolical thing. Your retina is very insensitive to light early in the day. You need a lot of photons to trigger this mechanism early in the day. As the day goes on, retinal sensitivity increases and it takes very little light to shift your circadian clock late in the day.
Starting point is 00:16:59 Keep in mind also that if you do see afternoon and evening sunlight, there's a beautiful study published in Science Reports two years ago showing that that can partially offset the negative effects of artificial light exposure at night. I think of this as your Netflix inoculation. The amount of melatonin suppression from nighttime light exposure is halved by viewing evening setting sun. Now keep in mind you don't need to see the sun cross the horizon, it can just be when it's low solar angle. So you're looking for those yellow, blue, or blue, pink,
Starting point is 00:17:29 blue, red contrasts. And on cloudy days, believe it or not, they're still there, just you don't perceive as much of it coming through. So that's three things that we should all strive to do. View low solar angle sunlight early in the day, view solar angle sunlight later in the day, and get as much bright light in our eyes As we safely can ideally from sunlight throughout the day
Starting point is 00:17:49 And if you can't do that perhaps invest in one of these sad lights so that they can be a bit expensive There are a couple companies that are starting to design sunrise Simulators and evening simulators that are actually good that actually work But right now my read is that aside from one company out there, which by the way I have no relationship to, it's called the 2O Light T.U.O. And that light bulb was developed by the biologist at the University of Washington who basically discovered these color opponent mechanisms. Those lights are not particularly expensive, but they do seem to work. In fact, the study that is emerging,
Starting point is 00:18:25 again, unpublished data seems to indicate that if you look at it for more than five or six minutes, it can induce a mild euphoria. That's how powerful this contrast is. And what they did there in that light, I'll just tell you the mechanism is they figured out that when most people look at low solar angle sunlight in the morning, they're getting 19 reversals of blue orange per second. So when you look at this light, it looks like a barely flashing white light, but it's reversals of orange and blue, orange and red and blue, and it's happening very fast. What is the person looking at it perceive? Well, I've used one of these, just looks like a flickering light,
Starting point is 00:18:59 and of course there's always the potential placebo effect. That's what I was going to say. Is there a way to control for that by having something that looks the same to the user, but of course, is not producing the same photo effect? Yeah, well, they've done that with the 10,000 lux sad lamps, which most people use to try and induce sunrise simulation in their home. But keep in mind that sunrise gives you this comparison of short and long wavelength light. Just a bright 10,000 lux light triggers one of the options, but it
Starting point is 00:19:31 won't set your circadian clock. So most of the sad lamps that are out there are activating only one of the mechanisms in these cells that's relevant and not the one that's most relevant. So I'm excited about what 2.0 is doing. I think that, and, I have no relation to them, except I know the biologists who do the work that provide the mechanistic logic for that engineering. I still think we're in the really early days of this stuff. What should be done is to have this stuff built into your laptop. It should be built into your phone, and hopefully it will be. Now, I mentioned this color contrast thing in sunrise and sunset. I mentioned the bright light throughout the day, but there's a fourth light stimulus that turns out to be really important and this will provide the segue into the paper. Turns
Starting point is 00:20:14 out that dark exposure at night, independent of light exposure during the day, is important for mental health outcomes. Most people think dark exposure. How do I think about that? Absence of light exposure. It's the absence of light, but what this paper really drives home is that people who make it a point to get dark exposure at night, aka the absence of light at night, actually benefit even if they're not getting enough sunlight during the day. And this is especially true for people with certain mental health issues. So I don't think we can overstate the value of accurately timed light exposure to the eyes in the context of mental health. I think there's so much data by now. I will say, however, that some people seem more resilient to these light effects than others,
Starting point is 00:20:59 meaning some people also don't suffer from jet lag too much. Some people can stay up late, get a lot of bright light exposure in the middle of the night and during the day, they've got their sunglasses on all day and they're in a great mood all the time. Other people are more susceptible to these sorts of things and we don't know whether or not polymorphisms underlie that. I personally am very sensitive to sunlight in the sense that if I don't get enough sunlight, I don't feel well after a couple of days, but I'm less sensitive to light exposure
Starting point is 00:21:25 at night, for instance. But I think it is perhaps, this is a big statement, but is perhaps the most fundamental environmental stimulus for levels of arousal and alertness, which correlate with all sorts of neuromodulator and hormone outputs. None of this should come as any surprise. I will mention one last thing. There was a study published gosh over 10 years ago now from Chuck Zeissers Lab at Harvard Medical School. It's a phenomenal lab exploring circadian human health behavior.
Starting point is 00:21:55 He's just considered a nopun, luminary in the field. But there was a study that was in error where they had published in Science Magazine that light shown behind the knee could shift circadian rhythms and that paper was retracted. And a lot of people don't know that it was retracted light exposure to the eyes is what's relevant here. As far as we know the color of one's eyes darkness or lightness of one's eyes bears no relevance on their sensitivity to these types of mechanisms. to these types of mechanisms. So one question, one comment.
Starting point is 00:22:24 The question again is going back to the morning, evening, light. And I spend a lot of time looking at those types of skies, for example, just because of the nature of my hobbies, because I'm always doing archery in the morning and rucking in the afternoon. So it's not uncommon that I'm seeing both of those. How relevant is it that the sun be above the horizon? So for example, it begins to get light about in 30 minutes before sunrise. So if sunrise is at 730, first light is 7 and then 715 to
Starting point is 00:22:54 730 is actually quite bright. I mean, you can see anything and everything and the same is true at sunset. So does that 30 minutes when Sun is beneath the horizon constitute part of that 10 minutes? It does. In an ideal circumstance, you'd get outside and see the sunrise every day and you'd see the sunset every day, even on cloudy days. Some people like myself wake up before the sun comes up
Starting point is 00:23:16 and I get this question all the time. Well, in the absence of powers to make the sunrise faster, which I'm not aware anyone has, certainly not me, I think the best thing to do is simply to turn on as many bright lights as you can indoors to trigger that melanopsin mechanism. If you wanna be awake, if you wanna stay asleep or sleepy, then keep them dim and then get outside
Starting point is 00:23:35 once the sun is starting to come out. Some people wake up after the sun has risen, in which case get what you can. And some people wake up 10 a.m. or noon, in which case you can you can. And some people wake up 10am or noon, in which case you can still get the bright light exposure but you won't shift your circadian clock. Now in the evening, especially in the winter months, it's important to look west and try and get some sunlight in your eyes in the evening. If you've ever gone into the clinic, for instance, at two o'clock in the afternoon after lunch, and then in the winter and then come out
Starting point is 00:24:05 and it's dark when you're walking to your car. It's an eerie feeling. That sort of eerie feeling may correlate with the fact that you missed a signal. Your brain is trying to orient your brain and body in time and that's what all of this is. It's trying to orient in time and again, some people are more susceptible to that than others. Some people might like that feeling of, oh, I went in when it was bright and I come out when it's dark. But the vast majority of people feel better when they're getting this morning and evening sunlight exposure.
Starting point is 00:24:33 And this is especially important in kids. This is one of the things that this paper points out and there are good data that people are spending approximately 90% of their time indoors nowadays. Daytime, time indoors. And those indoor environments are simply not bright enough. You think, oh, there's all these bright lights and some people are putting blue blockers on in the middle of the day,
Starting point is 00:24:54 which is the worst thing you could possibly do. If you're gonna wear blue blockers and I don't think they're necessary, but if you're going to wear them, you'd wanna wear them at night. And in the evening, you don't need to wear blue blockers, you just simply should dim the lights and ideally have lights that are set a little bit lower in your environment, which the Scandinavians have been doing for a long time. So kill
Starting point is 00:25:13 the overhead lights and don't obsess about bright light exposure in the middle of the night. In fact, for a long time, I and some other people were saying, oh, you know, even just a brief flash of light in the middle of the night can quash your melatonin. That's true. But the other time in which you're in this quote unquote circadian dead zone is in the middle of the night. You can't shift your circadian clock in the middle of the night. But all of this gets down to interweaving rhythms of light sensitivity, temperature,
Starting point is 00:25:43 hormone output, cortisol. I mean, there's a whole landscape of circadian biology. This paper, which was published in a new journal I'm really excited about called Nature Mental Health. This journal was just launched recently, is entitled Day and Night Light Exposure Are Associated with Psychiatric Disorders and Objective Light Study in More Than 85,000 People. Now, I have to say that I think the title of this paper
Starting point is 00:26:06 is terrible, sorry folks at Nature Mental Health, because if one just read the title, it sounds like day and night light exposure associated with psychiatric disorders. This sort of newspaper headline, you'd be like, oh my goodness, what are you supposed to do? But that's not the conclusion. The conclusion is that getting a lot of sunlight exposure during the day and getting
Starting point is 00:26:26 a lot of dark exposure at night is immensely beneficial for psychiatric health in a number of ways. Now, I'm not one to bring up another paper unannounced, but I will say that this paper built off a previous study entitled, Time Spent in Outdoor Light is Associate with Mood Sleep and Circadian Rhythm-Related Outcomes. And that was a cross-sectional longitudinal study in 400,000 entitled time spent in outdoor light is associated with mood, sleep and circadian rhythm related outcomes. And that was a cross-sectional longitudinal study in 400,000 biobank participants. So this UK biobank is an incredibly valuable resource and there are now multiple studies establishing that one's pattern of light exposure is extremely important. Now the previous study in 400,000 participants basically nailed home the idea
Starting point is 00:27:06 that the more time you spend outdoors, the better your is your mood, the better is your sleep, the better is the rhythmicity of your sleep wake cycles and on and on. Something that I think, even though people say we've known that for thousands of years, needed scientific substantiation. thousands of years needed scientific substantiation. This new study essentially looked at the relative contributions of daytime light exposure and nighttime dark exposure. And they did that on a background looking in particular people who had major depressive disorder, generalized anxiety, PTSD, bipolar disorder. Here's the basic takeaway.
Starting point is 00:27:42 I'll quote them here. I'll tell you my interpretation. Here I'm quoting, avoiding night at light and seeking light during the day. I love that word seeking. Maybe a simple and effective non-pharmacologic means for broadly improving mental health. So that's a pretty bold statement. And I love that they say seeking because it implies that people aren't reflexively getting the light exposure that they need, that this needs to be a practice, much like zone 2 cardio or resistance training. So what do they do in this study?
Starting point is 00:28:09 So basically, they gathered up 100,000 people or so, it eventually was pared down to about 86,000 participants because some just didn't qualify or didn't report their data back. They equipped them with accelerometers on their wrist and those wrist devices also could measure ambient light. Now that's not a perfect tool because what you'd love to do is measure ambient light at the level of the eyes. By the way, will somebody design an eyeglass frame that changes color when you've gotten sufficient light from sunlight during the day and then at night is a different color and then if you're getting too much light exposure,
Starting point is 00:28:46 we'll go to a different color frame. This has to be possible so that you don't have to wonder if you got enough light during the day. And of course, if it's at the level of the eyes, then that's what's landing at the eyes. So I was going to ask you about that. Do these wrist-based devices potentially get covered by clothing?
Starting point is 00:29:01 And some like, you have your sleeves down. I have my sleeves up. They had it on the outside of the sleeve, but they asked that people just keep it on their dominant hand. It's not perfect. But in some ways, it's kind of nice that it's not perfect. We could turn that disadvantage into advantage by thinking when the person is out and about, they're not often looking right at the sun.
Starting point is 00:29:19 If you're talking to a colleague under an overhang, for instance. So it's not. It's directionally. it's not... Perfect. ...directionally. It's directionally right. Okay. And then they had two hypotheses, two primary hypotheses. One that greater light exposure in the day is associated with lower risk for psychiatric disorders and two second hypotheses, greater light exposure at night is associated with higher risk for psychiatric disorders in poorer mood. This is oh so relevant for the way we
Starting point is 00:29:43 live now, people on screens and tablets in the middle of the night. Then they collected information about how much light exposure people were getting, as well as their sleep and their activity and so on. As you mentioned, this was done in males and females. It was a slightly older cohort than one is used to seeing people in their 50s and 60s. They had psychiatric diagnosis information. And then they divided people into essentially two groups, but they had a lower, so a Q1 and a Q2, a lower quartile. That meant people that were getting less daytime light as opposed to the third and fourth quartile, more daytime light.
Starting point is 00:30:17 They also had a nighttime light exposure evaluation and they had people with the low Q1 and Q2 so these people are getting less nighttime light versus Q3, Q4 more nighttime light. Nicely, they also looked at sleep duration and they looked at photo period, meaning how long the days were for those individuals, how active they were. Look, 10 hours a day, 14 hours a day because the more active you are, the more opportunity for light exposure you have during the day or night, for instance. Okay. So they had, I would say, fairly complete datasets.
Starting point is 00:30:54 And I'm just going to hit the top contour of what they did in each… And sorry, sleep duration, sleep efficiency, et cetera, was determined off the accelerometer? That's right, as well as self-report. Not ideal, right? You'd love for people to be wearing a woot band or a ring or something of that sort, but this was initiated some time ago, so they either didn't have access to that technology or for whatever reason didn't select it. Then what they did is they have information on who has major depressive disorder, who has PTSD, generalized anxiety, bipolar, psychosis, etc. And then they ran
Starting point is 00:31:26 three models. You can tell me what you think about the power of these models, but as somebody who thinks about the mechanistic aspect of all of this a lot, but not somebody who's ever run this type of study, I'd be really curious. Model one examined the unadjusted association between day and nighttime light exposure and psychiatric outcomes. So just basically asking, is there a relationship between how much light you get during the day and how much light you get at night and how bad your depression is or anxiety is, etc. Looking at just a standard ratio of the probability that you have a certain symptom or set of symptoms versus you don't given
Starting point is 00:32:06 a certain amount of light exposure. Model two, adjusted for the age of the person, their sex and ethnicity and photo period. So they looked at how long the days were in that given person's region of the world. Marshall Spicer These people were all in the UK or were they around the world? Stan Mallow They were all in the UK as far as I know. And then model three 3 adjusted for employment. So employed versus unemployed, which if you think about it
Starting point is 00:32:28 is pretty important. Like you say, well, an unemployed person has a lot more time to control these variables, but an employed person who's doing shift work does not. They incorporated information about employed versus unemployed physical activity, which turns out to be very important, and then things like shift work, etc. We can say very safely, the outcomes with each of these models, the results were very similar. So we don't want to discard the differences between those models
Starting point is 00:32:57 entirely. But in my read is in every figure of the paper, it doesn't seem like model one, two or three differ from one another in terms of total outcome. That's an unusual aspect of this paper. So these adjustments are very standard. This is a classic tool that's used in most epidemiology because you don't have randomization. So once randomization is out the window, so for example, the paper I'm going to present is based on an RCT, there will be no models. It's just here the data.
Starting point is 00:33:24 Yeah, here they're asking people, what do you do? Report back to us. We're going to measure your light exposure, but no one was assigned to any groups or swap. Whatever quote unquote controls are there. They were really not there. It's just comparisons between groups. So what is interesting to me is that it's exactly as you said, and we'll make all these figures available in addition to the papers, but it's very unusual that there's no difference between the unadjusted and the adjusted models and as you say there's Probably two places out of 30 when you look at all the different quartile comparisons where you might creep from
Starting point is 00:34:02 Statistically significant just out of it or just into it. But yeah, you could simplify this figure two completely by just showing one of the models and you would be getting 95% of the information. I think in one way that suggests that there's less dependency on those variables. Of course, it still doesn't address probably the greatest question I have here, which I'm sure we'll get to at some point as you continue. I'm very curious what that question is. I'll suppress my curiosity for the moment. So if we look at Figure 2 of this paper and I realize a lot of people are listening and they're not able to look at this, although we have posted the figures on the YouTube version of this,
Starting point is 00:34:41 just want to make clear what's going on just for those that are listening. Essentially what they're looking at is what they call the odds ratio, which is the probability of something happening in one group divided by the probability of something happening in another group. I guess by way of example, if you were going to look at the odds ratio of the probability of somebody getting lung cancer if they smoke versus probability of somebody getting lung cancer if they don't smoke.
Starting point is 00:35:05 So, odds ratios and hazard ratios are often confused. They're very similar and odds ratios generally refer to a lifetime exposure whereas a hazard ratio is defined over a specific period of time. But the math is still effectively the same. And using the example you gave, if you took the odds ratio of death, so let's talk all-cause mortality for a smoker versus a non-smoker, and the answer were 1.78, I'm making that up, but that's directionally correct. 1.78 as an odds ratio means there's a 78% chance greater of the outcome of interest, in this case death by any cause, in the affected group, which would be the smokers.
Starting point is 00:35:45 So odds ratio of two is 100%, and odds ratio of three is 200%. So the math is take the number, subtract one, and that's the percent. Thank you for that. Right, so figure two of this paper is one of the key take-homes. Essentially look at the odds ratio
Starting point is 00:36:02 of people who are in the, let's just look at the odds ratio of people who are in the, let's just look at the nighttime light exposure. And just remind me, Andrew, and everybody else watching, every one of these is showing second, third, fourth as your X-axis, meaning they're all being compared to the first quartile. That's right. And the first quartile is lowest light exposure or highest light exposure. Lowest.
Starting point is 00:36:24 Well. With the differentiate between day and night. That's right. That's right. Restate it. Sure. So if we look at what is your risk of a psychiatric challenge, broadly speaking, well, panel A is major depressive disorder. If you are in the second quartile, third quartile, or fourth quartile of nighttime light exposure. So second being the least amount of nighttime light exposure, third being more nighttime light exposure and fourth, the most nighttime light exposure relative to the first quartile.
Starting point is 00:36:54 This is just a stupid thing. Like if I were doing this figure, if you were doing this in a lecture, you know what you would do to make it so easy? You would draw arrows on it that say increasing light exposure at night, decreasing light exposure in the day. It's the same information. It just makes it easier for the reader to understand. Maybe the teaching point I think is for people when they review articles, like, don't be afraid
Starting point is 00:37:16 to do that and just wait. Oh, I've got scribble all over this. Yeah, exactly. So it's like, I draw the arrow. That's increasing light, that's decreasing light, and that's how I can pay attention to what's actually happening. Right. And I'm actually in touch with the arrow. That's increasing light, that's decreasing light, and that's how I can pay attention to what's actually happening. Right. And I'm actually in touch with the editorial staff at Nature Mental Health, although they
Starting point is 00:37:29 don't know that I'm covering this paper until after this comes out. You know, I think one thing that scientific journals really, really need to do is start making the readability of the articles better for non-experts. I mean, chances are, if you can't understand a graph and this is true for everybody, chances are there's a problem with the way it's presented. Put it on them. But then of course try and parse it because rarely if ever is it all spelled out clearly. But anyway, that's what we're trying to do here. So yeah, the way I would have done is say second quartile is low amounts of nighttime light exposure and define what that is. Third
Starting point is 00:38:03 quartile is more light exposure and then fourth maximum amount of light exposure at night. And basically what you see is that the probability of having worse major depressive symptoms linearly increases as you go from the second to third to fourth quartile. So more nighttime light exposure, worse for you. And there's a dose response, if you will, of the effect. Now, we can march through or describe Figure 2 pretty quickly by saying the same thing is true. Now we're just talking about nighttime light exposure for generalized anxiety disorder. So that's Panel C.
Starting point is 00:38:41 Bipolar disorder, although the difference between the second and third quartile in bipolar disorder isn't as dramatic. Once you get up to the fourth quartile, bipolar symptoms get much worse when people are getting nighttime light exposure. I really want to emphasize that point because they go on in the discussion of this paper to reemphasize that point several times. In fact, they say that while light exposure during the day, of course, we will go into the data is beneficial for mental health. For people with bipolar
Starting point is 00:39:10 disorder, it seems that light exposure at night is especially problematic, independent of how much sunlight they're getting during the day. The person with bipolar disorder who's struggling with either a manic or depressive episode who's making a point to get sunlight during the day, who's also getting light exposure at night, is making their symptoms worse. And keep in mind, they couldn't completely control this, but this is largely independent of things like sleep duration. So that doesn't necessarily mean that the person sleeping lasts, although in a manic episode, presumably they are, it's independent of exercise.
Starting point is 00:39:42 It's independent of a bunch of other things because any logical person will hear this and say, okay, well, they gain more light at night because they're doing a bunch of other things, but it's largely independent of those other things. Likewise, the symptomology of PTSD gets far worse with increasing light exposure at night. Self-harm really takes a leap from being fairly, I don't want to say minimal at the second and third quartiles, so low and let's say medium, I'm taking some liberties here, but low and medium amounts of artificial light exposure at night, then for people who get quite a lot of nighttime light exposure, self-harm goes up, probability of psychotic episodes goes up or psychotic symptoms.
Starting point is 00:40:20 Now, what's nice about the data is that the exact inverse is basically true for daytime light exposure, although not across the board. We can generally say that for major depressive disorder, generalized anxiety, bipolar symptoms there, it's a little more scattered, PTSD and self-harm, the more daytime light exposure, ideally from sunlight, because that's actually what's being measured in most cases. We can talk about how we know that is going to approximately linearly drop the probability or the severity of these symptoms. And we could just explain again that the odds ratios now seem to be going down. So an odds ratio of 0.7 now refers to a 30% reduction in the variable of interest here.
Starting point is 00:41:03 Exactly. Now the psychosis panel F which focuses on psychosis, I think is also worth mentioning in a bit more detail. There's a fairly dramatic reduction in psychotic symptoms as one gets more daytime light exposure, independent of nighttime light exposure. There's a well-known phenomenon called ICU psychosis, which is that people come into the hospital for a broken leg or a car accident. Maybe they were getting surgery from Peter back when for something totally independent. They're housed in the hospital. And as anyone who's ever been in a hospital as a patient or visitor knows, the lighting environment, the hospital is absolutely dreadful for health. Just dreadful. People often complain about the food in the
Starting point is 00:41:42 cafeteria as being unhealthy. That's often not always true Not always true, but the lighting environments in hospitals is absolutely counter to health Especially in the intensive care unit. I think the intensive care unit Hopkins the main one the main sick you didn't have windows People who go into the hospital with a brain injury or with a stroke or something I get contacted all the time even though I'm not a clinician. What should I do for my kid, my parent? I always say get them near a window and start to the best of your abilities controlling their sleep wake cycle.
Starting point is 00:42:13 Now oftentimes there's nurses coming in and taking blood tests and measuring pulses in the middle of the night. That's disruptive. There's bright light, not just blue light. That's disruptive. It's noisy. That's disruptive. ICU psychosis is when
Starting point is 00:42:25 non-psychotic individuals start having psychotic episodes in the hospital because of nighttime light exposure and in some cases lack of daytime sunlight. We can say that with some degree of confidence because when those people go home, even though sometimes their symptoms for what brought them to the hospital in the first place get worse, their psychosis goes away and it's independent of medication. So let's just be really direct. There is a possibility that we are all socially jet lagged, that we are all disrupting these mood regulation systems by not getting enough daytime light and by getting too much nighttime light.
Starting point is 00:43:04 If we want to look at just some of the bullet points of the takeaways, and then Peter, thank you, you highlighted a few of these. Can we just go back to this figure too for a second? Oh yeah, sure. There's a handful of things that really jump out. I had a feeling Joe was gonna wanna dig in the data more. Let's do it, let's do it.
Starting point is 00:43:16 And again, I normally wouldn't make so much hay out of this except for the fact that they're so tight. There are a few that really stand out. And again, I love this figure. I would have labeled it a little differently to make it completely user friendly. But nevertheless, the increasing light at night and the impact on depression,
Starting point is 00:43:35 let me be really technical in what I say, and the relationship or correlation to depression is very strong. The relationship to light and self-harm in the upper quartile. So when you take those 25% of people with the most nighttime light, that relationship to self-harm is interesting and completely uncoupled from the other 75%. That's interesting.
Starting point is 00:43:57 But uncoupled, you mean that at the lower levels of light exposure at night, you're not seeing an increase in self-harm. Not whatsoever. And then once you get to that fourth quartile. It's a big step. It's like a 30 seeing an increase in self-harm. Not whatsoever. And then once you get to that fourth quartile... It's a big step. It's like a 30% greater risk of self-harm. That's right. Yeah, so it's totally flat.
Starting point is 00:44:11 The first, second, third quartile, no different, and then fourth big jump. And then the inverse relationship. As light increases during the daytime, you see this reduction in self-harm. Interesting. The PTSD relationship based on nighttime light and the psychosis relationship based on daytime light. Those are the ones that really jumped out to me. I think anxiety, relatively less impressive here and bipolar disorder didn't seem as strong as well. So I think those are the big ones that jumped out to me.
Starting point is 00:44:43 Yeah, I agree. There's a bit more scatter on generalized anxiety and the degree of significant change is not as robust. In other words, getting a lot of daytime light, ideally from sunlight, is not necessarily going to reduce your levels of anxiety. Getting a lot of nighttime light exposure is not increasing nighttime anxiety that much. Although 20% is not nothing for nighttime light exposure. But yeah, the psychosis, major depression and self-harm, they leap out. Maybe we can just drill a little bit deeper on major depression. And basically when you go from the second to third quartile of nighttime light exposure,
Starting point is 00:45:17 so more than nighttime light exposure, you basically go from no significant increase to almost a 20% increase. And then as you get up to the fourth quartile, so the most nighttime light exposure at about 25% increase in major depressive symptoms. That's no joke. I think that we don't have the data right here, but if we were to look at standard SSRI treatment for major depression, people debate this pretty actively, but light is a very potent stimulus. And the timing of light is critical because the inverse is also true as you get to the fourth quartile of daytime light exposure, you get about a 20% reduction
Starting point is 00:45:55 in major depressive disorder. Yep. What I like about a study like this is that it puts the error bars so easy to see on the data. And why is that interesting? Well, there's a belief that bigger is always better in sample size. And we often talk about that through the lens of power analysis. So how many subjects do we need to reach a conclusion that is powered to this level? That's true. But what I don't think gets discussed as often is the opposite of that, which is what if you overpower a study?
Starting point is 00:46:28 In other words, what if the power analysis says to have a level of power at 90%, you need a thousand subjects and you say, great, we're going to do 10,000 subjects? Well you're clearly powered for it, but you might be overpowered and people might say, well, why would that be a bad thing? It could be a bad thing because it means you are very likely to reach statistical significance in things that might not be actually significant. And so one thing about this study that is just a quick and dirty way to tell that it's probably not overpowered is that you have varying lengths of error bars. And what that tells me is that,
Starting point is 00:47:06 and again, this is not like a formal statistical analysis, it's just kind of like a back of the envelope statistical analysis. If you look, for example, at self harm in the top quartile, you actually have pretty big error bars. In fact, all the self harm have sort of slightly bigger error bars. And yet when you look at, for example, the depression,
Starting point is 00:47:22 even though the error bars aren't all the same size, they're tighter. In fact, when you look at the for example, the depression, even though the airbars aren't all the same size, they're tighter. In fact, when you look at the relationship between depression and daytime light, the airbars are really, really small. So that just gives me confidence that there is variability in this, which paradoxically you kind of want to see. Because it tells me that this wasn't just done... There's, I think you said 8,000 subjects were in this. 86,000. 86,000, sorry.
Starting point is 00:47:44 Yeah, you realize that it wasn't that, oh, this should have been done with a tenth of that or a half of that. And we're picking up signal that is statistically relevant but clinically irrelevant. Thanks for that point. I didn't pay attention to that. I mean, I paid attention to the error bars, but I didn't know that. So thank you. I'm learning too.
Starting point is 00:48:03 I suppose for people that are listening, we can just give them a sense of what the error bar ranges are for self-harm. They're running as much as 20% either side of the mean, the average. And for major depression, it looks like it's more like- If that, yeah. If that, maybe more like five. Yeah, I see what you're saying. So when you get a very large sample size, you're going to have some outliers in there and you can mask those outliers just by having so many data points. Because these error bars directly tell you whether or not you're statistically significant.
Starting point is 00:48:34 So what's really nice about this type of graph and you see these in, there's going to be a graph in my paper where you see the same analysis. They're always drawing the 95% confidence interval on the data point. And if the 95% confidence interval does not touch the line of unity, which in this case is the Hodg's ratio of 1.0 or the x-axis, then you know it's statistically significant to the confidence interval they've defined, which is almost assuredly 95%. Sometimes they'll make it tighter at 99. And so that's why you can just look right at these and go, oh look, in depression, the second quartile didn't reach statistical significance because the error bars are touching
Starting point is 00:49:12 the line, just as the case for the second and third quartile for self-harm. But when you look at the fourth quartile, you can see that the lower tip of the error bars isn't anywhere near unity. And so we know without having to look up the p value that it's smaller than either 0.05 or 0.1, however they defined it. And it's really amazing when you see these overpowered studies, which are easier to do epidemiologically, where the p value ends up being microscopic. They can drive their p values down to anything low because sample size can be infinite, but you can see that it's just the error bar is just skimming above the unity line, but it's so so so tight
Starting point is 00:49:52 interesting one thing that I Hope people are taking away from this study is that? Imagine you're somebody who has a very sensitive Circadian mood system Well, that would mean you need less daytime light exposure to feel good or less bad, but it also means that you might need very little light at night in order to negatively impact your mood systems. And in fact, they make this
Starting point is 00:50:22 argument in the discussion as an interesting point. Again, what I like about this study is that they've separated day and nighttime light exposure. It turns out that many of the drugs that are used to treat bipolar disorder are effective, perhaps in part because they reduce the sensitivity of the light sensing circadian apparatus. Now that's interesting, right? If you think about this, okay, so these are drugs that can ameliorate some of the symptoms of bipolar, perhaps in part by
Starting point is 00:50:50 reducing the extent to which nighttime light exposure can exacerbate bipolar symptoms. Conversely, there's evidence that people who take certain antidepressants may suppress the ability for daytime light to positively impact the mood systems of the brain. Now, of course, we don't want people halting their medication on the basis of that statement alone. Please don't talk to your psychiatrist. But if we know one thing for sure, it's that if you want a significant outcome and a paper as a scientist, give a drug, any drug, and look at the amount of rapid eye movement sleep or the circadian cycle. Pretty much any drug alters a circadian rhythm, for better or worse.
Starting point is 00:51:32 We start to think about which medications might adjust our overall sensitivity to light. Sometimes this could be a good thing. You think less sensitivity to light. Well, for people who have bipolar disorder, the amount of daytime light exposure isn't that important for their overall mood regulation, but the amount of nighttime light exposure really is. In other words, darkness for eight hours every night should be viewed, in my opinion, as a treatment for bipolar disorder, not the only treatment.
Starting point is 00:51:56 But it's also clear that we should all be avoiding really bright, extensive, really bright nighttime light exposure. I mean, if anything, my takeaway from this study is that darkness at night is the fourth key light stimulus. A couple of things, very bright moonlight, very bright candlelight is probably only like, gosh, three to 50 lux. When you go outside on a brightly lit full moon night, I encourage people to download this free app. I have no relationship to it called Light Meter.
Starting point is 00:52:27 It gives you a pretty good read of what the lux are in that environment. By the way, a lot of people don't realize this. They think you just tap the button and then it tells you how many lux. You hold it down. It's kind of fun. You can scan around the room and see how many lux are on average coming from that location or outside. Go out on a really bright moonlit night.
Starting point is 00:52:44 We should have a full moon tonight. Yeah, let's do it. You're not gonna get above 100 lux. That's incredible. You're sitting at a candlelight dinner with your spouse or with friends and it's clearly bright enough to see them. Put that lux meter right up, not too close to the flame,
Starting point is 00:52:59 50 to 200 lux. Maybe 400 lux. I had an interesting experience a couple of months ago on an Elk Hunt where it was a full moon, which actually makes the hunting not so great. But it was the first time I've ever noticed my shadow in relation to the moon. That's how bright it seemed the light was. This is Halloween appropriate. We're recording this close to Halloween.
Starting point is 00:53:22 Super interesting to think it could be that dim. Campfire. Firelight. You think, okay, a gathering around a campfire. Then, okay, you know, everyone's circadian rhythm must have been disrupted for ages before the development of electricity. No. Those campfires are extremely bright, but they're not that bright compared to a very densely overcast day. And what is your phone if you don't use any sort of light mitigating tech on it? Well, distance matters. At the distance we're holding it?
Starting point is 00:53:50 Yeah. So with all the wavelengths cranked up, there is a nice feature intrinsic to the phone where you can eliminate the blues at night or this kind of thing. But if you crank it up to maximum light intensity, probably something like 500 to 1000 lux. Now keep in mind though, it's additive. So it's over time. So lux is a measure of, I think it relates back to Candela's as the amount of light shown. And I think it's like the one meter away and there's a squaring and a falling off of distance. We can look it up. These are old school measurements compared to lux. But keep in mind that if you're
Starting point is 00:54:22 looking at your phone or tablet at 800 lux or 500 lux in the evening, and you do that for two hours, well, you're summing quite a lot of photons. Now, it is true. I do want to be fair to the biology and it'd be dishonest to say anything different. We've hammered on people about not shifting their circadian rhythm with light at night. But we know that the middle of the day and the middle of the night are circadian dead zones. You can't shift your circadian rhythm that well in the middle of the day and the middle of the night. But you can provide a wake-up signal for your body and brain. It's really that sunrise and sunset that are critical. That's why I said there are four things. See sunrise or sunrising. You don't need to see it cross the horizon. Sunset, bright light during the day, minimize light exposure at night. You don't need pitch black. In fact, pitch black probably just increases the frequency of injury.
Starting point is 00:55:09 You know, I get up in the middle of the night to use the bathroom probably once. I think it's normal. I go back to sleep. If it were pitch black, I'd probably injure myself. So just dim it down. Some people use red lights. Our mutual friend. Yeah, Rick Rubin is. Can we tell a funny story about Rick? Yeah, of course. Of course. You know this story, but just in case Rick's listening, Rick Rubin is. Can we tell a funny story about Rick? Yeah, of course, of course. You know this story, but in case Rick's listening, he'll appreciate this.
Starting point is 00:55:29 Rick was here staying last summer. He's up in our guest house and he came down after the first night and he was like unacceptable. You know what I'm talking about, right? Unacceptable accommodations. What did he do? He removed all the lighting that existed in that room and replaced it with red light bulbs, which I later used when I stayed here and then later stole when I left.
Starting point is 00:55:49 The teenage me, I took them. I took them, that's why Derek, when he stayed here, or Lane, or anyone else who stayed here, Conte, or anyone else didn't have those. I took them, I love them. So funny, Jill is like, you know, Rick changed every light in the guest house to red. No, I didn't know that, but I'm not surprised.
Starting point is 00:56:07 Yeah. Well, in his place, he has mostly either no lighting or red lighting. So during the day, it just goes by ambient light and then red light in the evening or candlelight. And it's great. And, you know, people hear red lights and they think they have to buy these expensive red light units. That's not what we're talking about.
Starting point is 00:56:24 You can literally buy red party lights or just a red bulb. Some people say, well, can I just use a red film or can I put a t-shirt over the lamp? I worry about people putting t-shirts over lamps because of the fire hazard. But I'll be honest, I dim the lights in my home at night. When I travel, sometimes I will bring one of the stolen from Rick Rubin red lights. The Rick Rubin stash. Here's something where I've sort of softened my tune. So I used to be kind of a hard liner, no blue light in the evening guy.
Starting point is 00:56:51 Everything was red light at night as far as my phone using flux on the computer, you know, whatever it was. I suspect that that matters somewhat. But I think what matters more is the stimulation that may come from those things. And what I've come to realize, at least in me, which means it probably is true in others as well, and at least some others, is that what I'm doing on my phone matters more than how bright my phone is. In other words, if I've got the best blue light filter in the world on my phone, but I'm doom scrolling social media
Starting point is 00:57:26 and getting lit up on email, that's way worse for me than if I've got my phone on maximum light and I'm watching YouTube videos of F1 cars and driving around having fun. It's a totally different experience. So the context matters. And I think for that reason, I would want people to be mindful of the whole picture going to bed under a period of intense duress brought on by something that's an equally dangerous
Starting point is 00:57:56 component to all of this that's distinct from what we're talking about. But I want people to be able to think of this in the context of everything. It's a really important point. One thing I'll say is that if you're going to stay up past your normal bedtime, if you're going to get a lot of light in your eyes, I would hope that it would be for fun reasons and for reasons you enjoy. You should definitely spend some nights out. You should definitely do some all-nighters studying. If it's going to help you get the grade that's permanent, right?
Starting point is 00:58:21 I'd certainly have done all-nighters studying and grant writing for years. There are going to be the inevitable all-nighters due to trip to the hospital or you heard something on the news that really amped you up or you just simply can't sleep. That stuff is going to happen. So I think the goal should be to minimize light exposure at night. And I think what you just said is especially true because we don't know, for instance, people talk about the negative impact of social media. Is it the fact that people are looking at this little box for so many hours per day? Is it all the things they're not doing? Is it what they're looking at per se? All of those things interact and are really important. We know based on studies from the Stanford sleep lab that if you wake up in the middle of the night, looking at what time
Starting point is 00:59:02 it is can be very disruptive to your ability to fall back asleep and to your sense the next day. It's a placebo effect, but it's a powerful one of how tired you are the next day. They've done this where they wake people up in the middle of the night and then they say it's 4 a.m. versus 2 a.m. versus 6 a.m. and people's perceived levels of energy during the day in some ways correlate with how much sleep they think they got. Likewise, and this is one of the potential concerns with sleep trackers. Allie Crumlin talked about this when she came on our podcast.
Starting point is 00:59:34 If people see a poor sleep score, they often feel worse than if they see a good sleep score. Now, of course, physiology matters. You can't lie to yourself and say, you got a great night's sleep simply by virtue of a sleep score. But I worry more about the false, if it's a false negative, we don't want to put valence on this. Seeing a bad sleep score and then deciding that you're going to have a terrible day. I think a bad sleep score is an indication that you might need to dial some things in
Starting point is 00:59:58 a bit better. Getting a great sleep score is an indication that you might be doing a number of things right and start looking at these things as averages. Would you agree? Yeah, completely. I don't think it's that different from CGM. I think that CGM is an amazing tool to provide insight and you pretty much know the insights after a relatively short period of time. 30 days, maybe at the outside 90 for a person with a very complicated life, and you know all you need to know about how the inputs affect the output.
Starting point is 01:00:26 Thereafter, if you choose to use it, it's a behavioral tool. In other words, you're using this to build in a Hawthorne effect. I think the same is largely true with sleep trackers. Most people have this profound sense of learning when they first encounter one of these things. And it's again, you've heard it all 100 times, oh my God, I can't believe what alcohol does to my sleep. Or caloric trackers. Exactly. Like I think Lane Norton's app, Carbon, I have no financial relationship to it. I use
Starting point is 01:00:52 it. It's taught me, wow, like I consume a lot of calories in the form of certain things at certain times of day. And there's just a lot of good learning in that. But it's the act of tracking that helps you manage it. And similarly, I think it's the act of knowing you're going to be looking at that score that gamifies it that kind of helps people do the right things. You know what? I'm not going to have that drink tonight or I'm not going to eat that snack before bed because I've now been conditioned to see how that impacts score. That said, I think that recovery scores and things like that are just notoriously poor at predicting performance.
Starting point is 01:01:27 And I think there's a reason that serious athletes would never use things like that. They would tend to rely on the more tried and true methods of predicting behavior such as heart rate, maybe heart rate variability, but morning resting heart rate probably more predictive than anything else. And then in workout things such as heart rate, heart rate recovery, lactate threshold, things like that. So yeah, I agree. I think we have to, and I say this as a guy who's generally perceived to be the most pro-device guy
Starting point is 01:01:54 in the world, people would be surprised how sparingly I use things like that. I mean, I do some tracking, not as much as you. I love things that seem to work the first time and every time in terms of our natural biology. Based on a couple of criteria, there's an established mechanism. It's been explored in the context of pathology like mental health disorders as well as pro health in healthy individuals that it make really good sense at the level of kind of wellness and let's just say ancient health, you know, when you're talking about getting a lot of sunlight during the day
Starting point is 01:02:29 A lot of people say, well, of course get outside and play Not getting too much light at night. Of course, this is just good old quote-unquote good old-fashioned advice People spend 90% of their time indoors now their daytime environments are too dim their nighttime environments are too dim. Their nighttime environments are too bright. This misleading aspect of artificial light that when you see a bright bulb, you think I'm getting a lot of photons is part of the problem. The fact that when you're out on an overcast day and you think there's sun, quote unquote, isn't out, well, it's hidden by cloud cover, but just think about how well you can navigate that environment without a flashlight versus at night where you would require a flashlight.
Starting point is 01:03:13 We evolved under this traumatic difference in day night availability of photons, independent of whether or not you can, quote, see the sun. And it's just very clear that all the mechanisms in our brain and body that regulate mood are just powerfully regulated by this stuff. So I've made it a point to really reduce the amount of nighttime light that I'm getting, but I'm less concerned about flipping on the light switch to use the bathroom as I used to be. I used to think, oh, I'm like quashing all my melatonin. This is terrible. I know I can't shift my circadian clock then I know that that light, yes, while it's bright, if it's brief, I'm not going to worry about it
Starting point is 01:03:49 too much. Would it be better to have a dim light on as opposed to a bright light? Sure, but I'm not going to stress it in a hotel bathroom or something. I'm not going to walk around shielding my eyes. People will sometimes ask me, by the way, is it different to look at the phone directly versus if you tilt the phone away. Well, it absolutely is. I mean, think about a flashlight shown on the ground in front of you, very few photons getting in your eyes, versus shown directly in your eyes. Think about ambient light from the sun going everywhere
Starting point is 01:04:14 versus looking in the general direction of the sun, so east in the morning, west in the afternoon, of course. The directionality of the light matters. So I'm not saying that you would need to like peek at your phone as if you're looking over the edge of a bowl or something into it. But my friend, Samar Hattaru, is head of the Chronobiology Unit
Starting point is 01:04:33 at the National Institutes of Mental Health. We used to room together at meetings. We stopped because he's a terrible snorer. There were a few times when I considered suffocating him in the middle of the night since he was already suffocating himself. Now we just, we don't stay in the same rooms anymore. We're no longer postdocs.
Starting point is 01:04:48 But I caught him looking at his phone in the middle of the night and he would tilt it away like he's holding a platter for those that are just listening and kind of like looking over at the screen there and like, what are you doing? This is ridiculous. He said, I'm trying not to get so much light in my eyes. That's a little extreme, but I think it illustrates the point, which is how much direct light exposure you get at night matters. How much direct sunlight exposure you get, especially early morning, late afternoon, and throughout the day, it really matters.
Starting point is 01:05:13 Now remind me, Andrew, what is the wavelength of sunlight? Great. So sunlight is going to include all visible spectrum, right? Which runs from how many nanometers to what? Yeah, so, well, let's answer two questions there. This wrist sensor detected 470 nanometer to 650 nanometer light. So that's going to be blue and ultraviolet. Ultraviolet's kind of high threes. Yeah, that's kind of like blue to orange. Yeah, blue to orange.
Starting point is 01:05:35 That's what this was measuring. So red light is going to be more like 680. Far red is getting out to 7, 720 and up upwards of that. Blue light is going to fall somewhere in the low fours. Ultraviolet is getting down into 7, 720 and up upwards of that blue light is going to fall somewhere in the low fours ultraviolet is getting down into the high threes and lower. And so these spectra of light. So during the day, you know, midday light, you're getting what looks like white light. You'll see, oh, the sky is blue and the sun is bright white light.
Starting point is 01:05:58 It's not even yellow to your eye. And of course, don't stare at it, especially in the middle of the day. You're getting all visible spectra. So you're getting everything from UV all the way out to red light. It's just coming in at equal intensities. So is that a potential limitation of this study in that it didn't have a sensor that could pick up the full spectrum of light? Potentially we don't think of humans as UV capable like we can't perceive UV light.
Starting point is 01:06:23 A ground squirrel for instance instance, has UV sensors in its eyes. Turns out you know why they use this. It's crazy. You know, when the ground squirrels sit up on their haunches, they're actually signaling one another, they rub urine on their belly and it reflects UV. The New York Times for some reason has been running a series of articles about naturally occurring fluorescence at night and all sorts of scorpions and monotremes like the platypus. No one really knows the reason for these odd wavelength of light emissions for all these animals.
Starting point is 01:06:52 But you know, we view things in the blue violet and up to red and we're not pit vipers, we can't see far red. But we can see lower than 470 nanometers and we can see higher than 650. Is there a technology reason why they had such a narrow band in these sensors? Is it not possible that they could have used a wrist sensor that was wider? This study was initiated in 2013. The tech was probably far worse than it is now. Again, I would love for somebody to design an eyeglass where it's measuring how many
Starting point is 01:07:23 photons you're getting across the day. I'm not a big fan of having everything be apified. So I would love it if the frame would just shift color across the morning. Like you go outside on a cloudy day, you know, you wear these glasses. And by the way, it's fine to wear eyeglasses or contacts for sunlight viewing for setting your circadian rhythm. People always say, well, why is that okay? And a window is not well, corrective lenses are actually focusing the light onto your retina. The windows and windshields are scattering the light and filtering. And how much are sunglasses filtering this out?
Starting point is 01:07:52 Too much. We can safely say way too much, probably causing a 10 fold decrement in the total luxe count that's landing on your retina. But of course, sunglasses are important driving into sun. And some people have very sensitive eyes. I can't sit at a cafe with a bright, brightly reflective table in the afternoon. I just squint like crazy.
Starting point is 01:08:12 I can't do it. My dad who's darker eyed and you know, he's just South American descent. He can just sit there just fine. My mom who's got light eyes like me, you know, we're like, oh, it's really tough. You just have a terrible time. People differ in their light sensitivity.
Starting point is 01:08:26 So there's one other macro question I have here, and it's not answerable because without randomization, we can't know it. But it's the question of how much reverse causality can exist in these observations. So again, these observations demonstrate very tight correlations, very strong associations, especially in the five areas that we highlighted. But it's possible that part of what we're seeing is reverse causality brought on by both the treatments, which you've already kind of alluded to, and also the condition itself. Do you want to explain reverse
Starting point is 01:09:03 causality for people? And maybe could you mention for those that missed the Hawthorne effect? The Hawthorne effect is an effect that is named after an observation of what took place in a factory where they were actually studying worker productivity with light of all things. What it refers to is the idea that people will change their behavior when they are observed. So if I said, well, I really want to know what a day in the life is like for Andrew Huberman, I'm going to follow him around for a day. It's very unlikely that his behavior that day will be exactly as it was if I wasn't there. Which is why you probably will never see a day in the life of Andrew Huberman.
Starting point is 01:09:40 Although it's pretty scripted unless I'm traveling. It's morning sunlight, hydration and some cardio or weight training and then a lot of time reading papers. It'd be the most boring video in the world because it's mostly me reading and underlining things. But it's why gamifying things can be beneficial, right? It's why a CGM can be beneficial because it's sort of like somebody's watching you and you're going to modify what you eat in response to it or why tracking can really be an effective way to reduce input because there's a sense of being monitored by doing that, especially if someone literally
Starting point is 01:10:13 monitors it. In other words, you can set up an accountability partner where your health coach or someone is actually seeing the data. So that's what it is. Now as far as reverse causality, when you look at variables, so let's just pick a common one that's unrelated to this. So there's an association that more diet soda consumption is associated with greater obesity. It's a bit paradoxical. Is that true?
Starting point is 01:10:39 It is. Yeah, it's been demonstrated in many series. The greater the consumption of diet soda, the greater the prevalence of obesity. And that has been postulated by some to suggest that non-nutritive sweeteners, such as aspartame or sucralose or things like that, are actually part of what's causing obesity. And while there are probably some arguments you could make around the impact that those things might have on the gut microbiome, and maybe there's some way and that's happening, it's also equally likely, if not probably more likely that there's reverse causality there, that a person who is obese is therefore contemplating how much they're eating or thinking, hey, what's
Starting point is 01:11:18 an easy way that I can reduce calories? How about instead of drinking a Coke, I drink a diet coke? And so there, the causality which you would impute to mean the drink is causing the obesity, it might be no, the obesity is causing the choice of drink. So here the question is, how much of the effect we're seeing is a result of the condition that's being studied? How much of the disruption in both day and night light exposure is the result of the depression? It's dysregulating the sleep.
Starting point is 01:11:51 Maybe they're sleeping more during the day and more awake at night because of depression. Again, you can't know this. This is where epidemiology never allows us to determine this. And sadly, these questions can only be answered through either direct randomization or Mendelian randomization, which by the way, I was going to also ask you, do you know if anyone has examined this from a Mendelian standpoint? That would be very interesting. I don't know enough about the biology to know what SNPs would be studyable,
Starting point is 01:12:20 but that would be interesting. Yeah. What Peter is saying is, you know, if you knew something about the genomes of these people, you would be in a great position to perhaps even link up sensitivity genes with genes for pathways involved in major depression and bipolar. Getting to this issue of reverse causality, I think it's very straightforward to imagine that the person who's experiencing a manic episode is going to be up for two weeks at a time, sadly, and getting a lot of nighttime light exposure. Now, nighttime dark exposure is a treatment for bipolar is something that people are starting to talk about. So making sure that even those people are awake, that they're at least blue
Starting point is 01:12:59 locking at night, reducing their online activities, but people with severe manic episodes have a hard time regulating their own behavior, of course. And it's not one or the other. Like I don't want the question to come across to the listener that it has to be one or the other. It's only A can cause B or B can cause A. No, it's actually a lot of times these things feed off each other. Going back to the soda example, I actually think there's a bit of both.
Starting point is 01:13:24 I actually think there's a real clear body habit that dictates beverage choice, but I also am starting to think that insusceptible individuals, non-nutritive sweeteners will alter the gut biome and that alters metabolism. What about just hunger? I remember Lane telling me, I've seen at least one of the studies that water is probably better for us than diet soda, but that for some people diet soda is a great tool for reducing caloric intake. I also know some individuals, not me, who drink diet soda. I drink diet soda from time to time, mainly stevia sweetened sodas. But what I'm referring to here are people besides myself who drink diet soda and it seems to stimulate their appetite. There's something about the perception of sweet as driving hunger, whereas not eating or drinking anything with any sweetness doesn't seem to. This is one of the things that I wonder if
Starting point is 01:14:19 it impacts why some people like intermittent fasting, because for some people, I wonder if the perception of sweetness, even just the smell of food, we know, can stimulate appetite. So you can mention the perception of sweetness in the mouth, even if there's no calories there. I don't think it necessarily makes people hypoglycemic, but perhaps it makes them think about like sweet means food. For instance, for years, I love the combination of a diet coke and a slice of pizza whenever I was in New York. Ideally two slices of pizza. Now, every time I have
Starting point is 01:14:49 a diet coke, which isn't that often, but I like diet coke, especially a little bit of lemon in it, I just think about a slice of cheese or mushroom or pepperoni pizza. It's like I want it, I crave it more. So there's a parrot association there that I think is real. And we know based on Dana Small's lab at Yale that there's this parrot association between the sweetness from sucralose and that there's an insulin response. They actually had to cease the study in kids because they were becoming prediabetic, which unfortunately meant the study was never published. Have you talked to Dana on this podcast?
Starting point is 01:15:20 I really want to get her onto my podcast. No, we wrote a premium newsletter on this several months ago. It's got to be like, I don't know, 10 to 20,000 words on all things related to sugar substitutes. Okay, I'll read that. So yeah, folks who are interested in this topic, I would refer them to the premium newsletter on sugar substitutes. I think it was our September edition. The short of it is the data are a little bit noisy, but there is indeed some
Starting point is 01:15:46 sweeteners in some studies do result in that phenomenon. You described the cephalic insulin response and I came away from the research that went into that, which was herculean effort on the part of the team, a little bit more confused than when I went in, but being even more cautious around artificial sweeteners than I was going in. And not for the reasons, I didn't find any evidence that these things are cancer causing. So that's the headline stuff people worry about. Oh, I protein causes. We have to ingest like 10 grams of it
Starting point is 01:16:15 or something crazy like that. I came away more confident that from a long-term safety perspective, in terms of cancer and catastrophic outcomes like that, that wasn't the issue. But I came away much more cautious around these things can really be mucking around with both your brain chemistry and your gut chemistry, which can pertain to your metabolism. And therefore, my takeaway was buyer beware, use limited amounts only. The one, by the way, that still emerged to me as a reasonable one, it's the only one that I use,
Starting point is 01:16:46 and I've talked about it a lot, is xylitol. Xylitol for chewing, so for gum, and alulose as an additive. Safer, you're saying? Yes, those are basically the only two I will consume. I'll drink a diet coke every now and again if I'm on a plane or something. This law that got passed few years ago
Starting point is 01:17:04 that you couldn't bring liquids of your own into the airport and onto the plane. What a great scheme to get people to buy overpriced fluids in the airport. I mean, there are more important issues in the world, but this one really gets me. But I drink things with a little bit of stevia and the occasional Diet Coke. I generally avoid sucralose. I don't like the way it tastes. Monkfruit's too sweet, but maybe we'll do a podcast on that in the future. Okay, so I think we can wrap this paper. But tell me what you think about that point. You know more about this stuff than I do, but if you had to just lay on your judgment, if it were 100 to 0, you would say the light
Starting point is 01:17:41 is 100% causal in the effects we're seeing. If it were 0 to 100, you'd say, nope, the behavior is 100% causal of the exposure to light. Again, you can't know it, but what does your intuition tell you? Okay, there's my intuition and then there's my recognition of my own bias because I started working on these circadian pathways, originating in the eye back in 98 as a graduate student at Berkeley. The cells, these melmopsin, intrinsically sensitive retinal ganglion cells were discovered
Starting point is 01:18:12 in the early 2000s by a guy named Iggy Provencio, Dave Burson, Sam Rattar, Sachin Panta, and others. And it was one of the most important discoveries in all of biology, clearly. So I've been very excited about these systems, but if I set that aside, so bias disclosure made, I think 65 to 75% of the effects are likely due to light directly. Now it's impossible to tease those apart, as you mentioned, but to play devil's advocate against myself, you could imagine that the depressed individual is laying around indoors with the curtains drawn.
Starting point is 01:18:48 They didn't sleep well the night before, which gives you a photosensitivity that isn't pleasant like it sucks to have bright light in your eyes first thing in the morning, especially if you didn't sleep well. And then they're making their coffee in a dimly lit, what they think is brightly lit environment and then they're looking at their phone and the state of the world sucks and their state of their internal landscape is rough and maybe they're dealing with a pain or injury or something and their likelihood of getting outside is low and when they do get outside they're going to shuffle and not. So I could see how the behaviors could really limit the amount of light exposure and then
Starting point is 01:19:23 evening rolls around, they've been tired all day and a common symptom of depression, you fall asleep and then two or three in the morning, they're wide awake. What are you going to do at two or three when you're wide awake, sit in the dark? No, you're going to get online, you're going to listen to things you might have. I'm not recommending this but alcoholic drink in order to try and fall asleep. I mean, this is the pattern. Shaking up that pattern is really what so much of my public health work these days is about and trying to get people onto a more natural daylight, night dark rhythm. But yeah, it's impossible to tease apart. We do know this and this is really serious. We know that in almost every instance, almost every psychopathology report of suicide in the weeks, but especially in the days preceding
Starting point is 01:20:06 suicide, that person's circadian rhythms looked almost inverted from their normal patterns. And that's true of non-bipolar individuals as well. Circadian disruption and disruption in psychiatric health are inextricable. Conversely, positive mood and affect and circadian behavior seem very correlated. I mean, I think it's clear that if you want to become an early riser, get light in your eyes and get activity in your body early in the day, you built the, you entrained to those rhythms so that you start to anticipate that morning workout, you start to anticipate the morning sunlight. Just one more scientific point, we know that when you view bright sunlight in the morning
Starting point is 01:20:47 or just sunlight that's illuminating your environment, as you said, you don't even have to see the sun itself, that there's a 50, 50% increase in the amplitude of the morning cortisol spike, which is a good thing because the amplitude of the morning cortisol spike is inversely related to the amplitude of the evening cortisol spike, and high evening cortisol is associated with middle of the night waking and on and on. So, you know, I'm very bullish on these mechanisms. I also love that they're so deeply woven into our evolutionary history, you know, that we share with single cell organisms. It's so wild. But, of course, there's going to be a bidirectionality there and it's impossible to see where one thing starts and the other one stops. Here's my take, Andrew. First of all, I actually, with far less authority than you, agree with your assessment and might even be a little bit more bullish, might even put it at 80-20.
Starting point is 01:21:37 And here I'll give you my explanations, which stem more from my fastidious battles with epidemiology in general. Because so much of the world that I live in still has to rely on epidemiologic data. And so how do you make sense of it? The truth of it is most of it is really pretty bad. I tend to find myself looking at the Austin Bradford Hill criteria all the time. And for folks who don't know, he was a statistician who basically proposed a set of criteria. Believe there are eight of them, and I can't believe I don't know every one of them off by heart, and I certainly used to. But the more of these criteria
Starting point is 01:22:16 that are met within your correlations, the more likelihood that you will find causality. So when I think of your data here, the data in this paper, I'll tell you what makes these correlations seem to have causality within them in the direction that's being proposed. Look at the dose effect. So dose effect matters, and this is done in quartiles, and that's a very elegant thing. If they just did it as on off, it would be harder. High low. That's right, but the fact that that they did in quartiles allows you to see that every example in figure two, I don't believe there is an exception to this. There's only one exception to what I'm about to say. Sorry, two out of like God knows how many.
Starting point is 01:22:55 They're all monotonically increasing and decreasing. In other words, the dose effect is always present. Another thing is biologic plausibility. You've spoken at length about that today. So in other words, sometimes you have to look at epidemiology and ask, is there a biologic explanation? And here there is. You've added another one which is evolutionary conservation to the biologic plausibility. Then you can talk about animal models or experiments in
Starting point is 01:23:20 humans over short durations that generally support these findings. And so those are just a couple of the Bradford Hill criteria that lead to my belief that, yeah, there's reverse causality here, but it's not the full explanation and that more of the explanation is probably the direction that's being proposed. At the end of the day, like what's the purpose of the discussion? The purpose of the discussion is if you are under the influence of any of these psychiatric conditions, in addition to the treatments you're doing now, what else can you do? And to me, the takeaway is, follow these light behaviors. That's a relatively low lift. When you consider some of the other things, like I'm over
Starting point is 01:24:01 here asking people to do zone two for three hours a week and VO2 max workouts and all this other stuff. And I think all those things matter for mental health as much as physical health. But this strikes me as on the spectrum of low asks. If it's only even 30% causality, 70% reverse causality, like I'll take those, I would still instate that. Yeah. And it's taking your coffee on the balcony. People will often say well how do you do this with kids? The kids should be doing it too. You know it means popping your sunglasses off,
Starting point is 01:24:33 it means getting out for just a few minutes and the fact that it's additive that these photon counting mechanisms they sum. This paper also says and I should have stated this earlier, if you missed your daytime light ration, get your nighttime dark ration. They are independent and additive. I mean, that's a really something, but of course, ideally, you get both. But I appreciate your take on it. Thanks for your expertise in parsing epidemiology. I look at fewer studies of that sort, but I learned from you. And that's one of the reasons I love doing these journal clubs is I learn. So along those lines, tell us about the paper you selected. I'm really eager to learn more. Well, I wanted to pick a paper that was interesting as a paper. This paper, I think, is interesting in that it is the landmark study of a class of drugs. But in the same way that you
Starting point is 01:25:24 kind of picked a paper that I think has a much broader overarching importance, the reason I picked this paper, which is from New England Journal of Medicine, it's about 13 years old, is because it is kind of the landmark study in a class of drugs that I believe are the most relevant class of drugs we've seen so far in cancer therapy. And even though the net effect of these drugs has only served to reduce mortality by maybe eight to 10%, which is not a huge amount, it's the manner in which they've done it that gives me great hope for the future, even if it's through other means.
Starting point is 01:26:04 So I'll take a step back before we go into the paper for again, just the context and background. So the human immune system is kind of a remarkable thing. It's hard when you're trying to imagine what's the most amazing part of the human system. And maybe it's my bias as well, because just as you spent your time in the light system and the photo sensing system, I spent my time in the immunology world. But it is remarkable to me how our immune systems evolved. And they have this really brutal task, which is how can they be tuned to detect any foreign pathogen that is harmful without knowing a priori what
Starting point is 01:26:47 that could be. So in other words, how can you tune a system to be so aggressive that it can eradicate any virus or bacteria billions of years into the future without knowing what it's going to be. But at the same time it has to be so forgiving of the self that it doesn't turn around and attack the self. It's remarkable. And of course, we can always think of the exceptions. There are things called autoimmune conditions. So clearly the system fails and the immune system turns around and attacks the self. If you see a person with vitiligo, I have a little bit of vitiligo on my back, a couple of spots. Clearly the immune system is attacking something there and destroying some of the pigment.
Starting point is 01:27:28 I didn't realize vitiligo was autoimmune. Yeah. There are lots of more serious autoimmune conditions. Of course, somebody that has lupus or immune system can be attacking the kidney, the immune system can be attacking any other. Autoimmune conditions can be deadly. But fortunately they are very rare and for the most part this immune system works remarkably well.
Starting point is 01:27:47 So how does it work and why is it that cancer seems to evade it virtually all of the time? This is the question. Let's first of all talk about how it works and then when I tell you how it works, you'll say that sounds amazing. Clearly it should be able to destroy cancer. I'm going to simplify it by only talking about one system, which is how T cells recognize
Starting point is 01:28:13 and get activated, how T cells recognize antigens. So we have something called an antigen. So an antigen is an antibody-generating peptide. So it's a protein, almost always a protein, they can be carbohydrates, but they're almost always proteins and they're very, very small peptides. Like we're talking as little as nine amino acids,
Starting point is 01:28:34 maybe up to 20 amino acids. So teeny, tiny little peptides. But it's amazing that in such a short peptide, the body can recognize if that's Andrew or not Andrew. We talk about proteins in kilodaltons. We're talking about proteins in terms of thousands of amino acids that make up every protein in your body.
Starting point is 01:28:55 And yet, if it samples a protein and sees that, hey, this little 9, 10, 15 peptide amino acid is not part of you, I know it's bad and therefore I'm going to generate an immune response to it. So we have what are called antigen presenting cells. You have cells that go around sampling peptides and they will, on these things called MHC class receptors, bring the peptide up to the surface and serve it up to the T cell. There are two types of these. There called MHC class receptors, bring the peptide up to the surface and serve it up to the T cell. There are two types of these. There's MHC class one and MHC class two. This is major histocompatibility complex. That's correct. We refer to them that way because of the context in which they were
Starting point is 01:29:39 discovered, which was for organ rejection. So not surprisingly, when you need to put a kidney into another person, if that kidney is deemed foreign, it will not last long. And the early days of organ transplantation were rife with immediate rejections. The immediate are the ABO incompatibilities, but the sort of next layer of incompatibility was MHC incompatibility, which would lead to within weeks the organ is gone as opposed to within hours. So you have these two classes of MHC. You have class one and class two. Class one is what we call endogenous. So this is basically what happens when a protein or an antigen is coming from inside the cell.
Starting point is 01:30:22 So let's consider the flu. So if you get the flu, the influenza virus infects the respiratory epithelium of your larynx. And that virus, as folks listening might remember from our days of talking about COVID, viruses can't replicate on their own. What they do is they hijack the replication machinery of the host and they use that either to insert their RNA or DNA to replicate. And in the process, proteins are being made. Well, those proteins are the proteins of the virus, not of us.
Starting point is 01:30:53 So some of those peptides get launched onto these MHC Class 1 gloves. Basically, the glove comes up to the surface, and a T cell comes along. And in the case of MHC class 1, it's a CD8 T cell. These are what are called the killer T cells, right? And so this cell comes along and with its T cell receptor, the T cell receptor meets the MHC class 1 receptor with the antigen in it. And if that's a lock, it realizes that's my target and it begins to replicate and proliferate and target those and that creates the immune response. And by the way, that's how it works when you vaccinate somebody. You're basically pre-building that thing up. So, would this fall under the adaptive immune response or the innate immune response?
Starting point is 01:31:39 So, this is adaptive. Inate is just the pure antibody response on the B cell side. I won't get into that for the purpose of this discussion. The other example is MHC class 2, and that's also part of the adaptive system or the innate system, which is more what we call the exogenous form. So these are peptides that are usually coming from outside the cell. So we're going to focus more on the MHC class 1 because this is peptides that come from inside the cell. So we're gonna focus more on the MHC class one, because this is peptides that come from inside the cell. Okay, so just keep in the back of your mind, if a foreign protein gets presented
Starting point is 01:32:12 from inside a cell to outside a cell, the T cells recognize that and they will mount a foreign response. And by the way, that's why we basically can beat any virus. If you consider how many viruses are around us, the fact that we almost never die from a viral infection is a remarkable achievement of how well this immune system works.
Starting point is 01:32:32 Constantly combating these viruses. Constantly, and by the way, we don't really have very effective antiviral agents. It's not like antibiotics. Like we have antibiotics up the wazoo. We're way better at fighting viruses than bacteria. I've always wondered about this. To what extent is our ability to ward off viruses on a dated basis as an adult, reliant
Starting point is 01:32:53 on us having been exposed to that virus during development? Like as I walk around today, maybe I'll be exposed to 100,000 different viruses. Would you say that half of those I've already got antibodies to because I was exposed to them at some prior portion of my life? Yeah, hard to quantify. And the other ones I'm just building up antibodies. Like I was on a plane last night, someone was coughing, so I was hiding.
Starting point is 01:33:14 And I had COVID a little while ago, so I wasn't too worried about that. And I feel great today, but I just assume that on that plane, I'm in a swamp of viruses, no matter what, and that most of them I've already been exposed to since I was a little kid. So I've got all the antibodies and they're just fighting it back, binding up those viruses and destroying them. Yeah, I think it's part that. And I also think it's part of them that our body can destroy
Starting point is 01:33:38 without mounting much of an immune response. So therefore, your immune system is doing the work, yet it's not mounting a systemic inflammatory response that you're not sensing. So is it also a physical trapping in, you know, in my nasal epithelium? Yeah, you have huge barriers, right? So the skin, the hairs in your nose, all of these things are huge barriers. But assuming that still a bunch of them are getting in, at least the respiratory ones, that's the other thing to keep in mind, right? There were certain viruses that are totally useless floating around the air. The viruses that most people are really afraid of, Hep C, Hep B, HIV, well, if they're sitting on a table or floating around the air, they're of no threat to you. They have to be transmitted
Starting point is 01:34:16 through the barrier. But again, some of these viruses, you're going to defeat without an enormous response. And then some of them, why is influenza, quote, unquote, such a bad virus, whereas the common respiratory cold kind of sidelines you for a day. It's the immune response that you're feeling. The bigger the immune response to the virus, the more you're feeling that you feel your immune system going crazy. The inner leucons that are spiking, the third spacing that occurs to get more and more of the immune cells there, the spike of your temperature as your body basically tries to cook the virus, all that stuff is your body. The fatigue.
Starting point is 01:34:52 Yeah. Yeah. You're being drained and all this happening. So one more point I'll mention just to close the loop on the autoimmunity. How is it that we learn not to attack ourselves? That's something called thymic selection that occurs in infancy. So you and I have a no good for nothing tiny little thymus that would be, it's almost impossible to see these things. You know, when we used to operate on people, the thymus is barely visible in an adult, in a healthy adult outside of thymic tumors. And a child, the thymus is quite large. And the purpose of the thymus is to educate T cells and basically show the T cells what self is and any T cell that doesn't immediately recognize it gets killed.
Starting point is 01:35:35 It's a really clever system where we basically teach you to recognize self at a very early age and if you can't do that, you're weeded out. And then the thymus involutes thereafter because it's sort of served its purpose. Okay. Now, let's talk about cancer. So what do we know about cancer? So we know that, again, cancer is a genetic disease in the sense that every cancer has genetic mutations.
Starting point is 01:36:00 Most of those mutations are somatic, which means most of those mutations are mutations that occur during the course of our life. They're not germline mutations. The germline being the eggs and sperm, right? So it's all other cells. And I love that you pointed out that cancer can be genetic, but isn't necessarily inherited. People hear genetic and they think inherited. Inherited is always genetic to some extent, but genetic isn't always inherited.
Starting point is 01:36:27 Yeah, so there are a handful of cancers that are derived from inherited mutations. So Lynch syndrome is an example of that. Hereditary polyposis is an example of that. Where you have a gene that gets passed through the germ line and that gene codes for a protein like all genes do and it's either you have too much of a gene or too little of a gene. So it's either a gene that promotes cancer and you have too much of that or it's a gene that prevents cancer and you have too little of it or a dysfunctional version of it. Right? So BRCA is an example of that. BRCA is hereditary. BRCA codes for a protein and the
Starting point is 01:37:07 women and men, but mostly the women that we think about who have a BRCA mutation that in some cases almost guarantees breast cancer, it's because of a defective copy. So, it's like they don't get the protein that they need to protect them from breast cancer. So, what do we know? Well, we know that that they need to protect them from breast cancer. So what do we know? Well, we know that, and this is probably one of the most remarkable things I've ever learned, and it still blows my mind every time.
Starting point is 01:37:33 Well, actually, before I get to that point, I wanna make another point. Okay, so cancer, our cells become cancerous, but they're clearly hijacked because they have these mutations, and as a result of these mutations, they make proteins that allow cancers to behave differently.
Starting point is 01:37:49 Cancers behave differently from non-cancers in two very critical ways. The first way is that they do not respond to cell cycle signaling. If you cut your skin, it heals. But how does it know to heal just right and not to keep growing and growing and growing and growing and growing? Well, it knows that because there are cell cycle signals that tell it time to grow, time to stop. Believe it or not, this is an extreme example. If you donated to me half of your liver, which I know you would. Absolutely. I'd give you more than half of my liver. If it meant that we could keep doing these journal clubs, right? Yeah, yeah, yeah. Within months, you would regenerate a full liver.
Starting point is 01:38:28 That's so. Isn't that amazing? That's so well. It's like a salamander. You cut off a salamander limb and please don't do that experiment because other people are doing it anyway. And it grows back. And it knows how much to grow back. When the cell is perfectly functioning, it knows how much to grow. Well, cancer loses that ability. That is one of the hallmarks of cancer. It just keeps growing. It doesn't grow faster, by the way.
Starting point is 01:38:49 That's a misnomer. People think cancers grow faster than non-cancer. There's no real evidence that that's the case. They just don't stop growing. The second property of cancer is the capacity to leave the site of origin, go someplace else and take up residence. So that's a metastasis. That's the metastasis component.
Starting point is 01:39:07 So if you think about it for a minute, a cell that never stops replicating and has the capacity to up and leave and move and take up residence is clearly different from the cell itself. So if I have a cell of colonic epithelium, the cell that lines the inside of my colon, clearly got a set of proteins in it. But if all of a sudden that thing can grow, grow, grow, grow, grow, not stop, not stop, not listen to the signal, and then somehow wind its way into the liver and just keep growing and growing and growing, it must have different proteins. So the question then becomes, why does
Starting point is 01:39:40 cancer even exist? How has our immune system not figured out a way to just silence this and eradicate it the way it does to virtually every virus you encounter? And to me, this is one of the most interesting questions in all of biology, and it really comes down to how clever cancer is, unfortunately, how evolutionarily clever it is. It basically does a lot of things to trick the immune system. So it has its own secretory factors that tamp down the immune system. It grows in an environment because of its nature. So one of the things that's long understood about cancer is it's heavily glycolytic. And when something is heavily glycolytic, it's going glucose to
Starting point is 01:40:25 pyruvate to lactate nonstop. There are lots of reasons for that. I think there's more than one. What does that afford it? Does that afford it a migratory potential? No, so it's super interesting. So that's the effect that what I just described is called the Warburg effect. And when Warburg proposed this, which God was probably in the 1920s, it was before World War II. He thought the mitochondria of cancer cells were defective. So he proposed that the cancer cells' mitochondria don't work, hence they have to undergo glycolysis. They can't undergo aerobic metabolism. We now know that that's not the case.
Starting point is 01:41:03 So we now know that the Warburg effect or the Warburg effect, if I'll refer to him correctly by his name, almost assuredly does not have to do with defective mitochondria. Others have proposed several mechanisms. I think there's probably more than one thing going on. So a paper that came out in 2009, very influential paper by a guy named Matt Vanderheiden, Craig Thompson, and Lou Cantley proposed that the reason that cancer cells do the Warburg effect is that they're not optimizing for energy, they're optimizing for cellular building blocks.
Starting point is 01:41:32 And if you do the mass balance, it completely makes sense. Dividing cells need building blocks more than energy. And glycolysis, while very inefficient for generating ATP, is much more efficient at generating substrate to make more cells. But another proposed mechanism is exactly at this one. Glycolysis lowers the surrounding pH because of lactate. Lactate attracts hydrogen, pH goes down, and guess what that does to the immune system?
Starting point is 01:41:57 Detracts the immune system. So it's also a way to hide from the immune system. Like a pH cloaking, leveraging pH to cloak the signal that the immune system would otherwise see. Yep. And then when you layer on top of that, that it knows how to secrete things like IL-10, TGF-beta, all of these other secretory factors that also inhibit the immune system, basically, it's figured out a way to kind of hide itself from the immune system. The way you describe it, cancer sounds like a virus. Dr. Justin Marchegiani Yes. Dr. Justin Marchegiani Sounds a lot like a virus. And that leads me to ask,
Starting point is 01:42:28 are there any examples of contagious cancers? I recall seeing some studies about these little critters down in Australia, Tasmanian devils that like they would scratch each other and fight, as Tasmanian devils do. They're actually quite cute. And they would get cancers and tumors growing on their faces. It was like a literal physical interaction that could transmit cancer from one animal to the next. So it's less that there are viruses that cause cancer. So in that sense, you could argue yes, there are contagious cancers. Well, HPV. Sure. Yeah, HPV, Hep B, Hep C. But there are even cancers like cutaneous cancers that arise from viruses. But I don't know if that's quite the same as what you're saying.
Starting point is 01:43:09 What you're saying is an important point. We don't want to go down the rabbit hole of the HPV, but that's increasing susceptibility to cervical cancer. Now there's a vaccine against HPV, right? There wasn't when we were in college as we all knew. There was no vaccine. But direct transmission of cancers from one organism to the next, more rare.
Starting point is 01:43:28 Yes. So now, a moment ago I said there's this really incredible thing about cancer that blows my mind and about our immune system, which is that at least 80% of solid organ tumors, and we're gonna mostly talk about solid organ tumors because that's where the field of oncology has made very little progress. So if you go back 50 years, where has oncology made huge progress? It's made great progress in blood tumors, leukemias, and some
Starting point is 01:43:56 kinds of lymphomas. In fact, there's two kinds of lymphomas where the progress has been remarkable. One has been in Hodgkin's lymphoma and the other has also been in immunotherapy, has been in a type of B cell lymphoma where that B cell demonstrates or presents something called a CD19 receptor. So in B cell lymphomas with CD19, there's a very unique niche immunotherapy. We won't talk about that today called CAR T therapy that has got rid of those guys. And then leukemias have also been pretty good. But in solid organ tumors, there've been only two real breakthroughs in the last 50 years. One has been the therapy for a certain type of testicular cancer. And it's really just a chemotherapy
Starting point is 01:44:38 cocktail that has been found to work really well. And the other has been in this really rare kind of gastric cancer called the GI stromal tumor, which happens to result from one mutation in a kinase pathway, and there's one drug that can now target that, and it works. It's kind of amazing. Cures that cancer. Cures that. What I'm talking about are the cancers that kill virtually everybody else. When you sort of line up, what are the big causes of cancer death? Let's start at the top.
Starting point is 01:45:05 It's lung, it's then breast and prostate in men and women. It's colorectal, it's pancreas. Those are the big five. More than 50% of cancer deaths in Americans come from those five. These are what we call the solid epithelial tumors. And you can march down the list and most cancers that most people are thinking of are those cancers. Well, here's the thing.
Starting point is 01:45:23 More than 80% of those cancers have antigens that are recognized by the host's immune system. I will state it again because it is so profound. 80% at least of those cancers actually generate an antigen, meaning a little peptide in that cell gets presented to the T cell and it is recognizable. And now the question is, why is that not sufficient to induce remission? And the short answer is, there are not enough T cells that are able to act and or they are being sufficiently inhibited from acting, which gets me to the point of this paper. One of the ways in which the body inhibits the immune system, which we should remind ourselves is an important thing, right, is something called the checkpoint inhibitor.
Starting point is 01:46:19 Okay. So go back to that idea that I talked about before. You have an antigen presenting cell. It brings up an MHC receptor with a peptide on it, and there is a T cell that is coming. And I actually brought a diagram, which I'm gonna link to this. I don't wanna make this too complicated, but I really think that this figure is helpful to understand how these drugs work. So the MHC receptor with the peptide is sitting there,
Starting point is 01:46:44 and it binds to the T-cell receptor on the T-cell, but there is another receptor on the T-cell, a CTLA4 receptor. And that binds to a receptor that I won't bother naming now. The names don't matter, but there's another receptor on the antigen presenting cell that binds to that. And that acts as the breaks in the reaction. So CTLA4, which is on the T cell, binds to another CD receptor on the antigen presenting cell and it says, tamp down the response. And the reason for that is, we want to keep our immune system in check.
Starting point is 01:47:24 This basically is a way to keep our immune system and check. This basically is a way of asking the immune system. Cause remember, when the immune system sees that antigen, it wants to go nuts. It wants to start replicating and killing. This is a CD8 T cell. It is a targeted killer T cell. The checkpoint says, let's double check that.
Starting point is 01:47:41 Let's be sure let's tamp down the response. And as a result of that, a thought experiment emerged which was what if we block CTLA4? What if we block the checkpoint? Could we unleash the immune system a little bit more? And I will say this, at the time it was proposed, it seemed a bit far-fetched. Because of the complexity of the immune system, it seemed a bit far-fetched. Because of the complexity of the immune system,
Starting point is 01:48:06 it seemed a little far-fetched that simply blocking the checkpoint would have any effect. It's also worth noting that prior to this, one immunotherapy had found some efficacy which was trying the exact opposite strategy. Rather than blocking the inhibitor, it was throwing more accelerant at the fire, which was giving something called interleukin-2. So interleukin-2 is, for lack of a better word, candy and fuel for T cells. So the idea was if we have T cells
Starting point is 01:48:39 that innately recognize a cancer antigen, can we just give high doses of interleukin-2 and have them undergo proliferation and response? And the answer turned out to be yes, but only in two cancers, melanoma and kidney cancer, and only at very small levels. About 10% of the population would respond to these things. Now look, that's 10% of people who were going to be dead within six months, because these are devastating cancers. And once they spread, there are no treatments that have any efficacy whatsoever.
Starting point is 01:49:11 In fact, I think median survival for metastatic melanoma at the time was probably four months. So this was a very grim death sentence. But the idea now was, what about doing the exact opposite approach? Instead of trying to throw more fire at the T cell, what if we can take its breaks down? Instead of giving more gas, let's give less breaks.
Starting point is 01:49:32 And there were some phase one studies that demonstrated efficacy phase two. And the paper I'm gonna talk about today is the phase three study that compared the first version of these. So the drug we're gonna talk about today is an anti-CTLA4 drug called Ipallilumab. There is another drug out there
Starting point is 01:49:50 that came along shortly thereafter that is an anti-PD1 drug. So PD1 turns out to be another one of these checkpoints on T cells. And the Nobel Prize, by the way, I think it was 2018 or 2019 in medicine or physiology was actually awarded to the two scientists who discovered CTLA-4 and PD-1. So I believe this is the only Nobel Prize in medicine for-CTLA4 to a placebo, and the placebo in this
Starting point is 01:50:30 case was not a real placebo. It was a peptide vaccine called GP100 to ask the question in patients with metastatic melanoma, what would be the impact on median survival and overall survival. Let's talk a little bit about the paper. So again, one of the funny things about this, I used to read these papers a lot, Andrew. These used to be my bread and butter papers, reading these like it's my hobby. And I don't read them that much anymore. So it was kind of amazing how long it took me to remind myself of stuff I used to remember. But you do have to kind of go back and read the methods and figure out who were the patients in this, what was the eligibility criteria, why did they do it this way, and of course
Starting point is 01:51:14 it all kind of came back to me, but it took a minute. So the first thing is these are all patients who had progressed through every standard therapy. So these are patients for whom there were no other options. These patients either had very advanced stage three melanoma, which means it was local regional melanoma, but it couldn't be resected. So an example of that would be a cancer
Starting point is 01:51:41 that was completely engulfing, let's say the primary site was the cheek, and it had completely grown into all of the surrounding soft tissue. It hadn't spread anywhere, but it was all the lymph nodes of the neck, and I've seen patients like this, and it's just completely disfiguring. And they'd already been through the standard chemotherapy, and nothing was working, and the thing was growing. And then it was mostly made up of patients with stage 4 cancer. Now melanoma has a very funny staging system. So in cancer we typically talk about something called the TNM staging system.
Starting point is 01:52:15 It is the standard way that cancers are staged. T refers to the tumor size, N refers to the lymph node status, and M refers to the presence or absence of metastases. And for most cancers, it is a very simple system. T is typically a number, one, two, sometimes up to three and four. N is typically zero, one or two, and M is zero or one. Either there's no Mets or there are Mets. So for example, in colorectal cancer, the T staging determines the depth in the colon
Starting point is 01:52:45 wall that it went. N is, did it go to METs? And I think in colon, I'm a little rusty on this. I think colon has N01 or two, depending on how many lymph nodes. And then M0, did it go to anything beyond that, like to the liver, lung, et cetera, or not? Melanoma is a bit more complicated. It has M0, meaning no mats, but it also has M1A,
Starting point is 01:53:06 M1B, M1C, and M1D, and within each of those, it has a threshold for high and low lactate dehydrogenase, or LDH. So it's both a staging based on imaging and biochemical, and the reason for that is LDH level is such a strong prognostic indicator of survival. In addition to M staging, higher LDH levels tend to reflect more acidity, which we talked about why that's problematic, tends to reflect faster growing tumors, higher turnover, higher metabolic activity.
Starting point is 01:53:40 M1A, let me see if I can remember this. M1As are cancers that have metastasized to surrounding soft tissue or soft tissue or anywhere in the body, so anywhere else on the skin. And you might think, well, that's kind of crazy, like how does that happen? And it's really bizarre. You can have a patient who had a melanoma
Starting point is 01:54:00 that showed up in one part of their body and then they have metastases on other parts of their skin. M1B, and I always get B and C confused. I think B is the lung. So M1B is to the lung, M1C is to any internal organ, so liver, et cetera, and M1D is to the CNS. And as those numbers increase,
Starting point is 01:54:21 as those letters increase, the prognosis gets lower and lower and lower. So one of the first things I always look at when I look at a paper like this is, tell me about the patient population. What was the breakdown of patients? And in table one, so that's again, in clinical papers like this,
Starting point is 01:54:37 table one is always, always, always baseline characteristics. Oh, I should mention one other thing, Andrew. This was done as a three to one to one randomization. So again, in the simplest form, the study would have two groups. We're going to just have a treatment group and a placebo group. But in this arm, you had three groups with one of them being the placebo. The placebo got just GP 100, which is just a cancer vaccine. By the way, this is a cancer vaccine that never showed any efficacy.
Starting point is 01:55:09 So it was a cancer vaccine that had been tested both with interleukin II directly, and as an adjuvant for patients who had melanoma resected, who were tumor-free, and then given the vaccine as adjuvants to see did that have an effect on outcomes and it didn't. So it's kind of a known placebo. So you had that group, then you had the anti-CTLA-4 group, and then you had anti-CTLA-4 plus GP100. What's the rationale? For the 3 to 1 to 1, it's basically it increases statistical power. This total study was a little under 700 people. They put 400 in the anti-CTLA4 plus GP100 group and then little over 130 in each of the other two groups.
Starting point is 01:55:56 So you're always going to be able to make these two comparisons. What you can check by doing this is, is there any effective GP100 in this setting, which had never been done before. So again, GP100 is a known protein expressed by melanoma. And all of these people were haplotyped to make sure that their immune system would recognize it. And the question was, would giving people anti-CTLA4, i.e., taking the breaks off their immune system with or without
Starting point is 01:56:25 GP100 make a difference. Going through this, you can see it sort of skews about 60% to 40% male to female. They talk about something called the ECOG performance status. That refers to how healthy a patient is coming in. So ECOG0 is no limitations whatsoever, which is kind of amazing when you really consider something. I think this speaks to just how devastating this disease is. These are patients who all have like six months to live. A year max, and yet look at this, 58 to 60% of them have no limitation on their quality of life at this very moment.
Starting point is 01:56:58 That's going to change dramatically absent a cure here. And then ECOG one has some limitation. And you can see that ECOG1 plus ECOG0 is basically 98% of the population. You can see the staging there. So again, very, very few of these patients are the M0 category. M0s are people who have stage 3 disease that is so aggressive it can't be resected. That's about 1%. but the majority of these people are the M1As, M1Bs, M1Cs. So these are people with very aggressive cancers.
Starting point is 01:57:31 You can also see that about 10 to 15% of these people also have CNS metastases. Again, the poorest prognosis of the poor, and then you can see about 40% of them have the LDH level above cutoff. All of this is to say we're talking about a group of patients who have a very high likelihood of not surviving more than a year. It would be very unlikely that many of these patients would survive more than a year. So basically more than 70% of these people have visceral metastases. A third have high LDH and more than 10% have brain mets.
Starting point is 01:58:08 They've also all progressed through standard therapy. Dr. Justin Marchegiani Yeah. And the chemo for melanoma can be a toxic chemo that really just doesn't really do anything. Dr. Justin Marchegiani So is it commonplace to use a treatment that failed in clinical trials as a placebo in these sorts of studies? Yeah, it's interesting.
Starting point is 01:58:30 You're referring, obviously, to the GP100. I think the thinking was okay. It hasn't been effective in other treatments, for example, when combined with IL-2 or as an adjuvant, but never before has it been tried with a checkpoint inhibitor, which is the technical term for this type of drug. I think there was also some belief that it would be easier to enroll patients. I don't think they stated this, but that's often the case. It would be easier to enroll patients if they would know that even in the placebo arm, they're still getting an active agent. Got it. And I suppose there's always the possibility that the combination of the
Starting point is 01:59:09 failed drug with a new drug would work. So you're increasing the probability for novel discovery. For sure. And again, if you go back to the randomization of three to one to one, it's really only one-fifth or 20% of the participants that would get just the GP100. So in other words, you're basically telling people when they come into this study, there's an 80% chance you're going to get anti-CTLA4. That's a much better set of odds than your typical study where you're going to be 50% likely to get the agent of interest. Right. And people who are literally dying of cancer, they don't want to be in the control group. Right.
Starting point is 01:59:49 So, the primary outcome for this study actually changed in the study. Now, they have to get permission to do that. So the original primary endpoint was the best overall response rate. So I have to explain how response rates are measured. This is a bit complicated. Remember, all of these patients by definition have measurable, visible cancer, by visible either on the surface of their body,
Starting point is 02:00:13 but more likely on an MRI or CT scan. So all of these patients had to be scanned, head to toe within 12 weeks of enrollment. Again, there's another thing I should point out here, which I know you understand, but it's always worth reminding people. When a study like this takes place, it usually takes place over many years. And so it's not the case that all 700 of these patients who were enrolled on the same day and finished observing them on the same day. No, no, no, this took place for a
Starting point is 02:00:37 very long period of time. This took place across tens of centers. I can't remember if this was just globally or across the world. It might have been across the world. And so every center really needs to adhere to a very strict protocol. And you have a central organization that is running this. So you have a drug company, I think this is Bristol Myers Squibb that makes the drug, they provide the drug.
Starting point is 02:00:59 And then you have a CRO, a clinical research organization that is basically managing the trial and the trial is being done at cancer centers all over the world or all over the country. And enrollment, I think, began in 2008 for this. No, no, I think it completed in 2008. It probably started in about 2004, 2005. And therefore, you had to kind of have real clear protocols around this. So a complete response is the easier of these to understand.
Starting point is 02:01:25 A complete response is everything vanishes completely. That's very rare in cancer therapy. So instead, what we look for is a partial response. A partial response, and there are really different ways to define this. There are different criteria, but this is the most common way you define a partial response. A partial response is at least a 50% reduction by diameter, because remember in this type of imaging, you're looking at 2D versus 3D. So if you're looking at a lung lesion and it's this big, you know, if it's two centimeters long, it has to go to at least one centimeter in diameter.
Starting point is 02:02:05 So it's a 50% reduction at least of every single lesion with no new lesions appearing and no lesions growing. So it's a very strict criteria. Right, again, CR means everything vanishes, PR means at least a 50% by diameter, which by the way is a much bigger reduction in terms of tumor volume when you consider the linear versus the third power relationship of length and volume of every single lesion with nothing new appearing regardless of how small and no lesion growing. So that's a PR. So you basically have no response, progression. We talk about those together,
Starting point is 02:02:44 and then partial response and complete response. So initially, the authors of this study, the primary endpoint of this was going to be the best overall response rate. So what was the proportion of patients that hit PR? What was the proportion that hit a CR? That's very common in this type of paper where the outcomes are typically so dire. I don't remember when this study ended, but the amendment was made to change the primary endpoint to overall survival at some point during the study. And by the way, that tends to be the metric everybody cares most about. So the overall survival for metastatic melanoma is zero, with the exception of people who respond to
Starting point is 02:03:25 interleukin-2, high-dose interleukin-2, and that will boost the overall survival rate to somewhere between 8 and 10 percent. Very, very low. These patients, many of whom had already taken and progressed through interleukin-2. Let me refresh my memory on what percentage of those patients. About a quarter of these patients had already taken high dose interleukin-2 and by definition, the fact that they're in this study means they had already progressed through that. That treatment had failed. Just reiterate the state these patients are in. So now let's look at figure one. So again, I'll describe it because I realize many people
Starting point is 02:04:10 are just listening to us. All of this will be available both in the video and then we'll link to the paper. So figure one is a figure that probably looks really familiar to people who look at any data that deal with survival. It's called the Kaplan-Meier survival curve. So on the x-axis for this curve is time, and time here is shown in months. And on the y-axis is the overall survival, the very top 100% at the bottom, 0%. And it has three graphs or three curves
Starting point is 02:04:42 that are superimposed on one another for each of the three groups. Again, the control group, which is the GP100, the anti-CTLA4 group by itself, and the anti-CTLA4 plus GP100. And one of the characteristics of a Kaplan-Meier curve is by definition they have to be decreasing in a monotonic fashion because it's cumulative overall survival. That just means it can't come down and go back up. Nobody comes back to life. So once a person dies, they are censored from the study
Starting point is 02:05:12 and the curve drops and drops and drops. And you can see that they kind of highlight, and I actually think it makes the graph a little harder to read when they put some of those marks on there. But what really becomes clear when you look at this is that there's a clear distinction between the curve for the placebo group, the GP100 group, and the other two, the two treatment groups. Now, you'll note at the very end that the two treatment groups appear to separate a little bit. I'll talk about that in a second. So when I look at these, Andrew,
Starting point is 02:05:46 the first thing I always turn my attention to, I can't resist, I have to look at the right hand side of the graph. Because what is that really telling me? The tale of this is showing me the true overall survival. And I wanna sort of figure out what is going on. So in the GP 100 group, which is the placebo group, it is kind of amazing to think that there is still
Starting point is 02:06:06 one person who is alive at 44 months. It's amazing. I mean, it's both sobering and amazing that like one person made it to 44 months. The next thing I ask myself is, well, how long did half of the people make it? That's called median survival. And to do that, you go up to the y-axis and you draw a little line from the 50 over and then you bring that down. That's awfully low. Yeah. In fact, the table will tell us exactly what that is because I think it's really hard to eyeball that stuff. So let's go to... So there's always a table that will accompany these things. And let's pull up that table. I've got this paper spread out over so many things.
Starting point is 02:06:48 Let's adverse events. Where's our survival table here? Two subgroup analysis of overall survival. It would probably be helpful if I stapled these things together because it would be easier. Well, this is always a trade-off. Since this is a Journal Club episode, I will say that stapling helps,
Starting point is 02:07:04 but it also prevents one from separating things out, writing in the margins. I like these little mini clips. No financial relationship to the mini clips either. Just have to state that because if you don't say that, people go, oh, you must have a stake in these mini clips. I like these little mini clips. In fact, I'm such a nerd.
Starting point is 02:07:22 I always have one of these pilot V5 V7s on my pocket or my hip. And then my pockets are always filled with these little mini clips. But then again, I have a friend who's a musician, he's always reigning guitar picks. As far as occupational hazards go of being a nerd, these mini clips are- I'm a big fan of the mini clip as well. Yeah. I went without it today. All right. So thank you. Yes. Table two. Let's look at table two while looking at the Kaplan MeMeier curve, because now this allows us to see a couple of things.
Starting point is 02:07:48 By the way, remember how I said there's like that one person who is still alive in the treatment group? Well, you can tell that he's not a complete responder. He or she is not a complete responder, because under evaluation of therapy in table two, it says best overall response, and it says complete responders, zero. So there was zero complete responders in the placebo. There were two partial responders.
Starting point is 02:08:14 Again, a partial responder is some lesions got smaller, some got bigger, stable disease. It didn't really change that much, and progressive disease is obviously it went beyond. And when you say partial response, like lesions got smaller, are they literally just tracing the circumference of one of these skin lesions and saying, okay, got bigger, smaller, like just morphology? Yep. Yep.
Starting point is 02:08:39 Gosh, this feels so crude in terms of like, I mean, it makes total sense, but like in terms of modern medicine, oh, like your total sense, but like in terms of modern medicine Oh, like your lesion grew from like three millimeters to six millimeters and they're literally like drawing little boundaries around little blotches on the skin Yeah, you're putting a little measuring tape on them Now again, most of these are happening in the radiology suite because most of the disease for these patients is inside the body Remember more than 70% of these patients had visceral metastases. So liver, soft tissue, lung, brain. In fact, if you include lung, liver, brain and viscera, it's pretty much all the patients.
Starting point is 02:09:18 So most of this is looking at a CT scan or an MRI for the brain. Okay. So that's kind of the first thing that comes up. The median response rate should be shown pretty prominently here. So I'm looking through this and... where is median response? Maybe it's shown in a different table. Not disease control rate. Time to progression. I remember it's about 10 months but maybe that's just in the text. Yeah, here it is. I thought this would be in a table, but it's on page 715 of the paper. It just reports it. So I'm sorry, I misspoke.
Starting point is 02:09:53 The 10 months was for the anti-CTLA4 plus GP100 and 6.4 months for the GP100 alone. That's the control. And then 10.1 for the NTC TLA4 alone. And again, I'm just always doing this. I'm kind of going back to the paper to be like, does that make sense? And yeah, you called it, right? You said median survival was about eight.
Starting point is 02:10:16 Well, it turns out it's actually like six and change because it has that little ding in it. And it's out to a little past 10 on the two others. So the net takeaway here is, again, just to put that in English, because it's so profound, 50% of the patients in the control group were dead in six months. 50% of the patients in the treatment group, both treatment groups, were dead in 10 months. So what that means in cancer speak is these drugs extended median survival by four months. Now that's an important concept. When we think about how has cancer therapy
Starting point is 02:10:55 changed over the past 50 years, median survival for metastatic cancer has increased across the board. So a person today with metastatic colorectal cancer or a woman today with metastatic cancer has increased across the board. So a person today with metastatic colorectal cancer or a woman today with metastatic breast cancer or a person with metastatic lung cancer, these people will live longer with those diseases today thanks mostly to treatments. This is not an early detection lead time bias issue. This is treatments are allowing people to live longer and that's an important part of the story but it's only half of the story yet it often gets touted as the story. The other half of the story and
Starting point is 02:11:35 frankly the story that I think is more important is what is overall survival doing and if you go back to those cancers, the answer is zero. Overall survival hasn't changed for solid epithelial tumors. It was 0% in 1970 and it's 0% today. Everyone dies. Everyone dies from metastatic solid organ tumors. There's those niche examples I gave you. Testicular cancer is now an exception. GI stromal tumors would be an exception. And
Starting point is 02:12:05 I'm not including leukemias and lymphomas where now there are exceptions. Dr. Justin Marchegiani Not to try and be overly optimistic, but if I look at the graph in figure one and I look out at the tail of the graph. Dr. Justin Marchegiani That's right. Dr. Justin Marchegiani And for those that are just listening, what I see and I'm far less, far less familiar with this type of work and this analyzing these type of data. But what I see is that people in the placebo group, they're all dead except that one.
Starting point is 02:12:33 They're basically all dead at 44 months. But when I look at how long it takes for everyone to be dead in the treatment groups, looks like 53, 54 months or so. And they're not dead. That's the point. They're hanging in there, right? So, because, you know, an extra, somebody who lost both of my scientific advisors, two of the three, the other one is suicide.
Starting point is 02:12:57 We've talked about this before. But the other two to different cancers both had the Brackayte mutation by the way. You know, an extra eight to 10 months with your kids or with your spouse or to quote unquote get your affairs in order is a big deal. I mean, it's still depressing in the sense that nobody survives long term, but you know, an extra 10 months as long as one is not miserable in that time, completely miserable. I mean, that's extra 10 months of living. And what's interesting here is the observation period stops and some of these patients are still going.
Starting point is 02:13:30 What you're highlighting is kind of the point I wanna make, which is overall survival is the most important metric and it's the highest bar, make no mistake about it, and it's certainly not the bar any drug company is ever going to want to talk about for a cancer drug. But why not? None of them work.
Starting point is 02:13:48 They only want to talk about cures. They don't want to talk about extending media and survival. They only want to talk about extending media and survival. And there are lots of people out there that are on this platform. I don't need to get onto it, but we'll say, look, it's a real racket in oncology today where drugs that are extending median survival by four weeks are being put on the market at a tune of $50,000 to $100,000 per treatment.
Starting point is 02:14:13 That's not uncommon in oncology. There was one drug that was approved for pancreatic cancer. I believe it extended median survival by nine days and it cost $40,000. And it's being advertised as significant. That was a statistical significant improvement in media and survival. It's really understandable why people are very skeptical of the pharma industry. And I think a much more nuanced view is necessary. Clearly I don't think pharma is all bad, but I really understand why people lose faith
Starting point is 02:14:38 in pharma when these types of products somehow make regulatory approval. Does insurance cover these kinds of drugs? It can, in fact, it often does. It depends on the FDA approval, of course, and the indication, but a lot of times they do. Yeah, there's a societal cost to these things, but there's also a patient cost. So a lot of times insurance doesn't fully cover it
Starting point is 02:14:59 and a patient has to bear the cost difference. And on top of that, you alluded to this a second ago, which is what if your quality of life is dramatically compromised as a result of this treatment? And yes, statistically, you're gonna live nine days longer or three weeks longer, but at what cost to your health in those final remaining days?
Starting point is 02:15:17 And by the way, you're potentially straddling your loved ones with enormous debt in your absence. So it's a super complicated topic. There's a dignity component too. I mean, I've seen this in people dying, you know, at some point they become such a diminished version of their former selves that they don't want to be seen by people that way. Yep. So what is exciting about this drug, this paper is not the one that shows it.
Starting point is 02:15:46 The reason I chose this paper, Andrew, is because it was the first approval. A second drug came along that is an anti-PD1 drug. That drug is called K-truda. That drug turned out to be even better. Has even a greater response rate, both in terms of median survival and overall survival. But this was the landmark paper.
Starting point is 02:16:07 I also have a slight bias here, and I'll disclose in a moment why, but I think it just talks about very interesting biology. Let's talk about a couple of things that stuck out to me in this paper. The first thing that stuck out to me, and the authors didn't comment on it unless they did, and I missed it, is look at Figure 2.
Starting point is 02:16:26 So Figure 2 is the subgroup analyses where you're sort of showing a similar graph to the one you showed earlier. You show the response rate or the change in response between the groups and then you put the error bars on it and this is where we talk about how well it's a 95% confidence interval so it doesn't touch the unity line. So these are called tornado plots typically. What you'll notice is that in the top it's comparing the anti-CTLA4 with GP100 versus the GP100 and in the bottom you're looking at the anti-CTLA4 versus the GP100. So at a glance you can see GP100 is not doing anything. That's the first takeaway of
Starting point is 02:17:09 comparing A to B. What I find most interesting is look at the subgroup analysis of females. Notice that in females while there's a trend towards risk reduction and this is risk reduction for overall mortality. So again I just want to restate that. The primary outcome of this trial was changed to overall survival, which I think is the better outcome, by the way. And overall, for all patients, when you compare anti-CTLA4 plus placebo versus placebo, there was a 31% risk reduction in overall mortality.
Starting point is 02:17:46 That's the mathematical interpretation of what you're seeing at the tail end of that Kaplan-Meier curve. Living longer. Living longer. And it sounds like a big difference. Sounds like a big difference. Sometimes it is a big difference.
Starting point is 02:17:57 Well, it is for those people because you're really looking at basically 0% surviving in the placebo group versus 20% of people are still alive at 56 months in the treatment group. But look, that means 80% have died. But notice that, and sorry, when you just look at the anti-CTLA4 plus GP100 in the subgroup B, that hazard ratio is even showing more compression. It's a 36% reduction in risk of death. But notice that the females did not reach significance. So in the first group, they barely do.
Starting point is 02:18:37 And you can see that because the confidence interval runs from 0.55 to 0.92. And notice the error bar almost touches the line and in the second one it does not reach significance at all. So I actually went and kind of did a little reading on this after and I said, hey, was this an outlier study? And it turned out it wasn't and that about half the studies of anti-CTLA-4 did indeed find that the drug was less effective in women than men, before did indeed find that the drug was less effective in women than men. Which I found interesting. Now, I couldn't find any great explanation for it, but the most plausible explanations fit into two categories.
Starting point is 02:19:13 The first are maybe there are differences in the immune response to the drug if you're a man or a woman. The second comes down to dosing. I should have said this at the outset, but of course these drugs are not like a pill where it's like Everybody gets 50 milligrams of this. They're all dose based on weight. So this study is dosed I believe at 3 milligrams per kilogram and because most men are heavier than women men are getting a higher dose than women and Weight and body surface area and immune system, like these things are not all perfectly linear. I kind of wonder if this difference is simply explained by
Starting point is 02:19:53 men on average getting a higher dose than women. Last thing I want to talk about here is in table three. So table three, always an important table to look at in any paper is what are the adverse outcomes? Where are the adverse effects of the drug? I've spent a little bit of time with this and I confess I definitely don't want cancer to the extent that I can avoid it, but this table made me wonder whether or not
Starting point is 02:20:19 I would also want to just avoid cancer treatment given the life extension provided. I mean, these adverse events are pretty uncomfortable. Just to put in perspective, and you always have to kind of be mindful of how many of these adverse events are occurring in people just because their disease is progressing. So the first thing I always want to look at is total adverse events in all three groups. So grade three and grade four are real toxicities. Grade four toxicity is life-threatening toxicity, by the way.
Starting point is 02:20:49 Grade three is pretty significant toxicity. Grade one and two, that's not that severe. The little rash, put some corticosteroids on it, it went away, kind of thing. Okay, so in the treatment plus GP 100 group, 98.4% of people reported some event. So all but 1.6%. In the NTC-TLA4 group alone, it was 96.7%,
Starting point is 02:21:12 so only 3.1% did not. But in the placebo group, it's 97%. So it's more than to keep in mind, everybody's having some adverse effect. Okay, well, what if you say, well, let's just limit it to the most severe events? Well, let's just talk about grade four toxicities. There were 6.1% of those in the placebo group, 8.4% in the anti-CTLA-4 group, and 6.8% in the combined group. So, not a huge difference in grade four toxicity. Meaning that whatever adverse events are occurring
Starting point is 02:21:44 may not be related to the treatment. occurring may not be related to the treatment. They may not be related to the treatment. If you think about it and it's a very awful, sad, morbid thought to imagine, you're looking at the adverse responses of people more than 80% of whom died during the course of a very, very short study. And so it's very difficult to disentangle what effects or what side effects a person is having just from that process as they are from the actual treatment.
Starting point is 02:22:09 But if there is an area where there's a really clear difference, it's down in the autoimmune category. So if you look at any immune related events, you can see that in the can see that in the anti-CTLA4 plus GP100 group, it's about 60% in both of those treatment groups versus 30%. And if you look at the grade 3 and 4 toxicities, it's 10% in the anti-CTLA4, 15% in the anti-CTLA4 alone group, and only 3% in the treatment. So that's a real difference. But it makes sense that people getting this drug plus placebo or just the drug would have autoimmune issues because this is an immunotherapy.
Starting point is 02:22:55 It's an immunomodulator. In fact, what is it doing? It is taking the breaks off the immune system. But then again, the things that they list out, Pruridus, is that an irritation of the skin? Yeah, irritation of the skin. I'm not a physician, but I know that any idus is going to be like an inflammation. And Oma, unfortunately, likely a cancer or cell replication.
Starting point is 02:23:15 Look at the gastrointestinal differences. And the vitiligo, so 3.7%, 2.3%, 0.8%. The GI stuff is the most common stuff you're going to see there. Those are the really big ones. And of course there's diarrhea and there's diarrhea. Oh yeah. Like there's traveler's diarrhea, there's ate an overly spicy large meal the night before diarrhea and then there's like can't really do anything besides make trips back and forth to the bathroom diarrhea. Put it this way, there's colitis here is diarrhea so significant these patients require IV fluids.
Starting point is 02:23:45 Now, what you don't see here is how many of these patients actually required corticosteroids to reverse the autoimmunity. So a lot of times what will happen here in these studies or with these drugs is the autoimmunity becomes so significant that you have to stop the drug and give corticosteroids. Do the exact opposite. You now have to shut the immune system down. So you just took the brakes off it with the drug And now you need to shut it down with corticosteroids when I was in my fellowship. I
Starting point is 02:24:11 wrote a paper about autoimmunity correlating with response rate in anti-ctLA4 Early on this was during the phase 2 work. So the NCI was a very early adopter of participating in these trials. And it was observed that, or at least hypothesized, this is what the paper basically wrote about, which was, is there any correlation between autoimmunity and response? And it turned out the answer was yes. There was a very strong correlation.
Starting point is 02:24:41 So there was no difference in autoimmunity between the doses. So the paper we wrote was two dosing schedules. So it was basically the full dose, the three milligrams per kilogram versus a low dose, one milligram per kilogram. This is a phase two trial. Those are your two arms. There turned out to be no difference in autoimmunity between them, but there was a big difference between the response rate that tied to auto-immunity. In other words, auto-immunity predicted response. Now I think over time these investigators, the doctors who administer these treatments are getting better and better at catching these things earlier because these auto-immune
Starting point is 02:25:20 conditions can actually be devastating. So on a very personal note, when Kytruda came out, I want to say it was around 2000, 2013, 2014, thereabouts, again, it was for treatment of metastatic melanoma. I want to come back and explain why melanoma gets all of the attention in immunotherapy conditions. I'll state that. But anyway, a friend of mine got pancreatic cancer and he got the bad type of pancreatic cancer. So this is adenocarcinoma in the pancreas. This is a non-survivable type of cancer. Furthermore, his was unresectable.
Starting point is 02:25:56 Can you explain what that is for people? About 20% of people who have pancreatic cancer technically have it in a way where you could still take out the head of the pancreas. Right, the Whipple procedure. The Whipple procedure. Now, tragically, most of those patients will still recur. My understanding is that pancreatic cancer
Starting point is 02:26:14 progresses from anterior to posterior in the pancreas, and that the Whipple is a removal of the front end of the anterior. That's the Whipple procedure. So if the cancer has progressed far enough caudal into the posterior pancreas, then there's nothing left to cut out basically. Can we survive without a pancreas for any amount of time? Oh yeah, absolutely.
Starting point is 02:26:34 So why don't they just remove the whole pancreas? Oh, that's my point. It's already micromitastasized. The surgical procedure is not the challenge anymore. It used to be. So at Johns Hopkins, which was one of the hospitals where this was pioneered, the 30 day mortality for a Whipple procedure was, I don't know, 80%. Whoa. And the reason was to figure out how to suture a pancreas to the bowel. So the pancreas is such an awful organ to operate on because its enzymes are
Starting point is 02:27:03 designed to digest anything and everything. So imagine now you have to cut the pancreas in half, take out the head of the pancreas with the duodenum, and then somehow sew that open half of a raw pancreas to the end of the jejunum and not let it digest itself. So when did Hopkins figure this out? No, the first one was actually done by A.O AO Whipple, but yes, at Hopkins is where they figured out the way to put drains in, the surgical technique, how to do it in two layers, what type of stitches to use.
Starting point is 02:27:37 All of the nuances of this were worked out in a few places, but I would say Hopkins more than any place else. And are there physicians who like try this on non-human primates or something? Or is this always just done on patients? Well, nowadays, I mean, put it this way, even 25 years ago at a major center like Hopkins, the mortality of that procedure was less than 1%. So there have been some victories. Well, yes, but here's my point. That's no longer the bottleneck. Taking out the pancreas safely,
Starting point is 02:28:04 as complicated and challenging as that is, and if you need a whip-up procedure, you only want to have it done by someone who just does that night and day. You don't want weekend warriors doing it. That's not why people are living or dying. They're dying because the cancer just comes back. It was already spread to the liver by the time you did it,
Starting point is 02:28:21 you just didn't realize it yet. So whether you took out the whole pancreas, or the head out the whole pancreas or the head of the pancreas or the tail of the pancreas, the location of the tumor is predictive of survival only in the extent that it basically is a window into how soon did symptoms occur. So pancreatic cancers in the tail tend to be more fatal even though they're way easier surgically to take out because by the time you develop symptoms of a tail, pancreas cancer, it's a big cancer. I was going to ask this question later, but I'll just ask it now. Given the link between the immune system and these cancers, is there an idea in mind that people who are, let's say, 40 and older or 50 and older who
Starting point is 02:29:07 don't yet, they're not diagnosed with any cancer, would periodically just stimulate their immune system to wipe out whatever early cancers might be cropping up? Just take a drug to just ramp up the immune system, even to the point where you start having a little diarrhea, maybe a few skin rashes, and then come off the drug, just basically to fight back whatever little cell growths are starting to take place in skin or liver for three weeks out of each year. I mean, why not? Yeah, it's an interesting question.
Starting point is 02:29:38 I've never thought of it through that lens. I suppose the question is, what can we do to keep our immune systems as healthy as possible as we age? Stay on a normal circadian schedule. There's evidence for that. Sure. No, there's evidence that certainly if it promotes sleep, anything that promotes better rest is going to promote immune health. Because if you ask the macro question, which is like, why does the prevalence of cancer increase so dramatically with age? There are certain diseases where it's really obvious why the prevalence of the disease dramatically with age. There are certain diseases where it's really obvious why the prevalence of the disease
Starting point is 02:30:07 increases with age. Yeah, like age-related macular degeneration. Sure. Or cardiovascular disease is by far the most obvious because it's an area under the curve exposure problem. The more exposure to lipoproteins and the more the endothelium gets damaged, the more likely you are to accumulate plaque. And again, it totally makes sense why
Starting point is 02:30:25 10 year olds don't have heart attacks and 80 year olds do. But when you sort of acknowledge that, well, hey, anybody's accumulating genetic mutations, we're always surrounded and being bombarded by things that are altering the genome of ourselves. Is it simply a stochastic process where the longer you live, the more of these mutations you're going to occur until at some point one of them just wins. I think that's got to be a big part of it. But I think another part of it, clearly I'm not alone in thinking this, is that our immune system is getting weaker and weaker as we age. People become more susceptible to infections as they get older. And I think that that's equally playing a role in our susceptibility to cancer. So yeah, I think the question is,
Starting point is 02:31:05 how do you modulate immunity as you age? And to me, that's one of the most interesting things about rapamycin potentially is that when taken the right way, it seems to enhance cellular immunity, which again, that's potentially a really big deal. Again, at least in short-term human experiments, in response to vaccination, it's enhancing vaccine response. So the question is, would that translate into cancer? Nobody knows. Could that be one of the reasons why animals treated with rapamycin live longer and get less cancer? Don't know.
Starting point is 02:31:36 You know, it could also be that it's at a fundamental level that's targeting nutrient sensing. Where I was going with that story was that... And maybe I'll back up for a moment, why melanoma? So we didn't really know this 30, 40 years ago in the early days of immunotherapy. But what we know now is that most cancers probably have about 40 mutations in them. That's like ballpark, 40, 50 mutations is standard fare for a cancer. But melanoma happens to be one of the cancers that has many, many more mutations. And the more mutations a cancer has, the more likelihood that it will produce an antigen that's recognized as non-self.
Starting point is 02:32:18 And that's why in the early days of immunotherapy, the only things that worked were IL-2 against metastatic melanoma and kidney cancer, because kidney cancer turned out to also be one of those cancers that, for reasons that are not clear, produced hundreds of mutations. And so it's no surprise that the early studies of checkpoint inhibitors were also done in metastatic melanoma, where you basically have more shots on goal. Again, if I'm going to take the brakes off my immune system, I might as well do it in an environment
Starting point is 02:32:49 where there are more chances for my T cells to find something to go nuts against. It's 2013, 2014, and this friend of mine who has something called Lynch syndrome, which is one of those few hereditary or germline mutations that results in a huge increase in the risk of cancer. He had already had colon cancer at about the age of 40 and had survived that. It was a stage 3 cancer but he had
Starting point is 02:33:14 survived it. Well, now five years later had developed pancreatic cancer and when he went to see the surgeon they said, there's nothing we can do. It's too advanced. To put that in perspective, that is a death sentence. That's a six month survival. And at around that time, there was a study that had come out in the New England Journal of Medicine that had talked about how patients with Lynch syndrome had lots of mutations.
Starting point is 02:33:41 So we talked with his doctors about the possibility of enrolling him in one of the Kytruda trials. There was one going on, I think, at Stanford. The thinking being, well, you would want to target a checkpoint inhibitor against somebody who has a lot of mutations. And even though typically we don't see that in pancreatic cancer, his is a unique variant of it because it's based on this. And so sure enough, he was tested for these mismatch repair genes. He had them enrolled in the trial and amazingly had not only a complete regression of his cancer and he's still alive and cancer-free today 10 years later.
Starting point is 02:34:16 But the treatment worked so well at activating his immune system that his immune system completely destroyed his pancreas. So now, he is effectively had a pancreatectomy based on his immune system. So now he actually has type 1 diabetes. He has no pancreas. What? Jackson's insulin to deal with that or implant. No, no. He has to use insulin just like someone with type 1 diabetes. Had to pick being alive with type 1 diabetes.
Starting point is 02:34:41 Yeah, of course. No comparison. But it's just an interesting example of how remarkable this treatment was able to work. You could completely unleash the immune system of a person and you eradicate the cancer and the rest of the cells around it. There are many organs we could live without. There are certain organs you can't live without. You can't live without your heart, lungs, liver, kidneys. But many things that kill people arise from organs, the breast, you could live without all breast tissue. Prostate, you can live without all the prostate tissue. I mean, would you choose to live without these?
Starting point is 02:35:14 Right, but I'm saying if you had metastatic cancer and you had a bullet that could selectively target a tissue, you would take it. And right now, the only tissue we can do that against is a CD19 B cell. And that's what those CAR T cells are. So right now these are not tissue specific treatments, but they're mutation specific.
Starting point is 02:35:34 The last thing I'll say about this paper that I found interesting, I was looking for it and I was surprised they didn't at all comment on if there was any correlation between auto-immunity and response. So they obviously acknowledged the auto-immunity in table three, but I would have loved to have seen a statistical analysis that said, hey, is there any correlation between response rate and autoimmunity? But they didn't comment to that effect. So we're
Starting point is 02:35:58 left wondering what the current state of that is. And I guess in summary I'll say that the reason I thought this was an interesting paper to present is that I still believe that immunotherapy is probably the most important hope we have for treating cancer. And while I think we're still only scratching the surface of it, collectively, the overall survival increase for patients with metastatic solid organ tumors is about 8% better than it was 50 years ago. And virtually all of that has come from some form of immunotherapy, I think is promising. And I think the holy grail, meaning the next step, if you go back to where we started the discussion, is coming up with ways to engineer
Starting point is 02:36:48 T cells to be even better recognizers of antigens. And there's many ways to do that. One is to directly engineer them. Another is to find T cells that have already migrated into tumors. Those are called tumor infiltrating lymphocytes or TIL and expanding those and engineering them to be better and younger. Is it possible to engineer our own T cells to be more pH variant tolerant? Meaning, since this cloaking of the local area by changing the pH, could we pull some T cells? I'm always saying about the inoculation stuff, like pull some T cells as part of our standard exam
Starting point is 02:37:28 when we're 30 and grow some up in an environment that the pH is slightly more acidic than normal and then reintroduce them to the body. I mean, after all they are T cells, in other words, give them a little opportunity to evolve the conditions they can thrive in. Or even just keep them in the freezer in case we need them. Yes.
Starting point is 02:37:50 So, the interesting thing is I don't know that if you just got them to be comfortable in a lower pH, it would be sufficient because there are still so many other things that the cancer is doing as far as using other secreting factors, it seems that by far the most potent thing comes down to expanding the number of T cells that recognize the antigen and making sure that you can get that number big enough without aging them too much. So in some senses, it has become a longevity problem of T cells. The way to think about it is you want an army of soldiers who are
Starting point is 02:38:30 wise enough to recognize the bad guys which comes with age but young enough to go and kill and right now both extremes seem to be unhelpful. When you go and find tumor infiltrating lymphocytes in a tumor, they're very wise. They've demonstrated that they can do everything. They can outmaneuver the cancer, but they're too old to do anything about it. And when you take them out to try to expand them by three logs, which is typically what you need to do, expand them by a thousand fold, they can't do anything. Darrell Bock And what about avoiding melanoma altogether? I mean, obviously avoiding sunburn. Somehow
Starting point is 02:39:05 I got couched as anti-sunscreen and that is absolutely not true. I said some sunscreens contain things that are endocrine disruptors and we're going to do a whole episode on sunscreen. Maybe we could do some journal clubs on them. I'm actually planning something on that as well. I wanted to do a deep dive on this. And some dermatologists reached out, some very skilled dermatologists reached out and said that indeed some sunscreens are downright dangerous, but of course melanoma is super dangerous. No one disputes physical barriers for sunscreen.
Starting point is 02:39:34 Everyone agrees that that is unlikely to have endocrine disruption. So physical barriers are undisputed, but aside from limiting sunlight exposure to the skin, what are some other risks for melanoma? I mean, I think that's the biggest one. I do not believe that smoking poses a risk for melanoma. And if it does, it's going to be very small. There are hereditary cases. So one needs to be pretty mindful when taking a family history. And by the way, there are really weird genetic conditions that link melanoma to other cancers, such as pancreatic cancer, by the way. So whenever I'm taking somebody's family history and I hear about somebody that had melanoma to other cancers such as pancreatic cancer, by the way. So, whenever I'm taking somebody's family history and I hear about somebody that had melanoma
Starting point is 02:40:09 and someone that had pancreatic cancer, there's a couple genetic tests we'll look at to see if that's a person that's particularly sensitive from a genetic predisposition. And by the way, I think with melanoma, although it's not completely agreed upon, I think it's less about sun exposure and more about sun burn. And again, I'm sure there's somebody listening to this who will chime in and apply a more nuanced response to that. But there's a fundamental difference between a mountain in the sun, getting sun, making some vitamin D, versus I'm getting scorched and undergoing significant UV damage.
Starting point is 02:40:43 There might also be something to be said for the time in one's life, and I've certainly seen things that suggest that early repeated sunburns would be more of a risk. So I think that's not a controversial point in the sense that who wants to be sunburned, right? So it's like whatever one needs to do to be sunburned, whether it's being mindful of what the UV index is, wearing the appropriate cover, wearing the appropriate sunscreen. I also find the whole anti-sunscreen establishment to be a little bit odd. Well, the anti-sunscreen establishment is odd.
Starting point is 02:41:13 I'm trying to open the door for a nuanced discussion about the fact that some sunscreens really do contain things like oxybenzene and things that are real. And you're spraying them on kids. Yeah, yeah, yeah, but when you just look at the straight, you know, the good old fashioned mineral sunscreens. Yeah, perfectly safe. Yeah. As far as we know, dare we cross the seed oil debate into this. Some of the folks who are really anti seed oil also claim that seed oils increase risk for sunscreen. Peter and I are
Starting point is 02:41:39 smiling because we have teed up a debate soon with some anti-seed oil and less anti-seed oil experts. So that's forthcoming. That's going to be a fun one. We'll be doing all of that with our shirts on. I really appreciate you walking us through this paper, Peter. I have never looked at a paper on cancer and certainly not one like this. I learned a lot and it's such an interesting field obviously because of the importance of getting people with cancer to survive longer and lead better lives, but also because of the interaction with the immune system. So we learned some really important immunology.
Starting point is 02:42:17 Yeah. And this was great. I feel much more confident now in the belief that the exposure to light early and late in the day can actually have benefits. And as I said, I think that there's some causality here and I think it shouldn't be ignored. Well, this was our second journal club. I look forward to our third. Next time you'll go first. We'll just keep alternating.
Starting point is 02:42:37 And we've also switched venues, but we both wore the correct shirt. I hope people are learning and not just learning the information, but learning how to parse and think about papers. And I certainly learned from you, Peter. Thank you so much. Yeah, thanks, Andrew. This was great. Thank you for listening to this week's episode of The Drive. It's extremely important to me to provide all of this content
Starting point is 02:42:58 without relying on paid ads. To do this, our work is made entirely possible by our members. And in return, we offer exclusive member only content and benefits above and beyond what is available for free. So if you want to take your knowledge of this space to the next level, it's our goal to ensure members get back much more than the price of the subscription. Premium membership includes several benefits. First, comprehensive podcast show notes that
Starting point is 02:43:24 detail every topic, paper, person, and thing that we discuss in each episode. And the word on the street is, nobody's show notes rival ours. Second, monthly ask me anything or AMA episodes. These episodes are comprised of detailed responses to subscriber questions typically focused on a single topic and are designed to offer a great deal of clarity and detail on topics of special interest to our members. You'll also get access to the show notes for these episodes, of course. Third, delivery of our premium newsletter, which is put together by our dedicated team of research analysts.
Starting point is 02:43:59 This newsletter covers a wide range of topics related to longevity and provides much more detail than our free weekly newsletter. Fourth, access to our private podcast feed that provides you with access to every episode including AMAs, Sons The Spiel you're listening to now, and in your regular podcast feed. Fifth, the Quallies, an additional member-only podcast we put together that serves as a highlight reel featuring the best excerpts from previous episodes of The Drive. This is a great way to catch up on previous episodes without having to go back and listen to each one of them.
Starting point is 02:44:34 And finally, other benefits that are added along the way. If you want to learn more and access these member-only benefits, you can head over to peteratea.com.com forward slash subscribe. You can also find me on YouTube, Instagram, and Twitter, all with the handle peteratea.com. You can also leave us a review on Apple podcasts or whatever podcast player you use. This podcast is for general informational purposes only and does not constitute the practice of medicine, nursing, or other professional health care services, including the giving of medical advice. No doctor-patient relationship is formed. The use of this information and the materials linked to this podcast is at the user's own
Starting point is 02:45:15 risk. The content on this podcast is not intended to be a substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis, or treatment. Users should not disregard or delay in obtaining medical advice from any medical condition they have and they should seek the assistance of their health care professionals for any such conditions. Finally, I take all conflicts of interest very seriously. For all of my disclosures and the companies I invest in or advise, please visit peteratiamd.com forward slash about where I keep an up to date and active list of all disclosures. you

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.