The Peter Attia Drive - #286 ‒ Journal club with Andrew Huberman: the impact of light exposure on mental health and an immunotherapy breakthrough for cancer treatment
Episode Date: January 22, 2024View the Show Notes Page for This Episode Become a Member to Receive Exclusive Content Sign Up to Receive Peter’s Weekly Newsletter Andrew Huberman, professor of neurobiology at Stanford Universi...ty and host of the Huberman Lab podcast, returns for another special journal club episode. Andrew introduces an observational study investigating the influence of light exposure on circadian clock regulation and its link to mental health, while Peter covers a phase III clinical trial employing immune checkpoint inhibitors for the treatment of metastatic cancer. They delve into the essential findings of their respective papers, elucidate the reasons for their enthusiasm, and tackle potential limitations and unanswered questions. Additionally, they provide valuable insights into their approaches for comprehending research studies, aiding listeners in independently navigating this process. We discuss: The intricate relationship between light exposure, circadian rhythms, and mental health [3:30]; The importance of low solar angle sunlight, and other types of light needed for optimal mental and physical health [12:00]; Promising new lightbulb technology that simulates low solar angle sunlight [17:45]; The significance of both darkness and the need for direct light exposure to the eyes, specifically [20:00]; Some tips and advice regarding optimizing light exposure, blue blockers, and effects on circadian rhythm [22:15]; Andrew presents a paper which suggests avoiding light at night and seeking light during the day is associated with better mental health [25:45]; Examining the data: the negative impact of increasing nighttime light exposure and the positive effects of daytime light exposure [34:30]; Statistical analysis: the importance of focusing not only on statistical significance but also clinical relevance, power analysis, error bar range, and more [45:45]; Takeaways from the study of daytime and nighttime light exposure [49:45]; The practicalities of minimizing light exposure and screen time at night, the use of sleep trackers, and overall challenge of modern, indoor lifestyles [55:15]; Potential limitations of the light exposure study, reverse causality, and the complex interplay of variables in epidemiological studies [1:06:00]; A tangent on diet soda and sugar substitutes as an example of reverse causality [1:13:15]; Andrew and Peter’s take on the causality vs. correlation of light exposure to mental health, the damage of circadian disruption, and the interpretation of observational data [1:17:30]; A primer on the immune system as background for the paper Peter chose [1:25:00]; Background on cancer: causes, how it evades the immune system, and the logic behind immune checkpoint inhibitor therapy [1:35:45]; Peter presents a paper on immune checkpoint inhibitor therapy in cancer patients [1:50:15]; Unpacking the results of the checkpoint inhibitor trial [1:59:45]; Other noteworthy observations, including the differing results between males and females [2:15:30]; Adverse effects resulting from treatment with an immune checkpoint inhibitor targeting CTLA-4 [2:20:00]; Why melanoma is especially responsive to immunotherapy, and the remarkable success story of immunotherapy for pancreatic cancer [2:25:15]; Why immunotherapy may be the most important hope we have for treating cancer [2:35:30]; Avoiding melanoma: the sunscreen debate, sunburn as the biggest risk factor, and more [2:38:45]; and More. Connect With Peter on Twitter, Instagram, Facebook and YouTube
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hey everyone, welcome to the Drive Podcast. I'm your host, Peter Atia. This podcast, my
website, and my weekly newsletter all focus on the goal of translating the science of
longevity into something accessible for everyone. Our goal is to provide the best content in health and wellness, and we've established
a great team of analysts to make this happen.
It is extremely important to me to provide all of this content without relying on paid
ads.
To do this, our work is made entirely possible by our members, and in return, we offer exclusive
member-only content and benefits above and beyond what is available for free.
If you want to take your knowledge of this space to the next level,
it's our goal to ensure members get back much more than the price of a subscription.
If you want to learn more about the benefits of our premium membership,
head over to peteratea.com.com.
Welcome to another special episode of The Drive.
This episode is actually a dual episode with my good friend, Andrew Huberman, where we
are going to be releasing our conversation on both the Huberman Lab podcast and The
Drive.
In this special episode, Andrew and I team up again for another round of Journal Club,
and you may recall this is the second time we've done it, having done it back in September
of 2023.
We enjoy this so much that I suspect we're going to continue to do this, potentially
at the cadence of about once a quarter, but of course, we'll see.
In today's Journal Club, we start by looking at a paper that Andrew highlighted, which
looks at how light exposure and dark exposure can
affect mental health.
After that, I present a paper which is kind of a landmark study on a class of drugs that
I believe are some of the most relevant classes of drugs in cancer therapy over the past 20
years, the so-called checkpoint inhibitors.
The hope here is not only that this conversation gives you insights in the specific papers
that we're discussing, both of which I think are highly fascinating, but equally importantly
that you can learn something about how to read scientific papers, what to look for, and
what the papers say and what's being reported and how that doesn't necessarily match with
what the news is telling you.
That's a really common issue as many of you know, and I certainly rail against this where I'll comment on a paper that the media has picked up on and completely
misrepresented. And again, there's really only one antidote to this, and the antidote
is learning how to read the papers yourself. And unfortunately, there really is no better
way to do that than practice. And so what we really hope is that people will sit with us
and maybe take a look at the papers before they watch the podcast or listen to the podcast
and try to get a sense of what they notice about these papers, what questions arise for them,
and see if we touch on similar topics. As a brief reminder to anyone who's been up in the
Himalayas hunting Yeti for the past six years and doesn't know who Andrew is, he is an associate
professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at the Stanford
University School of Medicine and the host of the very popular Huberman Lab podcast.
He's also a former podcast guest on episodes 249 and 270. So, without further delay, please
enjoy my conversation with Andrew Huberman. Andrew, great to have you here for Journal Club number two. I'm already confident this is going to
become a regular for us.
I'm excited. I really enjoy this because I get to pick papers. I'm really excited about it. I get
to hear papers that you're excited about and we get to sharpen our skills at reading and sharing data.
And people listening can do that as well. So last time I went first, so I think I'm going to put
you on the hot seat first and have you go first and I'll follow you. Okay. Well, I'm really excited
about this paper for a number of reasons. First of all, it, at least by my read, is a very powerful paper in the sense that it examined
light exposure behavior as well as dark exposure behavior. And that's going to be an important
point in more than 85,000 people as part of this cohort in the UK. I'll just mention a couple of
things to give people background and I'll keep this relatively brief. First of all, there's a
long standing interest in the relationship between light and mental
health and physical health.
And we can throw up some very well agreed upon bullet points.
First of all, there is such a thing as seasonal effective disorder.
It doesn't just impact people living at really northern locations.
But basically there's a correlation between day length and mood
and mental health, such that for many people, not all, but for many people, when days are longer in
the spring and summer, they feel better. They report fewer depressive symptoms. And conversely,
when days are shorter, significantly more people report feeling lower mood and affect.
There's a long-standing treatment for seasonal effective disorder, which is to give people
exposure to very bright light, especially in the morning. The way that that's normally accomplished
is with these sad lamps, seasonal effective disorder lamps, and those lamps are basically
bright, meaning more than 10,000 lux lights
that they place on their kitchen counter or at their table in the morning or in their
office. So they're getting a lot of bright light. That has proven to be fairly effective
for the treatment of seasonal effective disorder. What's less understood is how light exposure
in the middle of the night can negatively impact mood and health.
And so where we are headed with this is that there seems to be, based on the conclusions of
this new study, a powerful and independent role of both daytime light exposure and nighttime dark
exposure for mental health. Now a couple of other key points. The biological mechanisms
for all this are really well established. There's a set of cells in the neural retina,
which aligns the back of your eye. They're sometimes called intrinsically photosensitive.
Retinal ganglion cells are sometimes called melanopsin retinal ganglion cells. We'll talk
about those in a bit of detail in a moment. It's well known that those cells are the ones that respond to two different types of light input,
not one, but two different types of light input
and send information to the hypothalamus
where your master circadian clock resides
and then your master circadian clock sends out
secretory signals, so peptides, hormones,
but also neural signals to the brain and body
and say, hey, now it's daytime, now
it's nighttime. Be awake, be asleep. But it goes way beyond that. These melanopsin,
intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells, we know also project areas of the brain
like the habanula, which can trigger negative affect, negative mood. They can trigger the release
of dopamine or the suppression of dopamine, the release of serotonin,
the suppression of serotonin. And so they're not just cells for setting your circadian clock.
They also have a direct line, literally one synapse away into the structures of the brain
that we know powerfully control mood. So the mechanistic basis for all this is there.
So there's just a couple of other key points to understand for people to really be able to digest the data in this paper fully.
There are basically two types of stimuli that these cells respond to.
One is very bright light as we just talked about.
That's why getting a lot of daytime sunlight is correlated with elevated mood.
That's why looking at a 10,000 lux, artificial lamp can offset seasonal effective disorder. By the way, just a couple questions on that. How many lux
does the sun provide on a sunny day at noon? Okay, great question. So if you're
out in the sun with no cloud cover or minimal cloud cover in the middle of the
day at noon, chances are it's over 100,000 lux. On a really bright day, could be 300,000 lux.
Most indoor environments,
even though they might seem very bright,
department store with the bright lights,
believe it or not, that's probably only closer
to 6,000 lux maximum and probably more like 4,000 lux.
Most brightly lit indoor environments are not that
bright when it comes down to total photon energy. Now, here's the interesting thing. On a cloudy day,
when you're outside, it can be as bright as an average of 100,000 lux. But it won't seem that
bright because you don't quote unquote see the sun. But it's also
because when there's cloud cover, a lot of those long wavelengths of light such as orange and red
light aren't coming through. However, and this is so important, the circadian clock, the super
charismatic nucleus, it sums photons, it's a photon summing system. So basically, if you're outside in 8,000 lux,
very overcast UK winter day, and you're walking around, hopefully without sunglasses, because
sunglasses are going to filter a lot of those photons out, your circadian clock is summing
the photons. So it's an integration mechanism. It's not triggered in a moment. And actually,
the experiments of recording from these cells first done by David Berson at Brown were historic
in the field of visual neuroscience when you show bright light on these intrinsically photosensitive
cells, you could crank up the intensity of the light and the neurons would ramp up their
membrane potential and then start spiking, firing action potentials or long
trains of action potentials that have been shown to go on for hours. And so that's this
signal that's propagating into the whole brain and body. So the important thing to understand
is this is not a quick switch. That's why I suggest on non-cloudy days, we'll call
them, that people get 10 minutes or so of sunlight in their eyes in the early part of
the day, another 10 minimum in the later part of 10 minutes or so of sunlight in their eyes in the early part of the day,
another 10 minimum in the later part of the day, as much sunlight in their eyes as they safely can throughout the day.
But since you're a physician and you had a guest on talking about this recently, when the sun is low in the sky, low solar angle sunlight,
that's really the key time for reasons we'll talk about in a moment. And when the sun is low in the sky, you run
very, very little risk of inducing cataract by looking in the general direction when the sun is low in the sky, you run very, very little risk of inducing
cataract by looking in the general direction of the sun. You should still blink as needed
to protect the eyes. It's when the sun is overhead, there's all those photons coming
in quickly in a short period of time. You do have to be concerned about cataract and
macular degeneration if you're getting too much daytime sunlight. So the idea is sunglasses
in the middle of the day are fine, but you really should avoid using them in the early and later part of the day unless
you're driving into the sun for safety reasons.
Another question, Andrew. If a person is indoors, but they have large windows, they're getting
tons of sunlight into their space. They don't even need ambient indoor light. How much of
the photons are making it through the glass and how does
that compare to this effect?
In general, unless the light is coming directly through the window, most of the relevant wavelengths
are filtered out.
In other words, if you can't see the sun through the window, even if sufficient light is being
provided, that's insufficient to trigger this phenomenon?
That's right. However, if you have windows on your roof, which some people do,
skylights, that makes the situation much, much better. In fact, the neurons in the eye that
signal to the circadian clock and these mood centers in the brain reside mainly in the
bottom two thirds of the neural retina and are responsible for looking up. Basically,
they're gathering light from above. These cells
are also very low resolution. Think of them as big pixels. They're not interested in patterns
and edges and movement. They're interested in how much ambient light there happens to be. Now,
keep in mind that this mechanism is perhaps the most well-conserved mechanism in cellular organisms.
And I'll use it as a way to frame up the four types of light
that one needs to see every 24 hours for optimal health. And when I say optimal health, I really
mean mental health and physical health, but we're going to talk about mental health mainly
today in this paper. There's an absolutely beautiful evolutionary story whereby single
cell organisms all the way to humans, Dogs, rabbits and everything in between have
at least two cone options. One that responds to short wavelength light, aka blue light,
and another one that responds to longer wavelength light, orange and red. So your dogs have this,
we have this, and it's a comparison mechanism in these cells of the eye, these neurons of
the eye. They compare contrast between blues and orange or sometimes blues and reds and pinks, which
are also all long wavelength light.
There are two times a day when the sky is enriched with blues, oranges, pinks, and reds.
And that's low solar angle sunlight at sunrise and in the evening. These cells are uniquely available to trigger the
existence of those wavelengths of light early in the day and in the evening, not in the
middle of the day. So these cells have these two cone photopigments and they say, how much
blue light is there? How much red light is there or orange light? And the subtraction
between those two triggers the signal for them to fire the signal off to the circadian clock of the brain and that's why I say look at
low solar angle sunlight early in the day. What that does is it what causes phase
advances the clock. This can get a little technical and we don't want to get too
technical here but think about pushing your kid on a swing. The period of that
swing, the duration of that swing is a little bit longer than 12
hours.
So when you stand closer to the kid, so your kid swings back and you give it a push, you're
shortening the period.
You're not allowing the swing to come all the way up.
That's what happens when you look at morning sunlight.
You're advancing your circadian clock, translate to English or non-nerd speak.
You're making it such that you will want to go
to bed a little bit earlier and wake up a little bit earlier the next day. In the evening, when you
view low solar angle sunlight, the afternoon setting sun or evening setting sun, you do the exact
opposite. You're face delaying the clock. It's the equivalent of your kid being at the very top of
the arc. And so it's gone, you know, maybe let's say 12 and a half hours is the duration of that swing
and you run up and you push them from behind and give them a little more push. That's the
equivalent of making yourself stay up a little later and wake up a little later. These two
signals average so that your clock stays stable. You don't drift, meaning you're not waking
up earlier every single day or going to sleep later every single day. This is why it's important to view low-cell angle sunlight in the morning
and again in the evening as often as possible. And it's done by that readout of those two photo
pigments. Now midday sun, it's bright light, but you see it as white light contains all of those
wavelengths that equal intensity.
So the middle of the day is the so-called circadian dead zone.
In the middle of the day, bright light triggers the activation of the other ops and the melanopsin,
which increases mood, increases feelings of well-being, has some other consequences,
but you can't shift your circadian clock by viewing the sun in the middle of the day because
it's in the circadian dead zone.
It's the equivalent of pushing your kid on the swing when they're at the bottom of the arc.
You can get a little bit more but not much and in biological terms you get nothing.
So this is why looking at sunlight in the middle of the day is great but it's not going to help
anchor your sleep wake cycle. And if you think about it, this is incredible, right? Every organism
from single cells to us has this mechanism to know when the sun is rising
and when the sun is setting, and it's a color comparison mechanism, which tells us that
actually color vision evolved first, not for pattern vision, not for seeing beautiful
sunsets and recognizing that's beautiful or paintings or things of that sort, but rather
for setting the circadian clock.
Now, what if you only do one of these enders?
So what if you've got constant exposure to low morning light, but your job prevents you
from doing the same in the evening or vice versa?
Better to get the morning light because if you have to pick between low solar angle light
earlier later in the day.
And keep in mind, if you miss a day, no big deal.
It's a slow integrative mechanism averaging across the previous two or three days. But if you
miss a day, you'll want to get twice as much light in your eyes that next morning. The reason
it's better to do in the morning as opposed to the evening, although best would be to do both,
is that most people are getting some artificial light exposure in the evening anyway. And here's
the diabolical thing. Your retina is very insensitive to light exposure in the evening anyway. And here's the diabolical thing.
Your retina is very insensitive to light early in the day.
You need a lot of photons to trigger this mechanism early in the day.
As the day goes on, retinal sensitivity increases and it takes very little light to shift your
circadian clock late in the day.
Keep in mind also that if you do see afternoon and evening sunlight, there's a beautiful
study published in Science Reports two years ago showing that that can partially offset the
negative effects of artificial light exposure at night.
I think of this as your Netflix inoculation.
The amount of melatonin suppression from nighttime light exposure is halved by viewing
evening setting sun.
Now keep in mind you don't need to see the sun cross the horizon, it can just be when it's low solar angle.
So you're looking for those yellow, blue, or blue, pink,
blue, red contrasts.
And on cloudy days, believe it or not, they're still there,
just you don't perceive as much of it coming through.
So that's three things that we should all strive to do.
View low solar angle sunlight early in the day,
view solar angle sunlight later in the day,
and get as much bright light in our eyes
As we safely can ideally from sunlight throughout the day
And if you can't do that perhaps invest in one of these sad lights so that they can be a bit expensive
There are a couple companies that are starting to design sunrise
Simulators and evening simulators that are actually good that actually work
But right now my read is that aside from one company
out there, which by the way I have no relationship to, it's called the 2O Light T.U.O. And that
light bulb was developed by the biologist at the University of Washington who basically discovered
these color opponent mechanisms. Those lights are not particularly expensive, but they do seem to
work. In fact, the study that is emerging,
again, unpublished data seems to indicate that if you look at it for more than five or six minutes,
it can induce a mild euphoria. That's how powerful this contrast is. And what they did there in
that light, I'll just tell you the mechanism is they figured out that when most people look at
low solar angle sunlight in the morning, they're getting 19 reversals of blue orange per second.
So when you look at this light, it looks like a barely flashing white light,
but it's reversals of orange and blue, orange and red and blue, and it's happening very fast.
What is the person looking at it perceive?
Well, I've used one of these, just looks like a flickering light,
and of course there's always the potential placebo effect.
That's what I was going to say.
Is there a way to control for that by having something that looks the same to the user,
but of course, is not producing the same photo effect?
Yeah, well, they've done that with the 10,000 lux sad lamps, which most people use to try
and induce sunrise simulation in their home.
But keep in mind that sunrise gives you this comparison of short and long
wavelength light. Just a bright 10,000 lux light triggers one of the options, but it
won't set your circadian clock. So most of the sad lamps that are out there are activating
only one of the mechanisms in these cells that's relevant and not the one that's most
relevant. So I'm excited about what 2.0 is doing. I think that, and, I have no relation to them, except I know the biologists who do the work that provide
the mechanistic logic for that engineering. I still think we're in the really early days of
this stuff. What should be done is to have this stuff built into your laptop. It should be built
into your phone, and hopefully it will be. Now, I mentioned this color contrast thing in sunrise and sunset.
I mentioned the bright light throughout the day, but there's a fourth light stimulus that
turns out to be really important and this will provide the segue into the paper. Turns
out that dark exposure at night, independent of light exposure during the day, is important
for mental health outcomes. Most people think dark exposure. How do I think about that?
Absence of light exposure. It's the absence of light, but what this paper really drives home is that people who make it a
point to get dark exposure at night, aka the absence of light at night, actually benefit even if
they're not getting enough sunlight during the day. And this is especially true for people with
certain mental health issues. So I don't think we can overstate the value of accurately
timed light exposure to the eyes in the context of mental health. I think there's so much data by
now. I will say, however, that some people seem more resilient to these light effects than others,
meaning some people also don't suffer from jet lag too much. Some people can stay up late,
get a lot of bright light exposure in the middle of the
night and during the day, they've got their sunglasses on all day and they're in a great
mood all the time.
Other people are more susceptible to these sorts of things and we don't know whether
or not polymorphisms underlie that.
I personally am very sensitive to sunlight in the sense that if I don't get enough sunlight,
I don't feel well after a couple of days, but I'm less sensitive to light exposure
at night, for instance. But I think it is perhaps, this is a big statement, but is perhaps the most
fundamental environmental stimulus for levels of arousal and alertness, which correlate with all
sorts of neuromodulator and hormone outputs. None of this should come as any surprise. I will
mention one last thing.
There was a study published gosh over 10 years ago now
from Chuck Zeissers Lab at Harvard Medical School.
It's a phenomenal lab exploring circadian
human health behavior.
He's just considered a nopun,
luminary in the field.
But there was a study that was in error
where they had published in Science Magazine
that light shown behind the knee could shift circadian rhythms and that paper was retracted.
And a lot of people don't know that it was retracted light exposure to the eyes is what's relevant here.
As far as we know the color of one's eyes darkness or lightness of one's eyes bears no relevance on their sensitivity to these types of mechanisms.
to these types of mechanisms. So one question, one comment.
The question again is going back to the morning, evening, light.
And I spend a lot of time looking at those types of skies, for example,
just because of the nature of my hobbies,
because I'm always doing archery in the morning and rucking in the afternoon.
So it's not uncommon that I'm seeing both of those.
How relevant is it that the sun be above the horizon?
So for example, it begins to get light about
in 30 minutes before sunrise. So if sunrise is at 730, first light is 7 and then 715 to
730 is actually quite bright. I mean, you can see anything and everything and the same
is true at sunset. So does that 30 minutes when Sun is beneath the horizon constitute
part of that 10 minutes?
It does.
In an ideal circumstance, you'd get outside
and see the sunrise every day
and you'd see the sunset every day, even on cloudy days.
Some people like myself wake up before the sun comes up
and I get this question all the time.
Well, in the absence of powers to make the sunrise faster,
which I'm not aware anyone has, certainly not me,
I think the best thing to do is simply to turn on
as many bright lights as you can indoors
to trigger that melanopsin mechanism.
If you wanna be awake, if you wanna stay asleep or sleepy,
then keep them dim and then get outside
once the sun is starting to come out.
Some people wake up after the sun has risen,
in which case get what you can.
And some people wake up 10 a.m. or noon, in which case you can you can. And some people wake up 10am or noon, in which case you
can still get the bright light exposure but you won't shift your circadian clock. Now in the evening,
especially in the winter months, it's important to look west and try and get some sunlight in
your eyes in the evening. If you've ever gone into the clinic, for instance, at two o'clock in the
afternoon after lunch, and then in the winter and then come out
and it's dark when you're walking to your car. It's an eerie feeling. That sort of eerie feeling
may correlate with the fact that you missed a signal. Your brain is trying to orient your brain
and body in time and that's what all of this is. It's trying to orient in time and again,
some people are more susceptible to that than others. Some people might like that feeling of,
oh, I went in when it was bright and I come
out when it's dark.
But the vast majority of people feel better when they're getting this morning and evening
sunlight exposure.
And this is especially important in kids.
This is one of the things that this paper points out and there are good data that people
are spending approximately 90% of their time indoors nowadays.
Daytime, time indoors.
And those indoor environments are simply not bright enough.
You think, oh, there's all these bright lights
and some people are putting blue blockers on
in the middle of the day,
which is the worst thing you could possibly do.
If you're gonna wear blue blockers
and I don't think they're necessary,
but if you're going to wear them,
you'd wanna wear them at night.
And in the evening, you don't need to wear blue blockers,
you just simply should dim the lights and ideally have lights that are set a little
bit lower in your environment, which the Scandinavians have been doing for a long time. So kill
the overhead lights and don't obsess about bright light exposure in the middle of the
night. In fact, for a long time, I and some other people were saying, oh, you know, even
just a brief flash of light in the middle of the night can quash your melatonin.
That's true.
But the other time in which you're in this quote unquote circadian dead zone is in the
middle of the night.
You can't shift your circadian clock in the middle of the night.
But all of this gets down to interweaving rhythms of light sensitivity, temperature,
hormone output, cortisol.
I mean, there's a whole landscape of circadian biology.
This paper, which was published in a new journal I'm really excited about called Nature Mental
Health.
This journal was just launched recently, is entitled Day and Night Light Exposure Are
Associated with Psychiatric Disorders and Objective Light Study in More Than 85,000
People.
Now, I have to say that I think the title of this paper
is terrible, sorry folks at Nature Mental Health,
because if one just read the title,
it sounds like day and night light exposure
associated with psychiatric disorders.
This sort of newspaper headline, you'd be like,
oh my goodness, what are you supposed to do?
But that's not the conclusion.
The conclusion is that getting a lot of sunlight exposure during the day and getting
a lot of dark exposure at night is immensely beneficial for psychiatric health in a number of
ways. Now, I'm not one to bring up another paper unannounced, but I will say that this paper built
off a previous study entitled, Time Spent in Outdoor Light is Associate with Mood Sleep and
Circadian Rhythm-Related Outcomes. And that was a cross-sectional longitudinal study in 400,000 entitled time spent in outdoor light is associated with mood, sleep and circadian rhythm related outcomes.
And that was a cross-sectional longitudinal study in 400,000 biobank participants.
So this UK biobank is an incredibly valuable resource and there are now multiple studies establishing
that one's pattern of light exposure is extremely important.
Now the previous study in 400,000 participants basically nailed home the idea
that the more time you spend outdoors, the better your is your mood, the better is your sleep,
the better is the rhythmicity of your sleep wake cycles and on and on. Something that I think,
even though people say we've known that for thousands of years, needed scientific substantiation.
thousands of years needed scientific substantiation. This new study essentially looked at the relative contributions of daytime light exposure and
nighttime dark exposure.
And they did that on a background looking in particular people who had major depressive
disorder, generalized anxiety, PTSD, bipolar disorder.
Here's the basic takeaway.
I'll quote them here.
I'll tell you my interpretation. Here I'm quoting,
avoiding night at light and seeking light during the day. I love that word seeking.
Maybe a simple and effective non-pharmacologic means for broadly improving mental health.
So that's a pretty bold statement. And I love that they say seeking because it implies that people
aren't reflexively getting the light exposure that they need, that this needs to be a practice,
much like zone 2 cardio or resistance training.
So what do they do in this study?
So basically, they gathered up 100,000 people or so, it eventually was pared down to about
86,000 participants because some just didn't qualify or didn't report their data back.
They equipped them with accelerometers on their
wrist and those wrist devices also could measure ambient light. Now that's not a perfect tool
because what you'd love to do is measure ambient light at the level of the eyes. By the way,
will somebody design an eyeglass frame that changes color when you've gotten sufficient
light from sunlight during the day and then at night is a different color and then if you're
getting too much light exposure,
we'll go to a different color frame.
This has to be possible so that you don't have to wonder
if you got enough light during the day.
And of course, if it's at the level of the eyes,
then that's what's landing at the eyes.
So I was going to ask you about that.
Do these wrist-based devices potentially
get covered by clothing?
And some like, you have your sleeves down.
I have my sleeves up.
They had it on the outside of the sleeve, but they asked that people just keep it on
their dominant hand.
It's not perfect.
But in some ways, it's kind of nice that it's not perfect.
We could turn that disadvantage into advantage by thinking when the person is out and about,
they're not often looking right at the sun.
If you're talking to a colleague under an overhang, for instance.
So it's not.
It's directionally. it's not... Perfect.
...directionally.
It's directionally right. Okay. And then they had two hypotheses, two primary hypotheses.
One that greater light exposure in the day is associated with lower risk for psychiatric
disorders and two second hypotheses, greater light exposure at night is associated with
higher risk for psychiatric disorders in poorer mood. This is oh so relevant for the way we
live now, people on screens and tablets in the middle of the night. Then they collected information about how much
light exposure people were getting, as well as their sleep and their activity and so on.
As you mentioned, this was done in males and females. It was a slightly older cohort than
one is used to seeing people in their 50s and 60s. They had psychiatric diagnosis information.
And then they divided people into essentially
two groups, but they had a lower, so a Q1 and a Q2, a lower quartile.
That meant people that were getting less daytime light as opposed to the third and fourth quartile,
more daytime light.
They also had a nighttime light exposure evaluation and they had people with the low Q1 and Q2 so these people are getting less
nighttime light versus Q3, Q4 more nighttime light.
Nicely, they also looked at sleep duration and they looked at photo period, meaning how
long the days were for those individuals, how active they were.
Look, 10 hours a day, 14 hours a day because the more active you are, the more opportunity
for light exposure you have during the day or night, for instance.
Okay.
So they had, I would say, fairly complete datasets.
And I'm just going to hit the top contour of what they did in each…
And sorry, sleep duration, sleep efficiency, et cetera, was determined off the accelerometer?
That's right, as well as self-report.
Not ideal, right?
You'd love for people to be wearing a woot band or a ring or something of that sort, but this was initiated some
time ago, so they either didn't have access to that technology or for whatever reason didn't
select it. Then what they did is they have information on who has major depressive disorder,
who has PTSD, generalized anxiety, bipolar, psychosis, etc. And then they ran
three models. You can tell me what you think about the power of these models, but as somebody
who thinks about the mechanistic aspect of all of this a lot, but not somebody who's
ever run this type of study, I'd be really curious.
Model one examined the unadjusted association between day and nighttime light exposure and psychiatric
outcomes. So just basically asking, is there a relationship between how much light you get
during the day and how much light you get at night and how bad your depression is or anxiety is,
etc. Looking at just a standard ratio of the probability that you have a certain symptom
or set of symptoms versus you don't given
a certain amount of light exposure. Model two, adjusted for the age of the person, their
sex and ethnicity and photo period. So they looked at how long the days were in that given
person's region of the world.
Marshall Spicer These people were all in the UK or were
they around the world?
Stan Mallow They were all in the UK as far as I know.
And then model three 3 adjusted for employment.
So employed versus unemployed, which if you think about it
is pretty important.
Like you say, well, an unemployed person has a lot more time
to control these variables, but an employed person
who's doing shift work does not.
They incorporated information about employed versus
unemployed physical activity, which turns out to be
very important, and then things like shift work, etc. We can say very safely, the outcomes with each of these models, the
results were very similar. So we don't want to discard the differences between those models
entirely. But in my read is in every figure of the paper, it doesn't seem like model one,
two or three differ from one another in terms of total outcome.
That's an unusual aspect of this paper.
So these adjustments are very standard.
This is a classic tool that's used in most epidemiology because you don't have randomization.
So once randomization is out the window, so for example, the paper I'm going to present
is based on an RCT, there will be no models.
It's just here the data.
Yeah, here they're asking people, what do
you do? Report back to us. We're going to measure your light exposure, but no one was
assigned to any groups or swap. Whatever quote unquote controls are there. They were really
not there. It's just comparisons between groups.
So what is interesting to me is that it's exactly as you said, and we'll make all these
figures available in addition to the papers, but it's very unusual that there's no difference between the unadjusted and the adjusted models and as you say there's
Probably two places out of 30 when you look at all the different quartile comparisons
where you might creep from
Statistically significant just out of it or just into it. But yeah,
you could simplify this figure two completely by just showing one of the models and you would be
getting 95% of the information. I think in one way that suggests that there's less dependency
on those variables. Of course, it still doesn't address probably the greatest question I have here,
which I'm sure we'll get to at some point as you continue.
I'm very curious what that question is. I'll suppress my curiosity for the moment.
So if we look at Figure 2 of this paper and I realize a lot of people are listening and they're
not able to look at this, although we have posted the figures on the YouTube version of this,
just want to make clear what's going on just for those
that are listening.
Essentially what they're looking at is what they call the odds ratio, which is the probability
of something happening in one group divided by the probability of something happening
in another group.
I guess by way of example, if you were going to look at the odds ratio of the probability
of somebody getting lung cancer if they smoke versus probability of somebody getting lung
cancer if they don't smoke.
So, odds ratios and hazard ratios are often confused.
They're very similar and odds ratios generally refer to a lifetime exposure whereas a hazard
ratio is defined over a specific period of time.
But the math is still effectively the same.
And using the example you gave, if you took the odds ratio of death, so let's
talk all-cause mortality for a smoker versus a non-smoker, and the answer were 1.78, I'm
making that up, but that's directionally correct. 1.78 as an odds ratio means there's a 78%
chance greater of the outcome of interest, in this case death by any cause, in the affected group, which would be the smokers.
So odds ratio of two is 100%,
and odds ratio of three is 200%.
So the math is take the number, subtract one,
and that's the percent.
Thank you for that.
Right, so figure two of this paper
is one of the key take-homes.
Essentially look at the odds ratio
of people who are in the,
let's just look at the odds ratio of people who are in the, let's just look
at the nighttime light exposure.
And just remind me, Andrew, and everybody else watching, every one of these is showing
second, third, fourth as your X-axis, meaning they're all being compared to the first quartile.
That's right.
And the first quartile is lowest light exposure or highest light exposure.
Lowest.
Well. With the differentiate between day and night.
That's right. That's right.
Restate it.
Sure. So if we look at what is your risk of a psychiatric challenge, broadly speaking,
well, panel A is major depressive disorder. If you are in the second quartile, third quartile,
or fourth quartile of nighttime light exposure. So second being the least amount of nighttime light exposure,
third being more nighttime light exposure and fourth,
the most nighttime light exposure relative to the first quartile.
This is just a stupid thing.
Like if I were doing this figure, if you were doing this in a lecture,
you know what you would do to make it so easy?
You would draw arrows on it that say increasing light exposure at night, decreasing light
exposure in the day.
It's the same information.
It just makes it easier for the reader to understand.
Maybe the teaching point I think is for people when they review articles, like, don't be afraid
to do that and just wait.
Oh, I've got scribble all over this.
Yeah, exactly.
So it's like, I draw the arrow.
That's increasing light, that's decreasing light, and that's how I can pay attention to
what's actually happening. Right. And I'm actually in touch with the arrow. That's increasing light, that's decreasing light, and that's how I can pay attention to what's actually happening.
Right.
And I'm actually in touch with the editorial staff at Nature Mental Health, although they
don't know that I'm covering this paper until after this comes out.
You know, I think one thing that scientific journals really, really need to do is start
making the readability of the articles better for non-experts.
I mean, chances are, if you can't understand a graph and this is true for
everybody, chances are there's a problem with the way it's presented. Put it on them. But
then of course try and parse it because rarely if ever is it all spelled out clearly. But
anyway, that's what we're trying to do here. So yeah, the way I would have done is say
second quartile is low amounts of nighttime light exposure and define what that is. Third
quartile is more light exposure and then fourth maximum amount of light exposure at night. And basically what
you see is that the probability of having worse major depressive symptoms linearly increases
as you go from the second to third to fourth quartile. So more nighttime light exposure, worse for you. And there's a dose response, if you will, of the effect.
Now, we can march through or describe Figure 2
pretty quickly by saying the same thing is true.
Now we're just talking about nighttime light exposure
for generalized anxiety disorder.
So that's Panel C.
Bipolar disorder, although the difference
between the second and third quartile
in bipolar disorder isn't as dramatic.
Once you get up to the fourth quartile, bipolar symptoms get much worse when people are getting
nighttime light exposure.
I really want to emphasize that point because they go on in the discussion of this paper
to reemphasize that point several times.
In fact, they say that while light exposure during the day, of course, we will go into the data is beneficial for mental health. For people with bipolar
disorder, it seems that light exposure at night is especially problematic, independent
of how much sunlight they're getting during the day. The person with bipolar disorder
who's struggling with either a manic or depressive episode who's making a point to get sunlight
during the day, who's also getting light exposure at night, is making their symptoms worse.
And keep in mind, they couldn't completely control this, but this is largely independent
of things like sleep duration.
So that doesn't necessarily mean that the person sleeping lasts, although in a manic
episode, presumably they are, it's independent of exercise.
It's independent of a bunch of other things because any logical person will hear this and say, okay, well, they gain more light at night because
they're doing a bunch of other things, but it's largely independent of those other things.
Likewise, the symptomology of PTSD gets far worse with increasing light exposure at night.
Self-harm really takes a leap from being fairly, I don't want to say minimal at the second and third quartiles,
so low and let's say medium, I'm taking some liberties here, but low and medium amounts
of artificial light exposure at night, then for people who get quite a lot of nighttime
light exposure, self-harm goes up, probability of psychotic episodes goes up or psychotic
symptoms.
Now, what's nice about the data is that the exact inverse is basically true for daytime
light exposure, although not across the board.
We can generally say that for major depressive disorder, generalized anxiety, bipolar symptoms
there, it's a little more scattered, PTSD and self-harm, the more daytime light exposure,
ideally from sunlight, because that's actually what's being measured in most cases. We can talk about how we know that is going to approximately linearly drop the probability or the severity
of these symptoms.
And we could just explain again that the odds ratios now seem to be going down. So an odds
ratio of 0.7 now refers to a 30% reduction in the variable of interest here.
Exactly. Now the psychosis panel F which focuses on psychosis, I think is also worth mentioning
in a bit more detail. There's a fairly dramatic reduction in psychotic symptoms as one gets
more daytime light exposure, independent of nighttime light exposure. There's a well-known
phenomenon called ICU psychosis, which is that people come into the hospital
for a broken leg or a car accident. Maybe they were getting surgery from Peter back
when for something totally independent. They're housed in the hospital. And as anyone who's
ever been in a hospital as a patient or visitor knows, the lighting environment, the hospital
is absolutely dreadful for health. Just dreadful. People often complain about the food in the
cafeteria as being unhealthy. That's often not always true
Not always true, but the lighting environments in hospitals is absolutely counter to health
Especially in the intensive care unit. I think the intensive care unit Hopkins the main one the main sick you didn't have windows
People who go into the hospital with a brain injury or with a stroke or something
I get contacted all the time even though I'm not a clinician.
What should I do for my kid, my parent?
I always say get them near a window and start to the best of your abilities controlling
their sleep wake cycle.
Now oftentimes there's nurses coming in and taking blood tests and measuring pulses in
the middle of the night.
That's disruptive.
There's bright light, not just blue light.
That's disruptive.
It's noisy.
That's disruptive.
ICU psychosis is when
non-psychotic individuals start having psychotic episodes in the hospital because of nighttime
light exposure and in some cases lack of daytime sunlight. We can say that with some degree of
confidence because when those people go home, even though sometimes their symptoms for what
brought them to the hospital in the first place get worse, their psychosis goes away and it's independent of medication.
So let's just be really direct.
There is a possibility that we are all socially jet lagged, that we are all disrupting these
mood regulation systems by not getting enough daytime light and by getting too much nighttime
light.
If we want to look at just some of the bullet points
of the takeaways, and then Peter, thank you,
you highlighted a few of these.
Can we just go back to this figure too for a second?
Oh yeah, sure.
There's a handful of things that really jump out.
I had a feeling Joe was gonna wanna dig in the data more.
Let's do it, let's do it.
And again, I normally wouldn't make so much hay out of this
except for the fact that they're so tight.
There are a few that really stand out.
And again, I love this figure.
I would have labeled it a little differently
to make it completely user friendly.
But nevertheless, the increasing light at night
and the impact on depression,
let me be really technical in what I say,
and the relationship or correlation to depression
is very strong.
The relationship to light and self-harm
in the upper quartile.
So when you take those 25% of people with the most nighttime light, that relationship
to self-harm is interesting and completely uncoupled from the other 75%.
That's interesting.
But uncoupled, you mean that at the lower levels of light exposure at night, you're not seeing
an increase in self-harm.
Not whatsoever.
And then once you get to that fourth quartile. It's a big step. It's like a 30 seeing an increase in self-harm. Not whatsoever. And then once you get to that fourth quartile...
It's a big step.
It's like a 30% greater risk of self-harm.
That's right.
Yeah, so it's totally flat.
The first, second, third quartile, no different, and then fourth big jump.
And then the inverse relationship.
As light increases during the daytime, you see this reduction in self-harm.
Interesting.
The PTSD relationship based on nighttime light and the
psychosis relationship based on daytime light. Those are the ones that really jumped out to me.
I think anxiety, relatively less impressive here and bipolar disorder didn't seem as strong
as well. So I think those are the big ones that jumped out to me.
Yeah, I agree. There's a bit more scatter on generalized anxiety and the degree of
significant change is not as robust. In other words, getting a lot of daytime
light, ideally from sunlight, is not necessarily going to reduce your levels of
anxiety. Getting a lot of nighttime light exposure is not increasing nighttime
anxiety that much. Although 20% is not nothing for nighttime light exposure.
But yeah, the psychosis, major depression and self-harm, they leap out.
Maybe we can just drill a little bit deeper on major depression.
And basically when you go from the second to third quartile of nighttime light exposure,
so more than nighttime light exposure, you basically go from no significant increase
to almost a 20% increase. And then as you get up to the fourth quartile, so the most nighttime light exposure at about
25% increase in major depressive symptoms.
That's no joke.
I think that we don't have the data right here, but if we were to look at standard SSRI
treatment for major depression, people debate this pretty actively, but light is a very
potent stimulus. And the timing of light is critical because the inverse is also true
as you get to the fourth quartile of daytime light exposure, you get about a 20% reduction
in major depressive disorder.
Yep. What I like about a study like this is that it puts the error bars so easy to see
on the data. And why is that interesting? Well,
there's a belief that bigger is always better in sample size. And we often talk about that
through the lens of power analysis. So how many subjects do we need to reach a conclusion
that is powered to this level? That's true. But what I don't think gets discussed as
often is the opposite of that,
which is what if you overpower a study?
In other words, what if the power analysis says to have a level of power at 90%, you
need a thousand subjects and you say, great, we're going to do 10,000 subjects?
Well you're clearly powered for it, but you might be overpowered and people might say,
well, why would that be a bad thing?
It could be a bad thing because it means you are very likely to reach statistical significance in
things that might not be actually significant. And so one thing about this study that is
just a quick and dirty way to tell that it's probably not overpowered is that you have
varying lengths of error bars. And what that tells me is that,
and again, this is not like a formal statistical analysis,
it's just kind of like a back of the envelope
statistical analysis.
If you look, for example, at self harm in the top quartile,
you actually have pretty big error bars.
In fact, all the self harm
have sort of slightly bigger error bars.
And yet when you look at, for example, the depression,
even though the error bars aren't all the same size,
they're tighter. In fact, when you look at the for example, the depression, even though the airbars aren't all the same size, they're tighter.
In fact, when you look at the relationship between depression and daytime light, the airbars are really, really small.
So that just gives me confidence that there is variability in this, which paradoxically you kind of want to see.
Because it tells me that this wasn't just done...
There's, I think you said 8,000 subjects were in this.
86,000.
86,000, sorry.
Yeah, you realize that it wasn't that, oh, this should have been done with a tenth of
that or a half of that.
And we're picking up signal that is statistically relevant but clinically irrelevant.
Thanks for that point.
I didn't pay attention to that.
I mean, I paid attention to the error bars, but I didn't know that.
So thank you.
I'm learning too.
I suppose for people that are listening, we can just give them a sense of what the error bar ranges are for self-harm. They're running
as much as 20% either side of the mean, the average. And for major depression, it looks
like it's more like-
If that, yeah.
If that, maybe more like five. Yeah, I see what you're saying. So when you get a very
large sample size, you're going to have some outliers in there and you can mask
those outliers just by having so many data points.
Because these error bars directly tell you whether or not you're statistically significant.
So what's really nice about this type of graph and you see these in, there's going to be
a graph in my paper where you see the same analysis.
They're always drawing the 95% confidence interval
on the data point. And if the 95% confidence interval does not touch the line of unity,
which in this case is the Hodg's ratio of 1.0 or the x-axis, then you know it's statistically
significant to the confidence interval they've defined, which is almost assuredly 95%. Sometimes
they'll make it tighter at 99. And so that's why you can just look right at these and go, oh look, in depression,
the second quartile didn't reach statistical significance because the error bars are touching
the line, just as the case for the second and third quartile for self-harm. But when you look at
the fourth quartile, you can see that the lower tip of the error bars isn't anywhere near unity.
And so we know without having to look up the p value that it's smaller than
either 0.05 or 0.1, however they defined it.
And it's really amazing when you see these overpowered studies, which are
easier to do epidemiologically, where the p value ends up being microscopic.
They can drive their p values down to anything low because sample size can be
infinite, but you can see that it's just the error bar is just skimming above the unity line, but it's so so so tight
interesting
one thing that I
Hope people are taking away from this study is that?
Imagine you're somebody who has a very sensitive
Circadian mood system
Well, that would mean you need less daytime
light exposure to feel good or less bad, but it also means that you might need very little
light at night in order to negatively impact your mood systems. And in fact, they make this
argument in the discussion as an interesting point.
Again, what I like about this study is that they've separated day and nighttime light
exposure.
It turns out that many of the drugs that are used to treat bipolar disorder are effective,
perhaps in part because they reduce the sensitivity of the light sensing circadian apparatus.
Now that's interesting, right?
If you think about this,
okay, so these are drugs that can ameliorate some of the symptoms of bipolar, perhaps in part by
reducing the extent to which nighttime light exposure can exacerbate bipolar symptoms.
Conversely, there's evidence that people who take certain antidepressants
may suppress the ability for daytime light to positively impact the mood systems of the brain.
Now, of course, we don't want people halting their medication on the basis of that statement alone.
Please don't talk to your psychiatrist. But if we know one thing for sure, it's that if you want a
significant outcome and a paper as a scientist, give a drug, any drug, and look at the amount of rapid eye movement sleep
or the circadian cycle.
Pretty much any drug alters a circadian rhythm, for better or worse.
We start to think about which medications might adjust our overall sensitivity to light.
Sometimes this could be a good thing.
You think less sensitivity to light.
Well, for people who have bipolar disorder, the amount of daytime light exposure isn't
that important for their overall mood regulation, but the amount of nighttime light exposure
really is.
In other words, darkness for eight hours every night should be viewed, in my opinion, as
a treatment for bipolar disorder, not the only treatment.
But it's also clear that we should all be avoiding really bright, extensive, really
bright nighttime light exposure.
I mean, if anything, my takeaway from this study is that darkness at night is the fourth
key light stimulus.
A couple of things, very bright moonlight, very bright candlelight is probably only like,
gosh, three to 50 lux.
When you go outside on a brightly lit full moon night, I encourage people to download this free app.
I have no relationship to it called Light Meter.
It gives you a pretty good read of what the lux are in that environment.
By the way, a lot of people don't realize this.
They think you just tap the button and then it tells you how many lux.
You hold it down.
It's kind of fun.
You can scan around the room and see how many lux are on average
coming from that location or outside.
Go out on a really bright moonlit night.
We should have a full moon tonight.
Yeah, let's do it.
You're not gonna get above 100 lux.
That's incredible.
You're sitting at a candlelight dinner
with your spouse or with friends
and it's clearly bright enough to see them.
Put that lux meter right up, not too close to the flame,
50 to 200 lux.
Maybe 400 lux.
I had an interesting experience a couple of months ago on an Elk Hunt where it was
a full moon, which actually makes the hunting not so great.
But it was the first time I've ever noticed my shadow in relation to the moon.
That's how bright it seemed the light was.
This is Halloween appropriate.
We're recording this close to Halloween.
Super interesting to think it could be that dim.
Campfire. Firelight. You think, okay, a gathering around a campfire. Then, okay,
you know, everyone's circadian rhythm must have been disrupted for ages before the development
of electricity. No. Those campfires are extremely bright, but they're not that bright compared to
a very densely overcast day.
And what is your phone if you don't use any sort of light mitigating tech on it?
Well, distance matters.
At the distance we're holding it?
Yeah.
So with all the wavelengths cranked up, there is a nice feature intrinsic to the phone where
you can eliminate the blues at night or this kind of thing.
But if you crank it up to maximum light intensity, probably something like 500 to 1000 lux.
Now keep in mind though, it's additive. So it's over time.
So lux is a measure of, I think it relates back to Candela's as the amount of light shown. And I
think it's like the one meter away and there's a squaring and a falling off of distance. We can
look it up. These are old school measurements compared to lux. But keep in mind that if you're
looking at your phone or tablet at 800 lux or 500 lux in the evening,
and you do that for two hours, well, you're summing quite a lot of photons. Now, it is true. I do want to be fair to the biology
and it'd be dishonest to say anything different. We've hammered on people about not shifting their circadian rhythm with light at night.
But we know that the middle of the day and the middle of the night are circadian dead zones. You can't shift your circadian rhythm that well in the middle
of the day and the middle of the night. But you can provide a wake-up signal for your body
and brain. It's really that sunrise and sunset that are critical. That's why I said there
are four things. See sunrise or sunrising. You don't need to see it cross the horizon.
Sunset, bright light during the day, minimize light exposure at night. You don't need pitch black. In fact, pitch black probably just increases the frequency of injury.
You know, I get up in the middle of the night to use the bathroom probably once. I think it's
normal. I go back to sleep. If it were pitch black, I'd probably injure myself. So just dim it down.
Some people use red lights.
Our mutual friend.
Yeah, Rick Rubin is.
Can we tell a funny story about Rick?
Yeah, of course. Of course. You know this story, but just in case Rick's listening, Rick Rubin is. Can we tell a funny story about Rick? Yeah, of course, of course.
You know this story, but in case Rick's listening, he'll appreciate this.
Rick was here staying last summer.
He's up in our guest house and he came down after the first night and he was like unacceptable.
You know what I'm talking about, right?
Unacceptable accommodations.
What did he do?
He removed all the lighting that existed in that room and replaced it with red light bulbs,
which I later used when I stayed here
and then later stole when I left.
The teenage me, I took them.
I took them, that's why Derek, when he stayed here,
or Lane, or anyone else who stayed here,
Conte, or anyone else didn't have those.
I took them, I love them.
So funny, Jill is like, you know,
Rick changed every light in the guest house to red.
No, I didn't know that, but I'm not surprised.
Yeah.
Well, in his place, he has mostly either no lighting or red lighting.
So during the day, it just goes by ambient light and then red light in the evening or
candlelight.
And it's great.
And, you know, people hear red lights and they think they have to buy these expensive
red light units.
That's not what we're talking about.
You can literally buy red party lights or just a red bulb.
Some people say, well, can I just use a red film or can I put a t-shirt over the lamp?
I worry about people putting t-shirts over lamps because of the fire hazard.
But I'll be honest, I dim the lights in my home at night.
When I travel, sometimes I will bring one of the stolen from Rick Rubin red lights.
The Rick Rubin stash.
Here's something where I've sort of softened my tune.
So I used to be kind of a hard liner, no blue light in the evening guy.
Everything was red light at night as far as my phone using flux on the computer, you know,
whatever it was.
I suspect that that matters somewhat.
But I think what matters more is the stimulation that may come from those things.
And what I've come to realize, at least in me, which means it probably is true in others
as well, and at least some others, is that what I'm doing on my phone matters more than
how bright my phone is.
In other words, if I've got the best blue light filter in the world on my phone, but I'm doom scrolling social media
and getting lit up on email, that's way worse for me
than if I've got my phone on maximum light
and I'm watching YouTube videos of F1 cars
and driving around having fun.
It's a totally different experience.
So the context matters. And I
think for that reason, I would want people to be mindful of the whole picture going to
bed under a period of intense duress brought on by something that's an equally dangerous
component to all of this that's distinct from what we're talking about. But I want people
to be able to think of this in the context of everything.
It's a really important point. One thing I'll say is that if you're going to stay up past your normal bedtime, if you're
going to get a lot of light in your eyes, I would hope that it would be for fun reasons
and for reasons you enjoy.
You should definitely spend some nights out.
You should definitely do some all-nighters studying.
If it's going to help you get the grade that's permanent, right?
I'd certainly have done all-nighters studying and grant writing for years.
There are going to be the inevitable all-nighters due to trip to the hospital or you heard something on the news that really amped you up or you just simply can't sleep. That stuff is going to happen.
So I think the goal should be to minimize light exposure at night. And I think what you just said
is especially true because we don't know, for instance, people talk about the negative impact
of social media. Is it the fact that people are looking at this little box for so many
hours per day? Is it all the things they're not doing? Is it what they're looking at per
se? All of those things interact and are really important. We know based on studies from the
Stanford sleep lab that if you wake up in the middle of the night, looking at what time
it is can be very disruptive to your ability to fall back asleep and to your sense the next day.
It's a placebo effect, but it's a powerful one of how tired you are the next day.
They've done this where they wake people up in the middle of the night and then they say
it's 4 a.m. versus 2 a.m. versus 6 a.m. and people's perceived levels of energy during
the day in some ways correlate with how much
sleep they think they got.
Likewise, and this is one of the potential concerns with sleep trackers.
Allie Crumlin talked about this when she came on our podcast.
If people see a poor sleep score, they often feel worse than if they see a good sleep score.
Now, of course, physiology matters.
You can't lie to yourself and say, you got a great night's sleep simply by virtue of
a sleep score.
But I worry more about the false, if it's a false negative, we don't want to put valence
on this.
Seeing a bad sleep score and then deciding that you're going to have a terrible day.
I think a bad sleep score is an indication that you might need to dial some things in
a bit better.
Getting a great sleep score is an indication that you might be doing a number of things
right and start looking at these things as averages.
Would you agree?
Yeah, completely. I don't think it's that different from CGM. I think that CGM is an
amazing tool to provide insight and you pretty much know the insights after a relatively
short period of time. 30 days, maybe at the outside 90 for a person with a very complicated
life, and you know all you need to know about how the inputs affect the output.
Thereafter, if you choose to use it, it's a behavioral tool. In other words, you're
using this to build in a Hawthorne effect. I think the same is largely true with sleep
trackers. Most people have this profound sense of learning when they first encounter one
of these things. And it's again, you've heard it all 100 times, oh my God, I can't believe
what alcohol does to my sleep.
Or caloric trackers.
Exactly.
Like I think Lane Norton's app, Carbon, I have no financial relationship to it. I use
it. It's taught me, wow, like I consume a lot of calories in the form of certain things
at certain times of day. And there's just a lot of good learning in that.
But it's the act of tracking that helps you manage it. And similarly, I think it's the act of knowing you're going to be looking at that score that
gamifies it that kind of helps people do the right things.
You know what?
I'm not going to have that drink tonight or I'm not going to eat that snack before bed
because I've now been conditioned to see how that impacts score.
That said, I think that recovery scores and things like that are just notoriously poor at predicting performance.
And I think there's a reason that serious athletes would never use things like that.
They would tend to rely on the more tried and true methods of predicting behavior such
as heart rate, maybe heart rate variability, but morning resting heart rate probably more
predictive than anything else.
And then in workout things such as heart rate, heart rate recovery, lactate threshold, things like that.
So yeah, I agree.
I think we have to, and I say this as a guy
who's generally perceived to be the most pro-device guy
in the world, people would be surprised
how sparingly I use things like that.
I mean, I do some tracking, not as much as you.
I love things that seem to work the first time and
every time in terms of our natural biology. Based on a couple of criteria, there's an
established mechanism. It's been explored in the context of pathology like mental health
disorders as well as pro health in healthy individuals that it make really good sense
at the level of kind of wellness and let's just say ancient health, you know, when you're talking about getting a lot of sunlight during the day
A lot of people say, well, of course get outside and play
Not getting too much light at night. Of course, this is just good old quote-unquote good old-fashioned advice
People spend 90% of their time indoors now their daytime environments are too dim
their nighttime environments are too dim. Their nighttime environments are too bright.
This misleading aspect of artificial light that when you see a bright bulb, you think I'm getting
a lot of photons is part of the problem. The fact that when you're out on an overcast day and you
think there's sun, quote unquote, isn't out, well, it's hidden by cloud cover, but just think about how well you can navigate
that environment without a flashlight versus at night where you would require a flashlight.
We evolved under this traumatic difference in day night availability of photons,
independent of whether or not you can, quote, see the sun.
And it's just very clear that all the mechanisms in
our brain and body that regulate mood are just powerfully regulated by this stuff. So
I've made it a point to really reduce the amount of nighttime light that I'm getting,
but I'm less concerned about flipping on the light switch to use the bathroom as I used
to be. I used to think, oh, I'm like quashing all my melatonin. This is terrible. I know
I can't shift my circadian clock then I know that that light, yes, while it's bright, if it's brief, I'm not going to worry about it
too much. Would it be better to have a dim light on as opposed to a bright light? Sure,
but I'm not going to stress it in a hotel bathroom or something. I'm not going to walk around shielding
my eyes. People will sometimes ask me, by the way, is it different to look at the phone directly
versus if you tilt the phone away. Well, it absolutely is.
I mean, think about a flashlight shown on the ground
in front of you, very few photons getting in your eyes,
versus shown directly in your eyes.
Think about ambient light from the sun going everywhere
versus looking in the general direction of the sun,
so east in the morning, west in the afternoon, of course.
The directionality of the light matters.
So I'm not saying that you would need to like
peek at your phone as if you're looking
over the edge of a bowl or something into it.
But my friend, Samar Hattaru,
is head of the Chronobiology Unit
at the National Institutes of Mental Health.
We used to room together at meetings.
We stopped because he's a terrible snorer.
There were a few times when I considered
suffocating him in the middle of the night
since he was already suffocating himself.
Now we just, we don't stay in the same rooms anymore.
We're no longer postdocs.
But I caught him looking at his phone in the middle of the night and he would tilt it away
like he's holding a platter for those that are just listening and kind of like looking
over at the screen there and like, what are you doing?
This is ridiculous.
He said, I'm trying not to get so much light in my eyes.
That's a little extreme, but I think it illustrates the point, which is how much direct light exposure
you get at night matters.
How much direct sunlight exposure you get, especially early morning, late afternoon, and throughout the day, it really matters.
Now remind me, Andrew, what is the wavelength of sunlight?
Great. So sunlight is going to include all visible spectrum, right?
Which runs from how many nanometers to what?
Yeah, so, well, let's answer two questions there. This wrist sensor detected 470 nanometer to 650 nanometer light.
So that's going to be blue and ultraviolet.
Ultraviolet's kind of high threes.
Yeah, that's kind of like blue to orange.
Yeah, blue to orange.
That's what this was measuring.
So red light is going to be more like 680.
Far red is getting out to 7, 720 and up upwards of that.
Blue light is going to fall somewhere in the low fours. Ultraviolet is getting down into 7, 720 and up upwards of that blue light is going to fall somewhere
in the low fours ultraviolet is getting down into the high threes and lower.
And so these spectra of light.
So during the day, you know, midday light, you're getting what looks like white light.
You'll see, oh, the sky is blue and the sun is bright white light.
It's not even yellow to your eye.
And of course, don't stare at it, especially in the middle of the day.
You're getting all visible spectra. So you're getting everything from UV all the way out
to red light.
It's just coming in at equal intensities.
So is that a potential limitation of this study in that it didn't have a sensor that
could pick up the full spectrum of light?
Potentially we don't think of humans as UV capable like we can't perceive UV light.
A ground squirrel for instance instance, has UV sensors
in its eyes. Turns out you know why they use this. It's crazy. You know, when the ground squirrels sit
up on their haunches, they're actually signaling one another, they rub urine on their belly and it
reflects UV. The New York Times for some reason has been running a series of articles about naturally
occurring fluorescence at night and all sorts of scorpions and monotremes
like the platypus.
No one really knows the reason for these odd wavelength of light emissions for all these
animals.
But you know, we view things in the blue violet and up to red and we're not pit vipers, we
can't see far red.
But we can see lower than 470 nanometers and we can see higher than 650.
Is there a technology reason why they had such a narrow band in these sensors?
Is it not possible that they could have used a wrist sensor that was wider?
This study was initiated in 2013.
The tech was probably far worse than it is now.
Again, I would love for somebody to design an eyeglass where it's measuring how many
photons you're getting across the
day. I'm not a big fan of having everything be apified. So I would love it if the frame
would just shift color across the morning. Like you go outside on a cloudy day, you know,
you wear these glasses. And by the way, it's fine to wear eyeglasses or contacts for sunlight
viewing for setting your circadian rhythm. People always say, well, why is that okay?
And a window is not well, corrective lenses are actually focusing the light onto your retina.
The windows and windshields are scattering the light and filtering.
And how much are sunglasses filtering this out?
Too much.
We can safely say way too much, probably causing a 10 fold decrement in the total
luxe count that's landing on your retina.
But of course, sunglasses are important driving into sun.
And some people have very sensitive eyes.
I can't sit at a cafe with a bright,
brightly reflective table in the afternoon.
I just squint like crazy.
I can't do it.
My dad who's darker eyed and you know,
he's just South American descent.
He can just sit there just fine.
My mom who's got light eyes like me,
you know, we're like, oh, it's really tough.
You just have a terrible time.
People differ in their light sensitivity.
So there's one other macro question I have here, and it's not answerable because
without randomization, we can't know it.
But it's the question of how much reverse causality can exist in these observations.
So again, these observations demonstrate very tight
correlations, very strong associations, especially in the five areas that we
highlighted. But it's possible that part of what we're seeing is reverse
causality brought on by both the treatments, which you've already kind of
alluded to, and also the condition itself. Do you want to explain reverse
causality for people? And maybe could you mention for those that missed the Hawthorne effect?
The Hawthorne effect is an effect that is named after an observation of what took place
in a factory where they were actually studying worker productivity with light of all things.
What it refers to is the idea that people will change their behavior when they are observed.
So if I said, well, I really
want to know what a day in the life is like for Andrew Huberman, I'm going to follow him around for
a day. It's very unlikely that his behavior that day will be exactly as it was if I wasn't there.
Which is why you probably will never see a day in the life of Andrew Huberman.
Although it's pretty scripted unless I'm traveling. It's morning sunlight, hydration and some cardio or weight training and then a lot
of time reading papers.
It'd be the most boring video in the world because it's mostly me reading and underlining
things.
But it's why gamifying things can be beneficial, right?
It's why a CGM can be beneficial because it's sort of like somebody's watching you and you're
going to modify what you eat in response to it or why tracking can really be an effective way to reduce input
because there's a sense of being monitored by doing that, especially if someone literally
monitors it.
In other words, you can set up an accountability partner where your health coach or someone
is actually seeing the data.
So that's what it is.
Now as far as reverse causality, when you look at variables, so
let's just pick a common one that's unrelated to this. So there's an association that more
diet soda consumption is associated with greater obesity. It's a bit paradoxical.
Is that true?
It is. Yeah, it's been demonstrated in many series. The greater the consumption of diet soda, the greater the prevalence of obesity.
And that has been postulated by some to suggest that non-nutritive sweeteners,
such as aspartame or sucralose or things like that, are actually part of what's
causing obesity.
And while there are probably some arguments you could make around the impact that
those things might have on the gut microbiome, and maybe there's some way and that's happening, it's
also equally likely, if not probably more likely that there's reverse causality there,
that a person who is obese is therefore contemplating how much they're eating or thinking, hey, what's
an easy way that I can reduce calories?
How about instead of drinking a Coke, I drink a diet coke? And so there, the causality which you would impute to mean the drink is causing
the obesity, it might be no, the obesity is causing the choice of drink.
So here the question is, how much of the effect we're seeing is a result of the
condition that's being studied?
How much of the disruption in both day and night
light exposure is the result of the depression?
It's dysregulating the sleep.
Maybe they're sleeping more during the day
and more awake at night because of depression.
Again, you can't know this.
This is where epidemiology never allows us to determine this.
And sadly, these questions can only be answered through either
direct randomization or Mendelian randomization, which by the way, I was going to also ask you,
do you know if anyone has examined this from a Mendelian standpoint? That would be very
interesting. I don't know enough about the biology to know what SNPs would be studyable,
but that would be interesting.
Yeah. What Peter is saying is, you know, if you knew something about the genomes of these people, you would
be in a great position to perhaps even link up sensitivity genes with genes for pathways
involved in major depression and bipolar.
Getting to this issue of reverse causality, I think it's very straightforward to imagine
that the person who's experiencing a manic episode is going to be up for two weeks at a time, sadly, and getting a lot of nighttime light exposure.
Now, nighttime dark exposure is a treatment for bipolar is something that people are starting
to talk about. So making sure that even those people are awake, that they're at least blue
locking at night, reducing their online activities, but people with severe manic episodes have a
hard time regulating their own behavior, of course.
And it's not one or the other.
Like I don't want the question to come across to the listener that it has to be one or the
other.
It's only A can cause B or B can cause A. No, it's actually a lot of times these things
feed off each other.
Going back to the soda example, I actually think there's a bit of both.
I actually think there's a real clear body habit that dictates beverage choice, but I also am starting to think
that insusceptible individuals, non-nutritive sweeteners will alter the gut biome and that
alters metabolism. What about just hunger? I remember Lane telling me, I've seen at least one of the studies that water is probably
better for us than diet soda, but that for some people diet soda is a great tool for reducing
caloric intake. I also know some individuals, not me, who drink diet soda. I drink diet soda from
time to time, mainly stevia sweetened sodas. But what I'm referring to here are people besides myself who drink diet soda and it seems to stimulate their appetite. There's
something about the perception of sweet as driving hunger, whereas not eating or drinking
anything with any sweetness doesn't seem to. This is one of the things that I wonder if
it impacts why some people like intermittent fasting, because for some people, I wonder
if the perception
of sweetness, even just the smell of food, we know, can stimulate appetite.
So you can mention the perception of sweetness in the mouth, even if there's no calories
there.
I don't think it necessarily makes people hypoglycemic, but perhaps it makes them think
about like sweet means food.
For instance, for years, I love the combination of a diet coke and a slice of pizza whenever I was in New York. Ideally two slices of pizza. Now, every time I have
a diet coke, which isn't that often, but I like diet coke, especially a little bit of
lemon in it, I just think about a slice of cheese or mushroom or pepperoni pizza. It's
like I want it, I crave it more. So there's a parrot association there that I think is
real. And we know based on Dana Small's lab at Yale that there's this parrot association between
the sweetness from sucralose and that there's an insulin response.
They actually had to cease the study in kids because they were becoming prediabetic, which
unfortunately meant the study was never published.
Have you talked to Dana on this podcast?
I really want to get her onto my podcast.
No, we wrote a premium newsletter on this several months ago.
It's got to be like, I don't know, 10 to 20,000 words on all things related to sugar substitutes.
Okay, I'll read that.
So yeah, folks who are interested in this topic, I would refer them to the premium newsletter
on sugar substitutes.
I think it was our September edition.
The short of it is the data are a little bit noisy, but there is indeed some
sweeteners in some studies do result in that phenomenon. You described the cephalic insulin
response and I came away from the research that went into that, which was herculean effort on the
part of the team, a little bit more confused than when I went in, but being even more cautious around artificial sweeteners than I was going in.
And not for the reasons, I didn't find any evidence
that these things are cancer causing.
So that's the headline stuff people worry about.
Oh, I protein causes.
We have to ingest like 10 grams of it
or something crazy like that.
I came away more confident that from a long-term safety
perspective, in terms of cancer and catastrophic outcomes
like that, that wasn't the issue.
But I came away much more cautious around these things can really be mucking around with
both your brain chemistry and your gut chemistry, which can pertain to your metabolism.
And therefore, my takeaway was buyer beware, use limited amounts only.
The one, by the way, that still emerged to me as a reasonable one, it's the only one that I use,
and I've talked about it a lot, is xylitol.
Xylitol for chewing, so for gum,
and alulose as an additive.
Safer, you're saying?
Yes, those are basically the only two I will consume.
I'll drink a diet coke every now and again
if I'm on a plane or something.
This law that got passed few years ago
that you couldn't bring liquids of your own into the airport and onto the plane. What a great scheme to get
people to buy overpriced fluids in the airport. I mean, there are more important issues in the
world, but this one really gets me. But I drink things with a little bit of stevia and the occasional
Diet Coke. I generally avoid sucralose. I don't like the way it tastes. Monkfruit's
too sweet, but maybe we'll do a podcast on that in the future. Okay, so I think we can
wrap this paper.
But tell me what you think about that point. You know more about this stuff than I do,
but if you had to just lay on your judgment, if it were 100 to 0, you would say the light
is 100% causal in the effects we're seeing. If it were 0 to 100,
you'd say, nope, the behavior is 100% causal of the exposure to light. Again, you can't know it,
but what does your intuition tell you?
Okay, there's my intuition and then there's my recognition of my own bias because I started
working on these circadian pathways, originating in the eye back in 98 as a graduate student
at Berkeley.
The cells, these melmopsin, intrinsically
sensitive retinal ganglion cells were discovered
in the early 2000s by a guy named Iggy Provencio, Dave
Burson, Sam Rattar, Sachin Panta, and others.
And it was one of the most important discoveries
in all of biology, clearly.
So I've been very excited about these systems, but if I set
that aside, so bias disclosure made, I think 65 to 75% of the effects are likely due to light
directly. Now it's impossible to tease those apart, as you mentioned, but to play devil's
advocate against myself, you could imagine that the depressed individual is laying around indoors with the curtains drawn.
They didn't sleep well the night before, which gives you a photosensitivity that isn't pleasant
like it sucks to have bright light in your eyes first thing in the morning, especially
if you didn't sleep well.
And then they're making their coffee in a dimly lit, what they think is brightly lit
environment and then they're looking at their phone and the state of the world sucks and their state of their internal landscape
is rough and maybe they're dealing with a pain or injury or something and their likelihood
of getting outside is low and when they do get outside they're going to shuffle and not.
So I could see how the behaviors could really limit the amount of light exposure and then
evening rolls around, they've been tired all day and a common symptom of depression, you fall asleep and then two or
three in the morning, they're wide awake. What are you going to do at two or three when you're
wide awake, sit in the dark? No, you're going to get online, you're going to listen to things you
might have. I'm not recommending this but alcoholic drink in order to try and fall asleep. I mean,
this is the pattern. Shaking up that pattern is really what so much of my public health work these days is about and trying to get
people onto a more natural daylight, night dark rhythm. But yeah, it's impossible to
tease apart. We do know this and this is really serious. We know that in almost every instance,
almost every psychopathology report of suicide in the weeks, but especially in the days preceding
suicide, that person's circadian rhythms looked almost inverted from their normal patterns.
And that's true of non-bipolar individuals as well.
Circadian disruption and disruption in psychiatric health are inextricable.
Conversely, positive mood and affect and circadian behavior
seem very correlated. I mean, I think it's clear that if you want to become an early
riser, get light in your eyes and get activity in your body early in the day, you built the,
you entrained to those rhythms so that you start to anticipate that morning workout,
you start to anticipate the morning sunlight. Just one more scientific point, we know that when you view bright sunlight in the morning
or just sunlight that's illuminating your environment, as you said, you don't even have
to see the sun itself, that there's a 50, 50% increase in the amplitude of the morning
cortisol spike, which is a good thing because the amplitude of the morning cortisol spike
is inversely related to the amplitude of the evening cortisol spike, and high evening cortisol is associated with middle of the night waking and on and on. So,
you know, I'm very bullish on these mechanisms. I also love that they're so deeply woven into our
evolutionary history, you know, that we share with single cell organisms. It's so wild. But,
of course, there's going to be a bidirectionality there and it's impossible to see where one thing starts and the other one stops.
Here's my take, Andrew. First of all, I actually, with far less authority than you, agree with your assessment and might even be a little bit more bullish, might even put it at 80-20.
And here I'll give you my explanations, which stem more from my fastidious battles with epidemiology in general.
Because so much of the world that I live in still has to rely on epidemiologic data.
And so how do you make sense of it?
The truth of it is most of it is really pretty bad.
I tend to find myself looking at the Austin Bradford Hill criteria all the time.
And for folks who don't know, he was a statistician
who basically proposed a set of criteria. Believe there are eight of them, and I can't believe I
don't know every one of them off by heart, and I certainly used to. But the more of these criteria
that are met within your correlations, the more likelihood that you will find causality.
So when I think of your data here, the data in this paper, I'll tell you what makes these correlations seem to have causality within them in the direction
that's being proposed. Look at the dose effect. So dose effect matters, and this is done in
quartiles, and that's a very elegant thing. If they just did it as on off, it would be harder.
High low. That's right, but the fact that that they did in quartiles allows you to see that every example in figure two,
I don't believe there is an exception to this.
There's only one exception to what I'm about to say.
Sorry, two out of like God knows how many.
They're all monotonically increasing and decreasing.
In other words, the dose effect is always present.
Another thing is biologic plausibility.
You've spoken
at length about that today. So in other words, sometimes you have to look at
epidemiology and ask, is there a biologic explanation? And here there is.
You've added another one which is evolutionary conservation to the
biologic plausibility. Then you can talk about animal models or experiments in
humans over short durations that generally support these findings.
And so those are just a couple of the Bradford Hill criteria that lead to my belief that,
yeah, there's reverse causality here, but it's not the full explanation and that more
of the explanation is probably the direction that's being proposed.
At the end of the day, like what's the purpose of the discussion?
The purpose of the discussion is if you are under the influence of any of these psychiatric conditions, in addition to the
treatments you're doing now, what else can you do? And to me, the takeaway is, follow these light
behaviors. That's a relatively low lift. When you consider some of the other things, like I'm over
here asking people to do zone two for three hours a week and VO2 max workouts and all this other stuff.
And I think all those things matter for mental health as much as physical health.
But this strikes me as on the spectrum of low asks.
If it's only even 30% causality, 70% reverse causality, like I'll take those, I would still
instate that.
Yeah.
And it's taking your coffee on the balcony. People will often say well how do you do this with
kids? The kids should be doing it too. You know it means popping your sunglasses off,
it means getting out for just a few minutes and the fact that it's additive that these
photon counting mechanisms they sum. This paper also says and I should have stated this earlier,
if you missed your daytime light ration,
get your nighttime dark ration. They are independent and additive. I mean, that's a really something, but of course, ideally, you get both. But I appreciate your take on it. Thanks for your
expertise in parsing epidemiology. I look at fewer studies of that sort, but I learned from you.
And that's one of the reasons I love doing these journal clubs is I learn. So along those lines, tell us about the paper you selected. I'm really eager to learn more.
Well, I wanted to pick a paper that was interesting as a paper. This paper, I think,
is interesting in that it is the landmark study of a class of drugs. But in the same way that you
kind of picked a paper
that I think has a much broader overarching importance, the reason I
picked this paper, which is from New England Journal of Medicine, it's about
13 years old, is because it is kind of the landmark study in a class of drugs
that I believe are the most relevant class of drugs we've seen so far in cancer therapy.
And even though the net effect of these drugs has only served to reduce mortality by maybe
eight to 10%, which is not a huge amount, it's the manner in which they've done it
that gives me great hope for the future, even if it's through other means.
So I'll take a step
back before we go into the paper for again, just the context and background. So the human immune
system is kind of a remarkable thing. It's hard when you're trying to imagine what's the most
amazing part of the human system. And maybe it's my bias as well, because just as you spent your
time in the light system and the photo sensing system, I spent my time in the immunology world.
But it is remarkable to me how our immune systems evolved.
And they have this really brutal task, which is how can they be tuned to detect any foreign
pathogen that is harmful without knowing a priori what
that could be. So in other words, how can you tune a system to be so aggressive
that it can eradicate any virus or bacteria billions of years into the
future without knowing what it's going to be. But at the same time it has to be
so forgiving of the self that it doesn't turn around and attack
the self. It's remarkable. And of course, we can always think of the exceptions. There are things
called autoimmune conditions. So clearly the system fails and the immune system turns around
and attacks the self. If you see a person with vitiligo, I have a little bit of vitiligo on my
back, a couple of spots. Clearly the immune system is attacking something there and destroying some of the pigment.
I didn't realize vitiligo was autoimmune.
Yeah.
There are lots of more serious autoimmune conditions.
Of course, somebody that has lupus or immune system can be attacking the kidney, the immune
system can be attacking any other.
Autoimmune conditions can be deadly.
But fortunately they are very rare and for the most part this immune system
works remarkably well.
So how does it work and why is it that cancer
seems to evade it virtually all of the time?
This is the question.
Let's first of all talk about how it works
and then when I tell you how it works,
you'll say that sounds amazing.
Clearly it should be able to destroy cancer.
I'm going to simplify it by only talking about one system, which is how T cells recognize
and get activated, how T cells recognize antigens.
So we have something called an antigen.
So an antigen is an antibody-generating peptide.
So it's a protein, almost always a protein,
they can be carbohydrates,
but they're almost always proteins
and they're very, very small peptides.
Like we're talking as little as nine amino acids,
maybe up to 20 amino acids.
So teeny, tiny little peptides.
But it's amazing that in such a short peptide,
the body can recognize if that's Andrew or not
Andrew.
We talk about proteins in kilodaltons.
We're talking about proteins in terms of thousands of amino acids that make up every
protein in your body.
And yet, if it samples a protein and sees that, hey, this little 9, 10, 15 peptide amino
acid is not part of you, I know it's bad and therefore I'm going to generate
an immune response to it. So we have what are called antigen presenting cells. You have cells that
go around sampling peptides and they will, on these things called MHC class receptors, bring the
peptide up to the surface and serve it up to the T cell. There are two types of these. There called MHC class receptors, bring the peptide up to the surface and serve it
up to the T cell. There are two types of these. There's MHC class one and MHC class two.
This is major histocompatibility complex.
That's correct. We refer to them that way because of the context in which they were
discovered, which was for organ rejection. So not surprisingly, when you need to put a kidney into another person,
if that kidney is deemed foreign, it will not last long. And the early days of organ transplantation
were rife with immediate rejections. The immediate are the ABO incompatibilities,
but the sort of next layer of incompatibility was MHC incompatibility, which would lead to within weeks the organ is gone as opposed to within hours.
So you have these two classes of MHC.
You have class one and class two.
Class one is what we call endogenous.
So this is basically what happens when a protein or an antigen is coming from inside the cell.
So let's consider the flu. So if you get the flu, the influenza virus
infects the respiratory epithelium of your larynx.
And that virus, as folks listening might remember
from our days of talking about COVID,
viruses can't replicate on their own.
What they do is they hijack the replication machinery
of the host and they use that either to insert their RNA or DNA to replicate.
And in the process, proteins are being made. Well, those proteins are the proteins of the virus, not of us.
So some of those peptides get launched onto these MHC Class 1 gloves. Basically, the glove comes up to the surface,
and a T cell comes along. And in the case of MHC class 1,
it's a CD8 T cell. These are what are called the killer T cells, right? And so this cell comes along
and with its T cell receptor, the T cell receptor meets the MHC class 1 receptor with the antigen
in it. And if that's a lock, it realizes that's my target and it begins to replicate and proliferate
and target those and that creates the immune response. And by the way, that's how it works
when you vaccinate somebody. You're basically pre-building that thing up.
So, would this fall under the adaptive immune response or the innate immune response?
So, this is adaptive. Inate is just the pure antibody response on the B cell side. I won't get into that for the purpose of this discussion.
The other example is MHC class 2, and that's also part of the adaptive system or the innate
system, which is more what we call the exogenous form.
So these are peptides that are usually coming from outside the cell.
So we're going to focus more on the MHC class 1 because this is peptides that come from inside the cell. So we're gonna focus more on the MHC class one,
because this is peptides that come from inside the cell.
Okay, so just keep in the back of your mind,
if a foreign protein gets presented
from inside a cell to outside a cell,
the T cells recognize that
and they will mount a foreign response.
And by the way, that's why we basically can beat any virus.
If you consider how many viruses are around us,
the fact that we almost never die from a viral infection
is a remarkable achievement
of how well this immune system works.
Constantly combating these viruses.
Constantly, and by the way,
we don't really have very effective antiviral agents.
It's not like antibiotics.
Like we have antibiotics up the wazoo.
We're way better at fighting viruses than bacteria.
I've always wondered about this.
To what extent is our ability to ward off viruses on a dated basis as an adult, reliant
on us having been exposed to that virus during development?
Like as I walk around today, maybe I'll be exposed to 100,000 different viruses.
Would you say that half of those I've already got antibodies to because I was exposed to them
at some prior portion of my life?
Yeah, hard to quantify.
And the other ones I'm just building up antibodies.
Like I was on a plane last night,
someone was coughing, so I was hiding.
And I had COVID a little while ago,
so I wasn't too worried about that.
And I feel great today,
but I just assume that on that plane,
I'm in a swamp of viruses, no matter what, and that most of them I've
already been exposed to since I was a little kid. So I've got all the antibodies and they're
just fighting it back, binding up those viruses and destroying them.
Yeah, I think it's part that. And I also think it's part of them that our body can destroy
without mounting much of an immune response. So therefore, your immune system is doing the
work, yet it's not mounting a systemic inflammatory response that you're not sensing. So is it also a physical trapping in, you know,
in my nasal epithelium? Yeah, you have huge barriers, right? So the skin, the hairs in your
nose, all of these things are huge barriers. But assuming that still a bunch of them are getting
in, at least the respiratory ones, that's the other thing to keep in mind, right? There were
certain viruses that are totally useless floating around the air. The viruses that
most people are really afraid of, Hep C, Hep B, HIV, well, if they're sitting on a table
or floating around the air, they're of no threat to you. They have to be transmitted
through the barrier. But again, some of these viruses, you're going to defeat without an
enormous response. And then some of them, why is influenza, quote, unquote,
such a bad virus, whereas the common respiratory cold kind of sidelines you for a day. It's
the immune response that you're feeling. The bigger the immune response to the virus,
the more you're feeling that you feel your immune system going crazy. The inner leucons
that are spiking, the third spacing that occurs to get more and more of the immune cells there, the spike of your temperature as your body basically
tries to cook the virus, all that stuff is your body.
The fatigue.
Yeah. Yeah. You're being drained and all this happening. So one more point I'll mention
just to close the loop on the autoimmunity. How is it that we learn not to attack ourselves?
That's something called thymic selection that occurs in infancy.
So you and I have a no good for nothing tiny little thymus that would be, it's almost impossible
to see these things. You know, when we used to operate on people, the thymus is barely visible
in an adult, in a healthy adult outside of thymic tumors. And a child, the thymus is quite large.
And the purpose of the thymus is to educate T cells and basically show the T cells what
self is and any T cell that doesn't immediately recognize it gets killed.
It's a really clever system where we basically teach you to recognize self at a very early
age and if you can't do that, you're weeded out.
And then the thymus involutes thereafter because it's sort of served its purpose.
Okay.
Now, let's talk about cancer.
So what do we know about cancer?
So we know that, again, cancer is a genetic disease in the sense that every cancer has
genetic mutations.
Most of those mutations are somatic, which means most of those mutations are mutations
that occur during the course of our life.
They're not germline mutations.
The germline being the eggs and sperm, right?
So it's all other cells.
And I love that you pointed out that cancer can be genetic, but isn't necessarily inherited.
People hear genetic and they think inherited.
Inherited is always genetic to some extent, but genetic isn't always inherited.
Yeah, so there are a handful of cancers that are
derived from inherited mutations. So Lynch syndrome is an example of that.
Hereditary
polyposis is an example of that. Where you have a gene that gets passed through the germ line and that
gene codes for a protein like all genes do and it's either you have too much of a gene
or too little of a gene. So it's either a gene that promotes cancer and you have too
much of that or it's a gene that prevents cancer and you have too little of it or a dysfunctional
version of it. Right? So BRCA is an example of that. BRCA is hereditary. BRCA codes for a protein and the
women and men, but mostly the women that we think about who have a BRCA mutation that in some cases
almost guarantees breast cancer, it's because of a defective copy. So, it's like they don't get the
protein that they need to protect them from breast cancer. So, what do we know? Well, we know that
that they need to protect them from breast cancer. So what do we know?
Well, we know that,
and this is probably one of the most remarkable things
I've ever learned,
and it still blows my mind every time.
Well, actually, before I get to that point,
I wanna make another point.
Okay, so cancer, our cells become cancerous,
but they're clearly hijacked
because they have these mutations,
and as a result of these mutations,
they make
proteins that allow cancers to behave differently.
Cancers behave differently from non-cancers in two very critical ways.
The first way is that they do not respond to cell cycle signaling.
If you cut your skin, it heals.
But how does it know to heal just right and not to keep growing and growing
and growing and growing and growing? Well, it knows that because there are cell cycle signals
that tell it time to grow, time to stop. Believe it or not, this is an extreme example. If you
donated to me half of your liver, which I know you would. Absolutely. I'd give you more than half
of my liver. If it meant that we could keep doing these journal clubs, right? Yeah, yeah, yeah. Within months, you would regenerate a full liver.
That's so. Isn't that amazing?
That's so well. It's like a salamander. You cut off a salamander limb and please don't
do that experiment because other people are doing it anyway. And it grows back.
And it knows how much to grow back. When the cell is perfectly functioning, it knows how
much to grow. Well, cancer loses that ability.
That is one of the hallmarks of cancer.
It just keeps growing.
It doesn't grow faster, by the way.
That's a misnomer.
People think cancers grow faster than non-cancer.
There's no real evidence that that's the case.
They just don't stop growing.
The second property of cancer is the capacity to leave the site of origin,
go someplace else and take up residence.
So that's a metastasis.
That's the metastasis component.
So if you think about it for a minute, a cell that never stops replicating and has the
capacity to up and leave and move and take up residence is clearly different from the
cell itself.
So if I have a cell of colonic epithelium, the cell that lines the inside of my colon,
clearly got a set of proteins in it.
But if all of a sudden that thing can grow, grow, grow, grow, grow, not stop, not stop,
not listen to the signal, and then somehow wind its way into the liver and just keep growing and
growing and growing, it must have different proteins. So the question then becomes, why does
cancer even exist? How has our immune system not figured out a way to just silence this and eradicate it
the way it does to virtually every virus you encounter?
And to me, this is one of the most interesting questions in all of biology, and it really
comes down to how clever cancer is, unfortunately, how evolutionarily clever it is.
It basically does a lot of things to trick the immune system.
So it has its own secretory factors that tamp down the immune system. It grows in an environment
because of its nature. So one of the things that's long understood about cancer is it's heavily
glycolytic. And when something is heavily glycolytic, it's going glucose to
pyruvate to lactate nonstop. There are lots of reasons for that. I think there's more than one.
What does that afford it? Does that afford it a migratory potential?
No, so it's super interesting. So that's the effect that what I just described is called the
Warburg effect. And when Warburg proposed this, which God was probably in the 1920s, it was before World War II.
He thought the mitochondria of cancer cells were defective.
So he proposed that the cancer cells' mitochondria don't work, hence they have to undergo glycolysis.
They can't undergo aerobic metabolism.
We now know that that's not the case.
So we now know that the Warburg
effect or the Warburg effect, if I'll refer to him correctly by his name, almost assuredly
does not have to do with defective mitochondria. Others have proposed several mechanisms. I
think there's probably more than one thing going on. So a paper that came out in 2009,
very influential paper by a guy named Matt Vanderheiden, Craig Thompson, and Lou Cantley
proposed that the reason that cancer
cells do the Warburg effect is that they're not optimizing for energy, they're optimizing
for cellular building blocks.
And if you do the mass balance, it completely makes sense.
Dividing cells need building blocks more than energy.
And glycolysis, while very inefficient for generating ATP, is much more efficient at
generating substrate
to make more cells.
But another proposed mechanism is exactly at this one.
Glycolysis lowers the surrounding pH because of lactate.
Lactate attracts hydrogen, pH goes down, and guess what that does to the immune system?
Detracts the immune system.
So it's also a way to hide from the immune system.
Like a pH cloaking, leveraging pH to cloak the signal that
the immune system would otherwise see. Yep. And then when you layer on top of that, that it knows
how to secrete things like IL-10, TGF-beta, all of these other secretory factors that also inhibit
the immune system, basically, it's figured out a way to kind of hide itself from the immune system.
The way you describe it, cancer sounds like a virus. Dr. Justin Marchegiani Yes.
Dr. Justin Marchegiani Sounds a lot like a virus. And that leads me to ask,
are there any examples of contagious cancers? I recall seeing some studies about these little
critters down in Australia, Tasmanian devils that like they would scratch each other and fight,
as Tasmanian devils do. They're actually quite cute. And they would get cancers and tumors
growing on their faces. It was like a literal physical interaction that could transmit cancer from one animal to the next.
So it's less that there are viruses that cause cancer. So in that sense, you could argue yes,
there are contagious cancers. Well, HPV. Sure. Yeah, HPV, Hep B, Hep C. But there are even cancers
like cutaneous cancers that arise from viruses.
But I don't know if that's quite the same as what you're saying.
What you're saying is an important point.
We don't want to go down the rabbit hole of the HPV, but that's increasing susceptibility
to cervical cancer.
Now there's a vaccine against HPV, right?
There wasn't when we were in college as we all knew.
There was no vaccine.
But direct transmission of cancers
from one organism to the next, more rare.
Yes.
So now, a moment ago I said there's this really incredible
thing about cancer that blows my mind
and about our immune system, which is that
at least 80% of solid organ tumors,
and we're gonna mostly talk about solid organ tumors
because that's where the field of oncology has made very little progress. So if you go back 50 years, where has
oncology made huge progress? It's made great progress in blood tumors, leukemias, and some
kinds of lymphomas. In fact, there's two kinds of lymphomas where the progress has been remarkable.
One has been in Hodgkin's lymphoma and the other has also been in immunotherapy,
has been in a type of B cell lymphoma where that B cell demonstrates or presents something
called a CD19 receptor. So in B cell lymphomas with CD19, there's a very unique niche immunotherapy.
We won't talk about that today called CAR T therapy that has got rid of those guys.
And then leukemias have also been pretty good.
But in solid organ tumors, there've been only two real breakthroughs in the last 50 years. One has
been the therapy for a certain type of testicular cancer. And it's really just a chemotherapy
cocktail that has been found to work really well. And the other has been in this really rare kind
of gastric cancer called the GI stromal tumor,
which happens to result from one mutation in a kinase pathway,
and there's one drug that can now target that, and it works. It's kind of amazing.
Cures that cancer.
Cures that. What I'm talking about are the cancers that kill virtually everybody else.
When you sort of line up, what are the big causes of cancer death?
Let's start at the top.
It's lung, it's then breast and prostate in men and women.
It's colorectal, it's pancreas.
Those are the big five.
More than 50% of cancer deaths in Americans come from those five.
These are what we call the solid epithelial tumors.
And you can march down the list and most cancers that most people are thinking of
are those cancers.
Well, here's the thing.
More than 80% of those cancers have antigens that are
recognized by the host's immune system. I will state it again because it is so
profound. 80% at least of those cancers actually generate an antigen, meaning a
little peptide in that cell gets presented to the T cell and it is recognizable.
And now the question is, why is that not sufficient to induce remission?
And the short answer is, there are not enough T cells that are able to act and or they are being sufficiently inhibited from acting, which gets me to the point of
this paper. One of the ways in which the body inhibits the immune system, which we should
remind ourselves is an important thing, right, is something called the checkpoint inhibitor.
Okay. So go back to that idea that I talked about before. You have an antigen presenting cell. It brings up an MHC receptor with a peptide on it,
and there is a T cell that is coming.
And I actually brought a diagram,
which I'm gonna link to this.
I don't wanna make this too complicated,
but I really think that this figure
is helpful to understand how these drugs work.
So the MHC receptor with the peptide is sitting there,
and it binds to the T-cell receptor on the T-cell, but there is another receptor on the
T-cell, a CTLA4 receptor. And that binds to a receptor that I won't bother naming now.
The names don't matter, but there's another receptor on the antigen presenting cell that binds to that. And that acts as the breaks in the reaction.
So CTLA4, which is on the T cell,
binds to another CD receptor on the antigen presenting cell
and it says, tamp down the response.
And the reason for that is,
we want to keep our immune system in check.
This basically is a way to keep our immune system and check.
This basically is a way of asking the immune system.
Cause remember, when the immune system sees that antigen,
it wants to go nuts.
It wants to start replicating and killing.
This is a CD8 T cell.
It is a targeted killer T cell.
The checkpoint says, let's double check that.
Let's be sure let's tamp down the response.
And as a result of that, a thought experiment emerged
which was what if we block CTLA4?
What if we block the checkpoint?
Could we unleash the immune system a little bit more?
And I will say this, at the time it was proposed,
it seemed a bit far-fetched.
Because of the complexity of the immune system, it seemed a bit far-fetched. Because of the complexity of the immune system,
it seemed a little far-fetched that simply blocking the checkpoint would have any effect.
It's also worth noting that prior to this, one immunotherapy had found some efficacy
which was trying the exact opposite strategy. Rather than blocking the inhibitor,
it was throwing more accelerant at the fire,
which was giving something called interleukin-2.
So interleukin-2 is, for lack of a better word,
candy and fuel for T cells.
So the idea was if we have T cells
that innately recognize a cancer antigen,
can we just give high doses of interleukin-2 and have them
undergo proliferation and response? And the answer turned out to be yes, but only in two cancers,
melanoma and kidney cancer, and only at very small levels. About 10% of the population would respond
to these things. Now look, that's 10% of people who were going to be dead within six months,
because these are devastating cancers.
And once they spread, there are no treatments
that have any efficacy whatsoever.
In fact, I think median survival
for metastatic melanoma at the time was probably four months.
So this was a very grim death sentence.
But the idea now was, what about doing
the exact opposite approach?
Instead of trying to throw more fire at the T cell,
what if we can take its breaks down?
Instead of giving more gas, let's give less breaks.
And there were some phase one studies
that demonstrated efficacy phase two.
And the paper I'm gonna talk about today
is the phase three study that compared
the first version of these.
So the drug we're gonna talk about today
is an anti-CTLA4 drug called Ipallilumab.
There is another drug out there
that came along shortly thereafter
that is an anti-PD1 drug.
So PD1 turns out to be another one of these checkpoints
on T cells.
And the Nobel Prize, by the way,
I think it was 2018 or 2019 in medicine or
physiology was actually awarded to the two scientists who discovered CTLA-4 and PD-1.
So I believe this is the only Nobel Prize in medicine for-CTLA4 to a placebo, and the placebo in this
case was not a real placebo. It was a peptide vaccine called GP100 to ask the question in
patients with metastatic melanoma, what would be the impact on median survival and overall survival.
Let's talk a little bit about the paper. So again, one of the funny things about this,
I used to read these papers a lot, Andrew. These used to be my bread and butter papers,
reading these like it's my hobby. And I don't read them that much anymore. So it was kind of
amazing how long it took me to remind myself of stuff I used to remember.
But you do have to kind of go back and read the methods and figure out who were the patients
in this, what was the eligibility criteria, why did they do it this way, and of course
it all kind of came back to me, but it took a minute.
So the first thing is these are all patients who had progressed through every standard
therapy.
So these are patients for whom there were no other options.
These patients either had very advanced stage three melanoma,
which means it was local regional melanoma,
but it couldn't be resected.
So an example of that would be a cancer
that was completely engulfing,
let's say the primary site was the cheek, and it
had completely grown into all of the surrounding soft tissue. It hadn't spread anywhere, but
it was all the lymph nodes of the neck, and I've seen patients like this, and it's just
completely disfiguring. And they'd already been through the standard chemotherapy, and
nothing was working, and the thing was growing. And then it was mostly made up of patients with stage 4 cancer.
Now melanoma has a very funny staging system.
So in cancer we typically talk about something called the TNM staging system.
It is the standard way that cancers are staged.
T refers to the tumor size, N refers to the lymph node status, and M refers to the presence
or absence of metastases.
And for most cancers, it is a very simple system.
T is typically a number, one, two, sometimes up to three and four.
N is typically zero, one or two, and M is zero or one.
Either there's no Mets or there are Mets.
So for example, in colorectal cancer, the T staging determines the depth in the colon
wall that it went.
N is, did it go to METs?
And I think in colon, I'm a little rusty on this.
I think colon has N01 or two, depending on how many lymph nodes.
And then M0, did it go to anything beyond that, like to the liver, lung, et cetera, or
not?
Melanoma is a bit more complicated.
It has M0, meaning no mats, but it also has M1A,
M1B, M1C, and M1D, and within each of those, it has a threshold for high and low lactate
dehydrogenase, or LDH. So it's both a staging based on imaging and biochemical, and the reason
for that is LDH level is such a strong prognostic indicator of survival.
In addition to M staging, higher LDH levels
tend to reflect more acidity,
which we talked about why that's problematic,
tends to reflect faster growing tumors,
higher turnover, higher metabolic activity.
M1A, let me see if I can remember this.
M1As are cancers that have metastasized
to surrounding soft tissue or soft tissue
or anywhere in the body, so anywhere else on the skin.
And you might think, well, that's kind of crazy,
like how does that happen?
And it's really bizarre.
You can have a patient who had a melanoma
that showed up in one part of their body
and then they have metastases on other parts of their skin.
M1B, and I always get B and C confused.
I think B is the lung.
So M1B is to the lung,
M1C is to any internal organ, so liver, et cetera,
and M1D is to the CNS.
And as those numbers increase,
as those letters increase,
the prognosis gets lower and lower and lower.
So one of the first things I always look at
when I look at a paper like this is,
tell me about the patient population.
What was the breakdown of patients?
And in table one, so that's again,
in clinical papers like this,
table one is always, always, always baseline characteristics.
Oh, I should mention one other thing, Andrew.
This was done as a three
to one to one randomization. So again, in the simplest form, the study would have two
groups. We're going to just have a treatment group and a placebo group. But in this arm,
you had three groups with one of them being the placebo. The placebo got just GP 100,
which is just a cancer vaccine.
By the way, this is a cancer vaccine that never showed any efficacy.
So it was a cancer vaccine that had been tested both with interleukin II directly,
and as an adjuvant for patients who had melanoma resected, who were tumor-free,
and then given the vaccine as adjuvants to see did that have an effect on outcomes and it didn't.
So it's kind of a known placebo. So you had that group, then you had the anti-CTLA-4 group,
and then you had anti-CTLA-4 plus GP100. What's the rationale? For the 3 to 1 to 1,
it's basically it increases statistical power. This total study was a little under 700 people.
They put 400 in the anti-CTLA4 plus GP100 group and then little over 130 in each of the other
two groups.
So you're always going to be able to make these two comparisons.
What you can check by doing this is, is there any effective GP100 in this setting, which had never been done
before.
So again, GP100 is a known protein expressed by melanoma.
And all of these people were haplotyped to make sure that their immune system would
recognize it.
And the question was, would giving people anti-CTLA4, i.e., taking the breaks off their
immune system with or without
GP100 make a difference. Going through this, you can see it sort of skews about 60% to
40% male to female. They talk about something called the ECOG performance status. That
refers to how healthy a patient is coming in. So ECOG0 is no limitations whatsoever,
which is kind of amazing when you really consider something. I think this speaks to just how devastating this disease is.
These are patients who all have like six months to live.
A year max, and yet look at this, 58 to 60% of them
have no limitation on their quality of life
at this very moment.
That's going to change dramatically absent a cure here.
And then ECOG one has some limitation.
And you can see that ECOG1 plus ECOG0 is
basically 98% of the population. You can see the staging there. So again, very, very few of these
patients are the M0 category. M0s are people who have stage 3 disease that is so aggressive it
can't be resected. That's about 1%. but the majority of these people are the M1As,
M1Bs, M1Cs.
So these are people with very aggressive cancers.
You can also see that about 10 to 15% of these people
also have CNS metastases.
Again, the poorest prognosis of the poor,
and then you can see about 40% of them
have the LDH level above cutoff. All of this is to say
we're talking about a group of patients who have a very high likelihood of not surviving
more than a year. It would be very unlikely that many of these patients would survive more than a
year. So basically more than 70% of these people have visceral metastases. A third have high LDH and more than 10% have brain mets.
They've also all progressed through standard therapy.
Dr. Justin Marchegiani Yeah.
And the chemo for melanoma can be a toxic chemo that really just doesn't really do
anything.
Dr. Justin Marchegiani So is it commonplace to use a treatment
that failed in clinical trials as a placebo in
these sorts of studies?
Yeah, it's interesting.
You're referring, obviously, to the GP100.
I think the thinking was okay.
It hasn't been effective in other treatments, for example, when combined with IL-2 or as
an adjuvant, but never before has it been tried with a checkpoint
inhibitor, which is the technical term for this type of drug. I think there was also some belief
that it would be easier to enroll patients. I don't think they stated this, but that's often the
case. It would be easier to enroll patients if they would know that even in the placebo arm,
they're still getting an active agent. Got it. And I suppose there's always the possibility that the combination of the
failed drug with a new drug would work. So you're increasing the probability for novel discovery.
For sure. And again, if you go back to the randomization of three to one to one,
it's really only one-fifth or 20% of the participants that would get just the GP100.
So in other words, you're basically telling people when they come into this study,
there's an 80% chance you're going to get anti-CTLA4. That's a much better set of odds than your typical
study where you're going to be 50% likely to get the agent of interest.
Right. And people who are literally dying of cancer, they don't want to be in the control group.
Right.
So, the primary outcome for this study actually changed in the study.
Now, they have to get permission to do that.
So the original primary endpoint was the best overall response rate.
So I have to explain how response rates are measured.
This is a bit complicated.
Remember, all of these patients by definition
have measurable, visible cancer,
by visible either on the surface of their body,
but more likely on an MRI or CT scan.
So all of these patients had to be scanned,
head to toe within 12 weeks of enrollment.
Again, there's another thing I should point out here,
which I know you understand,
but it's always worth reminding people. When a study like this takes place, it usually takes
place over many years. And so it's not the case that all 700 of these patients who were enrolled
on the same day and finished observing them on the same day. No, no, no, this took place for a
very long period of time. This took place across tens of centers. I can't remember if this was just
globally or across the world. It might have been across the world.
And so every center really needs to adhere
to a very strict protocol.
And you have a central organization that is running this.
So you have a drug company,
I think this is Bristol Myers Squibb that makes the drug,
they provide the drug.
And then you have a CRO, a clinical research organization
that is basically managing the trial and the trial is being
done at cancer centers all over the world or all over the country.
And enrollment, I think, began in 2008 for this.
No, no, I think it completed in 2008.
It probably started in about 2004, 2005.
And therefore, you had to kind of have real clear protocols around this.
So a complete response is the easier of these to understand.
A complete response is everything vanishes completely.
That's very rare in cancer therapy.
So instead, what we look for is a partial response.
A partial response, and there are really different ways to define this.
There are different criteria, but this is the most common way you define a partial response. A partial response is at least a 50% reduction by diameter, because
remember in this type of imaging, you're looking at 2D versus 3D. So if you're looking at a
lung lesion and it's this big, you know, if it's two centimeters long, it has to go to
at least one centimeter in diameter.
So it's a 50% reduction at least of every single lesion with no new lesions appearing
and no lesions growing.
So it's a very strict criteria.
Right, again, CR means everything vanishes, PR means at least a 50% by diameter, which
by the way is a much bigger reduction in terms of tumor
volume when you consider the linear versus the third power relationship of length and volume
of every single lesion with nothing new appearing regardless of how small and no lesion growing.
So that's a PR. So you basically have no response, progression. We talk about those together,
and then partial response and complete
response. So initially, the authors of this study, the primary endpoint of this was going
to be the best overall response rate. So what was the proportion of patients that hit PR?
What was the proportion that hit a CR? That's very common in this type of paper where the
outcomes are typically so dire. I don't remember when this study ended,
but the amendment was made to change the primary endpoint to overall survival at some point during
the study. And by the way, that tends to be the metric everybody cares most about. So the overall
survival for metastatic melanoma is zero, with the exception of people who respond to
interleukin-2, high-dose interleukin-2, and that will boost the overall survival
rate to somewhere between 8 and 10 percent. Very, very low. These patients, many of
whom had already taken and progressed through interleukin-2. Let me refresh my memory on what percentage of those patients.
About a quarter of these patients had already taken high dose interleukin-2
and by definition, the fact that they're in this study means they had already
progressed through that. That treatment had failed.
Just reiterate the state these patients are in.
So now let's look at figure one. So again, I'll describe it because I realize many people
are just listening to us. All of this will be available both in the video and then we'll
link to the paper. So figure one is a figure that probably looks really familiar to people
who look at any data that deal with survival. It's called the Kaplan-Meier survival curve.
So on the x-axis for this curve is time,
and time here is shown in months.
And on the y-axis is the overall survival,
the very top 100% at the bottom, 0%.
And it has three graphs or three curves
that are superimposed on one another
for each of
the three groups. Again, the control group, which is the GP100, the anti-CTLA4 group
by itself, and the anti-CTLA4 plus GP100. And one of the characteristics of a
Kaplan-Meier curve is by definition they have to be decreasing in a monotonic
fashion because it's cumulative overall survival. That just means it can't come down and go back up.
Nobody comes back to life.
So once a person dies, they are censored from the study
and the curve drops and drops and drops.
And you can see that they kind of highlight,
and I actually think it makes the graph a little harder
to read when they put some of those marks on there.
But what really becomes clear when you look at
this is that there's a clear distinction between the curve for the placebo group, the GP100 group,
and the other two, the two treatment groups. Now, you'll note at the very end that the two
treatment groups appear to separate a little bit. I'll talk about that in a second. So when I look at these, Andrew,
the first thing I always turn my attention to,
I can't resist, I have to look at the right hand side
of the graph.
Because what is that really telling me?
The tale of this is showing me the true overall survival.
And I wanna sort of figure out what is going on.
So in the GP 100 group, which is the placebo group,
it is kind of amazing to think that there is still
one person who is alive at 44 months. It's amazing. I mean, it's both sobering and amazing that like
one person made it to 44 months. The next thing I ask myself is, well, how long did half of the
people make it? That's called median survival. And to do that, you go up to the y-axis and you
draw a little line from the 50 over and then you bring that down. That's awfully low. Yeah.
In fact, the table will tell us exactly what that is because I think it's really hard to
eyeball that stuff. So let's go to... So there's always a table that will accompany these things.
And let's pull up that table.
I've got this paper spread out over so many things.
Let's adverse events.
Where's our survival table here?
Two subgroup analysis of overall survival.
It would probably be helpful if I stapled these things together
because it would be easier.
Well, this is always a trade-off.
Since this is a Journal Club episode,
I will say that stapling helps,
but it also prevents
one from separating things out, writing in the margins.
I like these little mini clips.
No financial relationship to the mini clips either.
Just have to state that because if you don't say that, people go, oh, you must have a stake
in these mini clips.
I like these little mini clips.
In fact, I'm such a nerd.
I always have one of these pilot V5 V7s on
my pocket or my hip. And then my pockets are always filled with these little mini clips.
But then again, I have a friend who's a musician, he's always reigning guitar picks. As far
as occupational hazards go of being a nerd, these mini clips are-
I'm a big fan of the mini clip as well. Yeah.
I went without it today. All right. So thank you. Yes. Table two. Let's look at table two
while looking at the Kaplan MeMeier curve, because now this allows
us to see a couple of things.
By the way, remember how I said there's like that one person who is still alive in the
treatment group?
Well, you can tell that he's not a complete responder.
He or she is not a complete responder, because under evaluation of therapy in table two,
it says best overall response,
and it says complete responders, zero.
So there was zero complete responders in the placebo.
There were two partial responders.
Again, a partial responder is some lesions got smaller,
some got bigger, stable disease.
It didn't really change that much,
and progressive disease is obviously
it went beyond. And when you say partial response, like lesions
got smaller, are they literally just tracing the circumference of one of these skin lesions
and saying, okay, got bigger, smaller, like just morphology?
Yep. Yep.
Gosh, this feels so crude in terms of like, I mean, it makes total sense, but like in
terms of modern medicine, oh, like your total sense, but like in terms of modern medicine
Oh, like your lesion grew from like three millimeters to six millimeters and they're literally like drawing little boundaries around little blotches on the skin
Yeah, you're putting a little measuring tape on them
Now again, most of these are happening in the radiology suite because most of the disease for these patients is inside the body
Remember more than 70% of these patients had visceral metastases.
So liver, soft tissue, lung, brain. In fact,
if you include lung, liver, brain and viscera, it's pretty much all the patients.
So most of this is looking at a CT scan or an MRI for the brain. Okay. So that's kind of the first thing that comes up.
The median response rate should be shown pretty prominently here. So I'm
looking through this and... where is median response? Maybe it's shown in a
different table. Not disease control rate. Time to progression. I remember it's
about 10 months but maybe that's just in the text. Yeah, here it is.
I thought this would be in a table, but it's on page 715 of the paper.
It just reports it.
So I'm sorry, I misspoke.
The 10 months was for the anti-CTLA4 plus GP100 and 6.4 months for the GP100 alone.
That's the control.
And then 10.1 for the NTC TLA4 alone.
And again, I'm just always doing this.
I'm kind of going back to the paper to be like,
does that make sense?
And yeah, you called it, right?
You said median survival was about eight.
Well, it turns out it's actually like six and change
because it has that little ding in it.
And it's out to a little past 10 on the two others.
So the net takeaway here is,
again, just to put that in English, because it's so profound, 50% of the patients in the control
group were dead in six months. 50% of the patients in the treatment group, both treatment groups,
were dead in 10 months. So what that means in cancer speak is these drugs extended median survival
by four months. Now that's an important concept. When we think about how has cancer therapy
changed over the past 50 years, median survival for metastatic cancer has increased across
the board. So a person today with metastatic colorectal cancer or a woman today with metastatic cancer has increased across the board. So a person today with metastatic colorectal cancer or a woman today with metastatic breast
cancer or a person with metastatic lung cancer, these people will live longer with those diseases
today thanks mostly to treatments.
This is not an early detection lead time bias issue.
This is treatments are allowing people to
live longer and that's an important part of the story but it's only half of the
story yet it often gets touted as the story. The other half of the story and
frankly the story that I think is more important is what is overall survival
doing and if you go back to those cancers, the answer is zero. Overall survival hasn't changed for solid epithelial tumors.
It was 0% in 1970 and it's 0% today.
Everyone dies.
Everyone dies from metastatic solid organ tumors.
There's those niche examples I gave you.
Testicular cancer is now an exception.
GI stromal tumors would be an exception. And
I'm not including leukemias and lymphomas where now there are exceptions.
Dr. Justin Marchegiani Not to try and be overly optimistic, but if I look at the graph in
figure one and I look out at the tail of the graph.
Dr. Justin Marchegiani That's right.
Dr. Justin Marchegiani And for those that are just listening, what I see and I'm far less,
far less familiar with this type of work and this
analyzing these type of data.
But what I see is that people in the placebo group, they're all dead except that one.
They're basically all dead at 44 months.
But when I look at how long it takes for everyone to be dead in the treatment groups, looks
like 53, 54 months or so.
And they're not dead.
That's the point.
They're hanging in there, right?
So, because, you know, an extra, somebody who lost both of my scientific advisors, two
of the three, the other one is suicide.
We've talked about this before.
But the other two to different cancers both had the Brackayte mutation by the way. You know, an extra eight to 10 months with your kids or with your spouse or to quote
unquote get your affairs in order is a big deal.
I mean, it's still depressing in the sense that nobody survives long term, but you know,
an extra 10 months as long as one is not miserable in that time, completely miserable.
I mean, that's extra 10 months of living.
And what's interesting here is the observation period stops
and some of these patients are still going.
What you're highlighting is kind of the point I wanna make,
which is overall survival is the most important metric
and it's the highest bar, make no mistake about it,
and it's certainly not the bar any drug company
is ever going to want to talk about
for a cancer drug.
But why not?
None of them work.
They only want to talk about cures.
They don't want to talk about extending media and survival.
They only want to talk about extending media and survival.
And there are lots of people out there that are on this platform.
I don't need to get onto it, but we'll say, look, it's a real racket in oncology today
where drugs that are extending
median survival by four weeks are being put on the market at a tune of $50,000 to $100,000
per treatment.
That's not uncommon in oncology.
There was one drug that was approved for pancreatic cancer.
I believe it extended median survival by nine days and it cost $40,000.
And it's being advertised as significant.
That was a statistical significant improvement in media and survival.
It's really understandable why people are very skeptical of the pharma industry.
And I think a much more nuanced view is necessary.
Clearly I don't think pharma is all bad, but I really understand why people lose faith
in pharma when these types of products somehow make regulatory approval.
Does insurance cover these kinds of drugs?
It can, in fact, it often does.
It depends on the FDA approval, of course,
and the indication, but a lot of times they do.
Yeah, there's a societal cost to these things,
but there's also a patient cost.
So a lot of times insurance doesn't fully cover it
and a patient has to bear the cost difference.
And on top of that, you alluded to this a second ago,
which is what if your quality of life
is dramatically compromised as a result of this treatment?
And yes, statistically, you're gonna live
nine days longer or three weeks longer,
but at what cost to your health
in those final remaining days?
And by the way, you're potentially straddling
your loved ones with enormous debt in your absence.
So it's a super complicated topic.
There's a dignity component too.
I mean, I've seen this in people dying, you know, at some point they become such a diminished
version of their former selves that they don't want to be seen by people that way.
Yep.
So what is exciting about this drug, this paper is not the one that shows it.
The reason I chose this paper, Andrew,
is because it was the first approval.
A second drug came along that is an anti-PD1 drug.
That drug is called K-truda.
That drug turned out to be even better.
Has even a greater response rate,
both in terms of median survival and overall survival.
But this was the landmark paper.
I also have a slight bias here,
and I'll disclose in a moment why,
but I think it just talks about very interesting biology.
Let's talk about a couple of things
that stuck out to me in this paper.
The first thing that stuck out to me,
and the authors didn't comment on it unless they did,
and I missed it, is look at Figure 2.
So Figure 2 is the subgroup analyses where you're sort of showing a similar graph to
the one you showed earlier.
You show the response rate or the change in response between the groups and then you put
the error bars on it and this is where we talk about how well it's a 95% confidence interval so it doesn't touch
the unity line. So these are called tornado plots typically. What you'll
notice is that in the top it's comparing the anti-CTLA4 with GP100 versus the
GP100 and in the bottom you're looking at the anti-CTLA4 versus the GP100. So at
a glance you can see GP100 is not doing anything. That's the first takeaway of
comparing A to B. What I find most interesting is look at the subgroup
analysis of females. Notice that in females while there's a trend towards
risk reduction and this is risk reduction for overall mortality. So again I
just want to restate that.
The primary outcome of this trial was changed to overall survival, which I think is the
better outcome, by the way.
And overall, for all patients, when you compare anti-CTLA4 plus placebo versus placebo, there
was a 31% risk reduction in overall mortality.
That's the mathematical interpretation
of what you're seeing at the tail end
of that Kaplan-Meier curve.
Living longer.
Living longer.
And it sounds like a big difference.
Sounds like a big difference.
Sometimes it is a big difference.
Well, it is for those people
because you're really looking at basically
0% surviving in the placebo group versus 20% of people are still alive at 56
months in the treatment group. But look, that means 80% have died. But notice that, and sorry,
when you just look at the anti-CTLA4 plus GP100 in the subgroup B, that hazard ratio is even showing more compression.
It's a 36% reduction in risk of death.
But notice that the females did not reach significance.
So in the first group, they barely do.
And you can see that because the confidence interval runs from 0.55 to 0.92.
And notice the error bar almost touches the line and in the second one it does
not reach significance at all. So I actually went and kind of did a little reading on this after
and I said, hey, was this an outlier study? And it turned out it wasn't and that about half the
studies of anti-CTLA-4 did indeed find that the drug was less effective in women than men,
before did indeed find that the drug was less effective in women than men.
Which I found interesting. Now, I couldn't find any great explanation for it, but the most plausible
explanations fit into two categories.
The first are maybe there are differences in the immune response to the
drug if you're a man or a woman.
The second comes down to dosing.
I should have said this at the outset, but of course these drugs are not like a pill where it's like
Everybody gets 50 milligrams of this. They're all dose based on weight. So this study is dosed
I believe at 3 milligrams per kilogram and because most men are heavier than women men are getting a higher dose than women and
Weight and body surface area and immune system, like these things
are not all perfectly linear. I kind of wonder if this difference is simply explained by
men on average getting a higher dose than women.
Last thing I want to talk about here is in table three. So table three, always an important table
to look at in any paper is what are the adverse outcomes?
Where are the adverse effects of the drug?
I've spent a little bit of time with this
and I confess I definitely don't want cancer
to the extent that I can avoid it,
but this table made me wonder whether or not
I would also want to just avoid cancer treatment
given the life extension provided.
I mean, these adverse events are pretty uncomfortable.
Just to put in perspective, and you always have to kind of be mindful of how
many of these adverse events are occurring in people just because their disease is progressing.
So the first thing I always want to look at is total adverse events in all three groups.
So grade three and grade four are real toxicities.
Grade four toxicity is life-threatening toxicity, by the way.
Grade three is pretty significant toxicity.
Grade one and two, that's not that severe.
The little rash, put some corticosteroids on it,
it went away, kind of thing.
Okay, so in the treatment plus GP 100 group,
98.4% of people reported some event.
So all but 1.6%.
In the NTC-TLA4 group alone, it was 96.7%,
so only 3.1% did not.
But in the placebo group, it's 97%.
So it's more than to keep in mind,
everybody's having some adverse effect.
Okay, well, what if you say, well,
let's just limit it to the most severe events? Well, let's just talk about grade four toxicities. There were 6.1% of those in the placebo group,
8.4% in the anti-CTLA-4 group, and 6.8% in the combined group. So,
not a huge difference in grade four toxicity. Meaning that whatever adverse events are occurring
may not be related to the treatment. occurring may not be related to the treatment.
They may not be related to the treatment.
If you think about it and it's a very awful, sad, morbid thought to imagine, you're looking
at the adverse responses of people more than 80% of whom died during the course of a very,
very short study.
And so it's very difficult to disentangle what effects or what side effects a person
is having
just from that process as they are from the actual treatment.
But if there is an area where there's a really clear difference, it's down in the autoimmune
category. So if you look at any immune related events, you can see that in the
can see that in the anti-CTLA4 plus GP100 group, it's about 60% in both of those treatment groups versus 30%.
And if you look at the grade 3 and 4 toxicities, it's 10% in the anti-CTLA4, 15% in the anti-CTLA4
alone group, and only 3% in the treatment.
So that's a real difference.
But it makes sense that people getting this drug plus placebo or just the drug would have
autoimmune issues because this is an immunotherapy.
It's an immunomodulator.
In fact, what is it doing?
It is taking the breaks off the immune system.
But then again, the things that they list out, Pruridus, is that an irritation of the
skin?
Yeah, irritation of the skin.
I'm not a physician, but I know that any idus is going to be like an inflammation.
And Oma, unfortunately, likely a cancer or cell replication.
Look at the gastrointestinal differences.
And the vitiligo, so 3.7%, 2.3%, 0.8%.
The GI stuff is the most common stuff you're going to see there. Those are
the really big ones. And of course there's diarrhea and there's
diarrhea. Oh yeah. Like there's traveler's diarrhea, there's ate an overly spicy large
meal the night before diarrhea and then there's like can't really do anything besides make
trips back and forth to the bathroom diarrhea. Put it this way, there's colitis here is diarrhea
so significant these patients require IV fluids.
Now, what you don't see here is how many of these patients actually required corticosteroids
to reverse the autoimmunity.
So a lot of times what will happen here in these studies or with these drugs is the autoimmunity
becomes so significant that you have to stop the drug and give corticosteroids.
Do the exact opposite.
You now have to shut the immune system down.
So you just took the brakes off it with the drug
And now you need to shut it down with corticosteroids when I was in my fellowship. I
wrote a paper about
autoimmunity
correlating with response rate in anti-ctLA4
Early on this was during the phase 2 work. So the NCI was a very early
adopter of participating in
these trials. And it was observed that, or at least hypothesized, this is what the paper
basically wrote about, which was, is there any correlation between autoimmunity and
response? And it turned out the answer was yes. There was a very strong correlation.
So there was no difference in autoimmunity between the doses. So the
paper we wrote was two dosing schedules. So it was basically the full dose, the three
milligrams per kilogram versus a low dose, one milligram per kilogram. This is a phase
two trial. Those are your two arms. There turned out to be no difference in autoimmunity
between them, but there was a big difference between the response rate that tied to auto-immunity.
In other words, auto-immunity predicted response.
Now I think over time these investigators, the doctors who administer these treatments
are getting better and better at catching these things earlier because these auto-immune
conditions can actually be devastating. So on a very personal note, when Kytruda came out,
I want to say it was around 2000, 2013, 2014, thereabouts, again, it was for treatment of
metastatic melanoma. I want to come back and explain why melanoma gets all of the attention
in immunotherapy conditions. I'll state that. But anyway, a friend of mine got pancreatic cancer
and he got the bad type of pancreatic cancer.
So this is adenocarcinoma in the pancreas.
This is a non-survivable type of cancer.
Furthermore, his was unresectable.
Can you explain what that is for people?
About 20% of people who have pancreatic cancer
technically have it in a way
where you could still take out the head of the pancreas.
Right, the Whipple procedure.
The Whipple procedure.
Now, tragically, most of those patients will still recur.
My understanding is that pancreatic cancer
progresses from anterior to posterior in the pancreas,
and that the Whipple is a removal
of the front end of the anterior.
That's the Whipple procedure.
So if the cancer has progressed far enough
caudal into the posterior pancreas, then there's nothing left to cut out basically.
Can we survive without a pancreas for any amount of time?
Oh yeah, absolutely.
So why don't they just remove the whole pancreas?
Oh, that's my point. It's already micromitastasized. The surgical procedure is not the challenge
anymore. It used to be. So at Johns Hopkins, which was one of the hospitals where this was pioneered,
the 30 day mortality for a Whipple procedure was, I don't know, 80%.
Whoa.
And the reason was to figure out how to suture a pancreas to the
bowel.
So the pancreas is such an awful organ to operate on because its enzymes are
designed to digest anything and everything.
So imagine now you have to cut the pancreas in half, take out the head of the pancreas
with the duodenum, and then somehow sew that open half of a raw pancreas to the end of the
jejunum and not let it digest itself.
So when did Hopkins figure this out?
No, the first one was actually done by A.O AO Whipple, but yes, at Hopkins is where they
figured out the way to put drains in, the surgical technique, how to do it in two layers,
what type of stitches to use.
All of the nuances of this were worked out in a few places, but I would say Hopkins more
than any place else.
And are there physicians who like try this on non-human primates or something? Or is
this always just done on patients?
Well, nowadays, I mean, put it this way, even 25 years ago at a major center like Hopkins,
the mortality of that procedure was less than 1%.
So there have been some victories.
Well, yes, but here's my point. That's no longer the bottleneck. Taking out the pancreas safely,
as complicated and challenging as that is,
and if you need a whip-up procedure,
you only want to have it done by someone
who just does that night and day.
You don't want weekend warriors doing it.
That's not why people are living or dying.
They're dying because the cancer just comes back.
It was already spread to the liver by the time you did it,
you just didn't realize it yet.
So whether you took out the whole pancreas, or the head out the whole pancreas or the head of the pancreas or the tail of
the pancreas, the location of the tumor is predictive of survival only in the extent
that it basically is a window into how soon did symptoms occur.
So pancreatic cancers in the tail tend to be more fatal even though they're way easier surgically to take out because
by the time you develop symptoms of a tail, pancreas cancer, it's a big cancer.
I was going to ask this question later, but I'll just ask it now. Given the link between
the immune system and these cancers, is there an idea in mind that people who are, let's say, 40 and older or 50 and older who
don't yet, they're not diagnosed with any cancer, would periodically just stimulate their
immune system to wipe out whatever early cancers might be cropping up?
Just take a drug to just ramp up the immune system, even to the point where you start
having a little diarrhea, maybe a few skin rashes, and then come off the drug, just basically to fight back whatever
little cell growths are starting to take place in skin or liver for three weeks out of each
year.
I mean, why not?
Yeah, it's an interesting question.
I've never thought of it through that lens.
I suppose the question is, what can we do to keep our immune systems as healthy as possible
as we age?
Stay on a normal circadian schedule. There's evidence for that.
Sure. No, there's evidence that certainly if it promotes sleep, anything that promotes better
rest is going to promote immune health. Because if you ask the macro question, which is like,
why does the prevalence of cancer increase so dramatically with age? There are certain diseases
where it's really obvious why the prevalence of the disease dramatically with age. There are certain diseases where it's really obvious why the prevalence of the disease
increases with age.
Yeah, like age-related macular degeneration.
Sure.
Or cardiovascular disease is by far the most obvious because it's an area under the curve
exposure problem.
The more exposure to lipoproteins and the more the endothelium gets damaged, the more
likely you are to accumulate plaque.
And again, it totally makes sense why
10 year olds don't have heart attacks and 80 year olds do. But when you sort of acknowledge that,
well, hey, anybody's accumulating genetic mutations, we're always surrounded and being
bombarded by things that are altering the genome of ourselves. Is it simply a stochastic process
where the longer you live, the more of these mutations you're going to occur until at some point one of them just wins. I think that's got to be a
big part of it. But I think another part of it, clearly I'm not alone in thinking this,
is that our immune system is getting weaker and weaker as we age. People become more susceptible
to infections as they get older. And I think that that's equally playing a role in our
susceptibility to cancer. So yeah, I think the question is,
how do you modulate immunity as you age? And to me, that's one of the most interesting things
about rapamycin potentially is that when taken the right way, it seems to enhance cellular immunity,
which again, that's potentially a really big deal. Again, at least in short-term human experiments,
in response to vaccination, it's enhancing vaccine response.
So the question is, would that translate into cancer?
Nobody knows.
Could that be one of the reasons why animals treated with rapamycin live longer and get less cancer?
Don't know.
You know, it could also be that it's at a fundamental level that's targeting nutrient sensing.
Where I was going with that story was that...
And maybe I'll back up for a moment, why melanoma?
So we didn't really know this 30, 40 years ago in the early days of immunotherapy. But what we
know now is that most cancers probably have about 40 mutations in them. That's like ballpark, 40,
50 mutations is standard fare for a cancer. But melanoma happens to
be one of the cancers that has many, many more mutations. And the more mutations a cancer
has, the more likelihood that it will produce an antigen that's recognized as non-self.
And that's why in the early days of immunotherapy, the only things that worked were IL-2 against
metastatic melanoma
and kidney cancer, because kidney cancer turned out to also be one of those cancers that,
for reasons that are not clear, produced hundreds of mutations.
And so it's no surprise that the early studies of checkpoint inhibitors were also done in
metastatic melanoma, where you basically have more shots on goal.
Again, if I'm going to take the brakes off my immune system,
I might as well do it in an environment
where there are more chances for my T cells
to find something to go nuts against.
It's 2013, 2014, and this friend of mine
who has something called Lynch syndrome,
which is one of those few hereditary
or germline mutations that
results in a huge increase in the risk of cancer. He had already had colon cancer
at about the age of 40 and had survived that. It was a stage 3 cancer but he had
survived it. Well, now five years later had developed pancreatic cancer and when
he went to see the surgeon they said, there's nothing we can do. It's too advanced.
To put that in perspective, that is a death sentence.
That's a six month survival.
And at around that time, there was a study
that had come out in the New England Journal of Medicine
that had talked about how patients with Lynch syndrome
had lots of mutations.
So we talked with his doctors about the possibility
of enrolling him in one of the Kytruda trials.
There was one going on, I think, at Stanford.
The thinking being, well, you would want to target a checkpoint inhibitor against somebody who has a lot of mutations.
And even though typically we don't see that in pancreatic cancer, his is a unique variant of it because it's based on this.
And so sure enough, he was tested for these mismatch repair genes.
He had them enrolled in the trial and amazingly had not only a complete regression of his cancer
and he's still alive and cancer-free today 10 years later.
But the treatment worked so well at activating his immune system that his immune system completely destroyed his pancreas.
So now,
he is effectively had a pancreatectomy based on his immune system. So now he actually has type 1
diabetes. He has no pancreas.
What?
Jackson's insulin to deal with that or implant.
No, no. He has to use insulin just like someone with type 1 diabetes.
Had to pick being alive with type 1 diabetes.
Yeah, of course. No comparison. But it's just an interesting example of how remarkable this treatment was able to work.
You could completely unleash the immune system of a person and you eradicate the cancer and the
rest of the cells around it. There are many organs we could live without. There are certain organs
you can't live without. You can't live without your heart, lungs, liver, kidneys.
But many things that kill people arise from organs,
the breast, you could live without all breast tissue.
Prostate, you can live without all the prostate tissue.
I mean, would you choose to live without these?
Right, but I'm saying if you had metastatic cancer
and you had a bullet that could selectively target a tissue,
you would take it.
And right now, the only tissue we can do that against
is a CD19 B cell.
And that's what those CAR T cells are.
So right now these are not tissue specific treatments,
but they're mutation specific.
The last thing I'll say about this paper
that I found interesting, I was looking for it
and I was surprised they didn't at all comment
on if there was any correlation
between auto-immunity and response.
So they obviously acknowledged the auto-immunity in table three, but I would have loved to
have seen a statistical analysis that said, hey, is there any correlation between
response rate and autoimmunity? But they didn't comment to that effect. So we're
left wondering what the current state of that is. And I guess in summary I'll say
that the reason I thought this was an interesting
paper to present is that I still believe that immunotherapy is probably the most important
hope we have for treating cancer. And while I think we're still only scratching the surface of it,
collectively, the overall survival increase for patients with metastatic solid organ tumors
is about 8% better than it was 50 years ago. And virtually all of that has come from some
form of immunotherapy, I think is promising. And I think the holy grail, meaning the next step,
if you go back to where we started the discussion, is coming up with ways to engineer
T cells to be even better recognizers of antigens. And there's many ways to do that. One is to
directly engineer them. Another is to find T cells that have already migrated into tumors.
Those are called tumor infiltrating lymphocytes or TIL and expanding those and
engineering them to be better and younger.
Is it possible to engineer our own T cells to be more pH variant tolerant? Meaning, since
this cloaking of the local area by changing the pH, could we pull some T cells? I'm always
saying about the inoculation stuff,
like pull some T cells as part of our standard exam
when we're 30 and grow some up in an environment
that the pH is slightly more acidic than normal
and then reintroduce them to the body.
I mean, after all they are T cells,
in other words, give them a little opportunity
to evolve the conditions they can thrive in.
Or even just keep them in the freezer in case we need them.
Yes.
So, the interesting thing is I don't know that if you just got them to be comfortable
in a lower pH, it would be sufficient because there are still so many other things that
the cancer is doing as far as using other secreting factors, it seems that
by far the most potent thing comes down to expanding the number of T cells that recognize
the antigen and making sure that you can get that number big enough without aging them
too much.
So in some senses, it has become a longevity problem of T cells. The way to think about it is
you want an army of soldiers who are
wise enough to recognize the bad guys which comes with age but young enough to go and kill and
right now both extremes seem to be unhelpful. When you go and find tumor infiltrating lymphocytes in a tumor,
they're very wise. They've demonstrated that they can do everything. They can outmaneuver
the cancer, but they're too old to do anything about it. And when you take them out to try to
expand them by three logs, which is typically what you need to do, expand them by a thousand fold,
they can't do anything.
Darrell Bock And what about avoiding melanoma altogether? I mean,
obviously avoiding sunburn. Somehow
I got couched as anti-sunscreen and that is absolutely not true. I said some sunscreens
contain things that are endocrine disruptors and we're going to do a whole episode on sunscreen.
Maybe we could do some journal clubs on them.
I'm actually planning something on that as well. I wanted to do a deep dive on this.
And some dermatologists reached out, some very skilled dermatologists reached out and
said that indeed some sunscreens are downright dangerous, but of course melanoma is super
dangerous.
No one disputes physical barriers for sunscreen.
Everyone agrees that that is unlikely to have endocrine disruption.
So physical barriers are undisputed, but aside from limiting sunlight exposure to the skin,
what are some other risks for melanoma?
I mean, I think that's the biggest one. I do not believe that smoking poses a risk for
melanoma. And if it does, it's going to be very small. There are hereditary cases. So one needs
to be pretty mindful when taking a family history. And by the way, there are really weird genetic
conditions that link melanoma to other cancers, such as pancreatic cancer, by the way. So whenever I'm taking somebody's family history and I hear about somebody that had melanoma to other cancers such as pancreatic cancer, by the way. So,
whenever I'm taking somebody's family history and I hear about somebody that had melanoma
and someone that had pancreatic cancer, there's a couple genetic tests we'll look at to see
if that's a person that's particularly sensitive from a genetic predisposition. And by the
way, I think with melanoma, although it's not completely agreed upon, I think it's less
about sun exposure and more about sun burn.
And again, I'm sure there's somebody listening to this who will chime in and apply a more
nuanced response to that.
But there's a fundamental difference between a mountain in the sun, getting sun, making
some vitamin D, versus I'm getting scorched and undergoing significant UV damage.
There might also be something to be said for the time in one's life,
and I've certainly seen things that suggest that early repeated sunburns would be more of a risk.
So I think that's not a controversial point in the sense that who wants to be sunburned, right?
So it's like whatever one needs to do to be sunburned, whether it's being mindful of what the UV index is,
wearing the appropriate cover, wearing the appropriate sunscreen.
I also find the whole anti-sunscreen establishment
to be a little bit odd.
Well, the anti-sunscreen establishment is odd.
I'm trying to open the door for a nuanced discussion
about the fact that some sunscreens really do contain
things like oxybenzene and things that are real.
And you're spraying them on kids.
Yeah, yeah, yeah, but when you just look at the straight, you know, the good old fashioned mineral sunscreens.
Yeah, perfectly safe. Yeah.
As far as we know, dare we cross the seed oil debate into this. Some of the folks who are
really anti seed oil also claim that seed oils increase risk for sunscreen. Peter and I are
smiling because we have teed up a debate soon with some anti-seed oil and less anti-seed oil
experts. So that's forthcoming. That's going to be a fun one. We'll be doing all of that
with our shirts on. I really appreciate you walking us through this paper, Peter. I have
never looked at a paper on cancer and certainly not one like this. I learned a lot and it's
such an interesting
field obviously because of the importance of getting people with cancer to survive longer
and lead better lives, but also because of the interaction with the immune system. So
we learned some really important immunology.
Yeah. And this was great. I feel much more confident now in the belief that the exposure
to light early and late in the day can actually have
benefits.
And as I said, I think that there's some causality here and I think it shouldn't be ignored.
Well, this was our second journal club.
I look forward to our third.
Next time you'll go first.
We'll just keep alternating.
And we've also switched venues, but we both wore the correct shirt.
I hope people are learning and not just learning the information,
but learning how to parse and think about papers.
And I certainly learned from you, Peter.
Thank you so much.
Yeah, thanks, Andrew. This was great.
Thank you for listening to this week's episode of The Drive.
It's extremely important to me to provide all of this content
without relying on paid ads.
To do this, our work is made entirely possible
by our members.
And in return, we
offer exclusive member only content and benefits above and beyond what is available for free.
So if you want to take your knowledge of this space to the next level, it's our goal to
ensure members get back much more than the price of the subscription.
Premium membership includes several benefits. First, comprehensive podcast show notes that
detail every topic, paper, person,
and thing that we discuss in each episode. And the word on the street is, nobody's show notes
rival ours. Second, monthly ask me anything or AMA episodes. These episodes are comprised of
detailed responses to subscriber questions typically focused on a single topic and are designed to
offer a great deal of clarity and detail on topics of special interest to our members.
You'll also get access to the show notes for these episodes, of course.
Third, delivery of our premium newsletter, which is put together by our dedicated team
of research analysts.
This newsletter covers a wide range of topics related to longevity and provides much more
detail than our free weekly newsletter.
Fourth, access to our private podcast feed that provides you with access to every episode including AMAs,
Sons The Spiel you're listening to now, and in your regular podcast feed.
Fifth, the Quallies, an additional member-only podcast we put together that serves as a highlight
reel featuring the best excerpts from previous episodes of The Drive.
This is a great way to catch up on previous episodes without having to go back and listen
to each one of them.
And finally, other benefits that are added along the way.
If you want to learn more and access these member-only benefits, you can head over to
peteratea.com.com forward slash subscribe.
You can also find me on YouTube, Instagram, and Twitter, all with the handle peteratea.com.
You can also leave us a review on Apple podcasts or whatever podcast player you use.
This podcast is for general informational purposes only and does not constitute the practice
of medicine, nursing, or other professional health care services, including the giving of medical advice. No doctor-patient relationship is formed.
The use of this information and the materials linked to this podcast is at the user's own
risk. The content on this podcast is not intended to be a substitute for professional medical
advice, diagnosis, or treatment. Users should not disregard or delay in obtaining medical
advice from any medical condition they have and they should seek the
assistance of their health care professionals for any such conditions.
Finally, I take all conflicts of interest very seriously. For all of my
disclosures and the companies I invest in or advise, please visit peteratiamd.com forward slash about where I keep an up to date and active
list of all disclosures. you