The Peter Attia Drive - #303 - A breakthrough in Alzheimer’s disease: the promising potential of klotho for brain health, cognitive decline, and as a therapeutic tool for Alzheimer's disease | Dena Dubal, M.D., Ph.D.
Episode Date: May 27, 2024View the Show Notes Page for This Episode Become a Member to Receive Exclusive Content Sign Up to Receive Peter’s Weekly Newsletter Dena Dubal is a physician-scientist and professor of neurology ...at UCSF whose work focuses on mechanisms of longevity and brain resilience. In this episode, Dena delves into the intricacies of the longevity factor klotho: its formation and distribution in the body, the factors such as stress and exercise that impact its levels, and its profound impact on cognitive function and overall brain health. Dena shares insights from exciting research in animal models showing the potential of klotho in treating neurodegenerative diseases as well as its broader implications for organ health and disease prevention. She concludes with an optimistic outlook for future research in humans and the potential of klotho for the prevention and treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Disclosure: Peter is an investor in Jocasta Neuroscience, a company working to develop klotho as a therapy for people with Alzheimer’s disease. We discuss: Dena’s fascination with aging and how she came to study klotho [3:30]; Biological properties of klotho: production, regulation, decline with age, and factors influencing its levels [11:45]; Potential benefits of klotho on brain health [22:00]; The relationship between soluble klotho protein, platelet factors, and cognitive enhancement [33:45]; The role of platelet factor 4 (PF4) and it’s interaction with GluN2B in mediating cognitive enhancement [46:45]; Benefits of klotho observed in a mouse model of Parkinson’s disease [55:45]; Benefits of klotho observed in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease [1:03:00]; Promising results of klotho in primate models, and the importance of finding an appropriate therapeutic dose before moving to human trials [1:08:00]; Speculating why a single klotho injection has such long-lasting effects [1:25:30]; Potential cognitive benefits of klotho in humans, the impact of the KL-VS genetic variant on klotho levels, and the need for human trials to confirm these effects [1:27:45]; The interaction between the KL-VS genetic variant and APOE4 and how it impacts risk of Alzheimer’s disease [1:34:45]; The significance of klotho levels: studies linking lower levels to increased mortality and the broader implications for organ health and disease prevention [1:47:15]; Measuring klotho levels and determining an individual’s KL-VS status [1:52:15]; The promising potential of klotho for Alzheimer’s disease treatment, and the importance of philanthropy for funding research [1:58:00]; and More. Connect With Peter on Twitter, Instagram, Facebook and YouTube
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hey everyone, welcome to the Drive Podcast. I'm your host, Peter Attia. This podcast,
my website, and my weekly newsletter all focus on the goal of translating the science of
longevity into something accessible for everyone. Our goal is to provide the best content in health and wellness, and we've established
a great team of analysts to make this happen.
It is extremely important to me to provide all of this content without relying on paid
ads.
To do this, our work is made entirely possible by our members, and in return, we offer exclusive
member-only content and benefits above and beyond what is available for free.
If you want to take your knowledge of this space to the next level,
it's our goal to ensure members get back much more than the price of a subscription.
If you want to learn more about the benefits of our premium membership,
head over to PeterAtiyaMD.com forward slash subscribe.
My guest this week is Dr. Dina Dubal.
Dina is a physician scientist, professor of neurology at the University of California,
San Francisco and holds the David Coulter Endowed Chair in Aging and Neurodegenerative
Disease.
She is also an investigator with the Simmons Foundation and the Baccar Aging Research Institute.
Her work is recognized for its
significant potential towards therapies to help people live longer and better. She directs
a laboratory focused on mechanisms of longevity and brain resilience that integrate genetic
and molecular approaches to investigate aging, Alzheimer's disease, and Parkinson's disease.
In my conversation with Dina, we focus around something called Clotho.
Now, if you've heard me talk on this podcast
and other podcasts,
you've probably heard me bring up Clotho,
either the protein or the gene that codes for the protein.
My interest in Clotho really started a couple of years ago
when I became aware of some of the genetic data in humans
about the relationship between clotho
and Alzheimer's disease prevention, particularly in people who are carriers of the A4 gene.
This of course has led me deeper and deeper down the clotho rabbit hole and really all
roads lead to Dina if you want to have this discussion.
So we begin our discussion with an overview of clotho.
What is it?
How is it formed?
How does it get around
our body and what does it do? We talk about the mechanisms regulating clotho
in the body and in particular in the brain. We also talk about things that
impact clotho levels such as stress and exercise and to what extent they do.
From there we look at the research that's being done in how clotho relates
to various cognitive functions as well as its role in brain health across different species and across different ages, as well
as understanding how clotho treatment may be helpful in treating neurodegenerative disease,
particularly Alzheimer's disease.
We wrap up this discussion speaking about the broader impacts of clotho on organ health
in addition to what its potential may hold for the treatment
of Alzheimer's disease in the future.
Before getting to this podcast, I'd like to mention a conflict of interest, which is that
I am an investor in a company called Jocasta.
Jocasta is a company that is trying to bring Clotho, the protein, into human clinical trials
for treatment of Alzheimer's disease.
So, without further delay, please enjoy my conversation with Dina Dubaul.
Dina, thanks so much for joining me today. This is a topic I've been very interested in for the
better part of about a year and
a half to two years.
And obviously there's no better person to discuss this with than you.
I also suspect that this is a topic not enough people know about given its potential implications
and significance vis-a-vis Alzheimer's disease, which we'll get to.
But maybe before we get into this, let's give people a bit of a sense of your background.
Tell me a little bit about your clinical work,
your research work, what you did during your PhD
and how that carried forward during your tenure.
Sure, well, thanks again for the invitation.
I'm delighted to join you today.
By way of introduction, I'm a neurologist
and I'm a neuroscientist and I direct a group
that is deeply involved
in the discovery around clotho.
I was thinking back to when my interest in aging actually began, what was this journey?
I thought back to my undergraduate days at UC Berkeley when I was a 19-year-old that
was oddly interested in aging, kind of obsessed
with aging. I worked with the medical anthropologist at Berkeley, Lawrence Cohen, on what it meant
to experience dementia in different cultures. What was it like in India versus the United
States? And then simultaneously, I took a class on the physiology of aging.
I remember it so clearly in Dwanell Hall and I was at the edge of my seat learning about
cellular senescence.
And I remember thinking, this is amazing.
This is happening to us day by day aging, yet we don't know so much about it.
And we don't know much about brain aging per se.
I was committed as an undergraduate to really learn more about brain aging and possibly
to do something about it.
That led me to an MD PhD at the University of Kentucky.
I trained with Willis Wise, a neuroendocrinologist who studied brain aging, who has an amazing
PhD, learned so much, fell in love with the discovery process of science.
And fast forward, I then trained as a neurologist at UCSF where I am now.
And that was over 20 years ago, 2003.
And I'm on the wards.
It's a regular day of patient rounding.
And the former chair of UCSF neurology, Steven Hauser,
turned to me and he said,
Dina, when you go back to the lab,
do things that are big and important
and not incremental or mediocre,
because they'll take you the same amount of time.
And that really clicked with me.
It became my mantra.
And after an Alzheimer's fellowship, both clinical and basic science, I had the chance
to build a group and to really start my own scientific discovery.
And we focused on clotho, the subject of what we'll talk about today, the Greek fate who
spins the thread of life.
And the idea of studying clotho was to understand whether factors that help us to live longer could
help us to live better. Whether this longevity factor could actually help the brain, could help
it stave off Alzheimer's disease. And that's where we began.
So let's go back to the first time Clotho crossed your radar.
Yes. Well, I was just beginning my junior faculty days and was thinking what were we going to focus
on? What was going to be our chance to do something big and important? I was very intrigued by a few decades of work that was emerging
that aging itself was malleable. And the first work with this was Cynthia Kenyon in worms,
where she demonstrated that tweaking genetics, aging in worms could dramatically increase
lifespan. And I really wanted to know whether that could have effects in the brain.
Clotho had emerged as a longevity factor and it was a chance to understand could clotho
do something in the brain.
Nothing was known about clotho.
Very little was known about clotho when we started.
A colleague had observed that the levels decreased in the white matter of monkey brains, that a variant of clotho that we
can talk about in a bit was associated with decreased stroke risk. And the person who
discovered clotho had noted that mice without clotho move slowly and they were cognitively
a little slow. But I was the right person at the right time and had the chance to really dig in. It was risky
to start with something that not much was known about in the brain, but it was a chance to do
something maybe big and important. Let's talk a little bit about the discovery of
clotho. Was it discovered through the mice that were deficient? Was it deliberately knocked out?
What was the process of discovery? Just? Was it deliberately knocked out?
What was the process of discovery? I mean, just contrasting it with Cynthia's work, obviously she knocked out a gene that
is, I think, sort of the analog of one of the IGF genes, if I'm not mistaken, correct?
Yeah, the DAF-16.
Yeah.
So how was clotho discovered?
It was in 1997, Makoto Kuro-O, a Japanese scientist,
accidentally discovered it.
And it's such a story of serendipity.
He was studying hypertension, and he engineered a mouse
to insert a gene, which I believe
was a sodium proton channel.
So he engineered mice, he inserted this gene, and then he noticed that in a few
lines of these mice, maybe it was just one line of these mice, there was this premature
aging phenotype. So the mice lived to about three months instead of about 30 months, and
they developed normally, but around two weeks of age, they became progeroid. They
looked like they were rapidly aging with osteoporosis, atherosclerosis, emphysema. They looked and
they moved slow. They looked really old. And so he went back to that mouse and he mapped out what had happened and he had accidentally caused
a mutation by inserting his sodium proton pump. So he went back, he mapped what had
been disrupted and that was clotho. That was the first time clotho was found and he named
it after the Greek fate who spins the thread of life, daughter of Zeus. Clotho, the Greek fate, is
C-L-O-T-H-O. He named it K-L-O-T-H-O in homage to his discovery. His name is Kuro-O, K-U-R-O.
And a longevity factor isn't something that just causes premature aging if disrupted. So,
it was very important for him to then go back and see
what would happen if he overexpressed it. Then he engineered mice to overexpress clotho and those
mice lived 30% longer. So that was a longevity factor. That disruption caused premature aging
and overexpression extended lifespan. And it's such a beautiful story. It's a great story for people, especially maybe who don't do or
haven't done science because it illustrates the role of curiosity and serendipity. Look,
there are some people who maybe wouldn't have even gone back and done the experiments that
he did to understand what turned out to be the most relevant thing. I'm sure that whatever he was doing
with a sodium proton pump exchanger
seemed interesting at the time,
and that what he observed was actually
kind of a disappointment in that it ruined
one of his lines of mice,
but obviously it led to the far more relevant finding
over the long arc of time.
He opened the field, he followed the science,
he opened the field, he looked into the mistake,
the oops of science, and here we are,
maybe on the cusp of a new therapy.
Yeah, exactly.
So let's talk a little bit about what this gene does.
How big is this gene?
How preserved is this gene over other species?
And tell us a little bit about the protein
that it codes for, what regulates it?
So clotho itself is a pretty big protein. It's about a thousand amino acids by weight,
maybe 130 kilodaltons if you look at it on a Western blot. And it codes for the aficionados,
a type one transmembrane protein, which means that its N-terminus will sit in the extracellular space.
It has one pass through the membrane and it sees terminus as inside the cell. Now,
it's N-term, that part that sits outside the cell has two repeat domains, KL1 and KL2.
And those domains have high homology to proteins that are found throughout mammalian
worm and fly biology. So there are homologs of clotho throughout species and pretty conserved
in mammals. So clotho is a big protein. It's a transmembrane protein. And then what happens
is there are a few forms of clotho. It's made primarily in the kidney, also in the choroid plexus of the brain, but it's
made in the cells of the kidney.
And then it gets transported into the membrane and then enzymes come across, base, atom 10,
atom 17, and they clip that extracellular portion of clotho and release it into the blood.
Or in the case of the brain, release it into the CSF.
And that form of clotho is known as a soluble form of clotho
or the secreted form of clotho.
I call it a hormonal form of clotho, the clotho hormone,
because it's released at one site
and then can act at different sites at multiple organs.
So that's how it's made. Those are the major forms.
So how big is that end terminus piece that gets clipped, that now goes and acts like a hormone
basically? It's the majority of the protein itself. The part that's inside the cell and
crosses the membrane is very small. So it's really the majority of the protein is the hormone itself.
If we were to take a blood test and measure a person's clotho levels, what could we observe
about it across people of the same age, people of different ages?
What factors do we think might influence the expression?
And does the expression and
production of the protein become the sole determinant of the soluble factor? In other
words, is there also variability at the cleavage efficacy or are there other factors that lead
to consumption of the protein or degradation of the protein in the periphery?
So starting with levels of clotho across the lifespan.
Well, first clotho, it circulates in us,
it circulates in mice.
And when we started our studies in mice,
I really wanted to make sure that this was gonna be relevant
to the human condition.
And it is very relevant to the human condition.
So we have circulating levels of clotho.
We're born with about six times the levels that we have now in our cord blood.
And then throughout our lifespan, clotho levels decline.
In our, from say 40 onwards, they can decrease by half during aging.
So, we're born with very high levels and they decrease across our lifespan.
They also have a circadian rhythm or a diurnal rhythm in that when we wake up, we have high
levels of clotho.
And by 4 p.m., 3 p.m., when people are looking to grab a cup of coffee, the clotho levels
are really starting to decline and then they nader by around midnight.
It can be a 40% or so decrease.
So there's a circadian rhythm to clotho.
It changes across the lifespan.
Sorry, Dina, just to make sure I understand that.
What explains the daily variation?
Is it some sort of quote unquote consumption?
Or is it being turned over that quickly and the supply of clotho
is decreasing by the time of day.
The answer to that is not known. What regulates the daily levels? Is it, how is it being degraded?
Is the expression being increased? The half-life is short in mice. In mice,
it's maybe seven to 10 minutes. But in humans, well, I would say in monkeys, it's much longer. It's at least a day or so.
So it's unlikely that the production is really actually increasing and decreasing because when
it's made, it's half-life will be longer in non-human primates like us. So I would speculate,
though I don't know Peter, that it may be sequestered in organs or degraded or not sure.
I've seen a couple of reports of this diurnal variation and it's really not clear why it's
increasing and decreasing, but it is known in aging, for example, this longer term decline
and slow decline that we see. It's been observed, and this was work by Fabrizio
and Brosio's lab, that there is a hypermethylation around the clothopromoter that occurs with aging
that stops its transcription and essentially transcription and translation.
So that's an interesting thing to think about if we wanted to preserve our clotho
levels, how can we interfere with that methylation that happens with aging?
That's interesting. So more methylation there leads to inhibition of the promoter. A lot of
times you'll see the opposite. It's the loss of methylation that shuts off the promoter, correct?
I don't know about that. In general, or clotho at least, the methylation that shuts off the promoter, correct? I don't know about that.
In general, or clotho at least, the methylation
really turns things off.
Off, OK.
I could be wrong about that, but I think that that's definitely
the case with clotho.
And is that believed to be the main mechanism driving
the reduced production of clotho is
the increase in the methylation of the promoter?
Dr. Julie Kinn That's what's known so far. There are other possibilities like maybe organs make it
less, maybe the kidney is less efficient at making it with aging. That's possible. I think that
Ambrosio's lab is really onto something. They showed it in chondrocytes and aging chondrocytes,
but this really the first
molecular demonstration about why clotho may be decreasing. Now there are other things,
Peter, that decrease clotho. One big one is chronic stress. We did a study with
Alyssa Epple and Eric Prather here at UCSF and mothers with neurodevelopmentally
at UCSF and mothers with neurodevelopmentally typical or atypical children and the mothers with high levels of stress with children with autism spectrum disorder had much lower levels
of clotho as well as shorter telomeres. Their level of stress, increasing stress, there was
a decrease in the clotho level.
Did you demonstrate in those folks a higher degree of methylation of the promoter relative
to their age and did it basically look like they were older at the level of the promoter?
I think that would be so neat to look at.
We haven't done that, but it would be really neat to look at.
So in other words, we don't yet know the mechanism by which chronic stress could be mediating the
reduction.
Ryan Brown here at UCSF and Eric Prather and also Alyssa Epple and myself looked
at whether there might be a relationship between clotho levels and telomere
length in these same women.
And there is a very direct relationship with a very tight correlation between shorter
telomeres and lower clotho levels.
So there may be some convergence around these different hallmarks of aging, maybe some relationship
of them regulating each other, or maybe they're changing in parallel, it's hard to say.
But here's some really good news, that in talking and thinking about
what changes clotho levels,
we do know that stress associates
with lower clotho levels.
But one of the most robust interventions
that increases clotho levels is exercise.
It's been shown in study after study and in meta-analysis,
it's been done in about 12 studies,
but the data are that after 12 weeks
of what people call chronic exercise,
that clotho levels increased by about 30%.
And maybe even increased in a study with preliminary data,
in mice, they can even double acutely
after a 45-minute treadmill run.
And how it's increased is unknown,
but it's beginning to be thought of as an exorkine, something
that is produced and released in the body following exercise.
How would this stack up in your mind
compared to something else that we would think of
along these lines, BDNF, where I've talked about this at length
on the podcast.
Of course, if you really look objectively at all of the modifiable behaviors around
dementia, routinely exercise is at the absolute top of the list.
More potent even in its magnitude than lipid management, glucose management, and hypertension,
all of which themselves are enormously powerful.
But something about exercise seems even greater. It could be that it indirectly mediates at
least two of those three. But of course, people point to something else that's going on and
BDNF is often discussed. Do you think that maybe part of that effect is clotho? And do
you think that the effect of clotho may be even more potent?
I was recently asked the same question
at an Alzheimer's conference at the Salk Institute,
and I don't know the answer,
but I think it's a really important question.
Is there a relationship between clotho and BDNF?
Does clotho increase BDNF levels in the brain?
I don't know.
BDNF has a very striking effect in the brain. Maybe, Dina, just tell folks a bit about BDNF levels in the brain, I don't know. BDNF has a very striking effect in the brain.
Maybe, Dina, just tell folks a bit about BDNF.
Sometimes I forget that maybe not everybody
listening to us today has also listened
to all of the other content over the years.
So maybe just give folks a little bit
of a primer on BDNF as well.
So BDNF is brain-derived neurotrophic factor,
and it has been shown in the brain to really
be associated with even drive positive brain health.
So intermittent fasting increases BDNF in the brain, exercise increases BDNF.
If you give BDNF to the brain, the neurons function better.
It's a really good trophic factor for the brain. And it does remind one of clotho
and effects of clotho. And in fact, intermittent fasting simultaneously increases clotho
in the brains of mice, along with BDNF. How long do the mice need to fast to get that effect?
So this work was done at the SOC. The mice fasted for 24 hours.
So not really something we can translate to humans given that that's – I mean 24 hours in a mouse is
that's half the distance to death. So that's –
That's interesting.
But this is a real problem. This is a real problem with extrapolating from mice data, right? Is even a 12 hour fast in a mouse is an overwhelming feat of calorie deprivation.
24 hours is staggering. We don't have a way to translate that to humans,
but it could be that that's on the order of weeks. So I just want to make sure people aren't listening
to this thinking, hey, all I got to do is skip breakfast and I'm getting the same amount of
BDNF
or cloth though that I would get from a 45 minute workout.
My guess is those two are not even in the same zip code.
I think differently about it.
So when I think about mouse time and mouse lifespan,
so they live to two to three years, but I believe that they're aging faster.
They have a shorter lifespan,
but I believe that they're aging faster. They have a shorter lifespan, but I think it's possible that their 24-hour lives are similar to our 24-hour lives.
But 48 hours of fasting will usually kill a mouse, right?
I don't know how long a mouse can go.
I used to be more facile with this literature, but I pay less and less attention to mouse
literature now than I used to. Maybe someone listening to this
could write in and correct me, but I don't think it's a stretch to say that 48 hours, put it this
way, what I do remember is an IRB would really scrutinize an investigator who's trying to get
to 36 hours of fasting in a mouse. It's a really big deal.
We don't do fasting studies in mice, but I would agree that in general,
we have to be careful when extrapolating from mice to humans. I do think that in the area of
cognition and neurodegenerative diseases, that studying mice and mouse brains and their neural
circuits and their hippocampi and their prefrontal cortices tell us fundamental
things about the human condition, particularly because they have to have very strong navigation
strategies and memories for foraging, for coming back to their nest.
And these memory circuits are very, very fundamental and quite preserved up to non-human primates, including us.
But having said that, we do the majority of our work in mice.
We are always aware of, are we doing something
that is important to the human condition?
Is it relatable to the human condition?
Is it relevant?
Who cares?
Will any of this matter?
These are always on my mind, these questions, particularly
as a physician scientist. And there are certainly limitations. I also think that there are many
strengths, particularly in studying memory and neural circuits in the brain. But we don't
stop there. And we have tested whether I haven't talked yet about what we have found in mice, but I'll
just spill the beans and say that we found early on that clotho enhances cognition in
mouse brains.
In a very recent study, we collaborated with biotech and Yale University and found that
actually clotho enhances learning and memory and cognition in old monkeys.
In a very complex brain, genetically diverse, anatomically diverse, functionally complex,
that in a brain like ours that clotho had very similar effects to what we see in mice.
There's so much I want to come back to on that topic because I've seen all of these data of course and truthfully,
they seem overwhelmingly positive. Let's go back and use some of the mice data to allow us to speak
about maybe greater insights around the causal relationship between clotho and brain health.
So we've already established a few facts here. So you're going back to the discovery of clotho and brain health. We've already established a few facts here. You're going back to the
discovery of clotho in 97, which was based on effectively a knockout. We saw that that is
incompatible with a long life. We saw that the reverse when clotho was enhanced, I believe you
said it enhanced lifespan by about 30%. Again, that's really remarkable in a mouse. There aren't many things that enhance a mouse lifespan by that much.
That's up there with the most draconian caloric restriction, rapamycin administration under
certain settings.
That's a very reproducible and robust finding.
Let's talk a little bit about specifics to brain health, what you're talking about with respect to not a disease state per se,
but just enhancing cognition in an aging mouse or even in a middle-aged mouse.
This is where we started. And we started with the hypothesis that in these mice that over express
clotho that live longer, that maybe these mice would be resilient to
Alzheimer's toxins. That was our starting point. And so we back-crossed Alzheimer's
models with clotho overexpressing models to create mice who have mouse-heimers and then
have mouse-heimers plus lots of clotho.
And then we had normal mice and normal mice
with lots of clotho.
So there were four experimental groups.
And to our complete surprise, the normal groups,
we're not talking about the mice that had Alzheimer's,
modeled Alzheimer's, but in the normal groups
that had higher levels of clotho versus normal
levels of that group showed a remarkable difference, a remarkable statistically significant difference
across multiple cognitive tasks and across different ages.
And that was that the mice that overexpressed clotho that live longer were smarter.
So you put them in a maze.
They could map the room better, escape quicker,
not because they were swimming faster,
not because they could see better,
but because they were remembering and learning
more efficiently and better.
And that was a complete surprise.
I remember the day unblinding the data, analyzing it.
It was unimaginable.
Dina, I just want to add a point. Sorry to interrupt, but it's so powerful. You a moment
ago said this was statistically significant, which of course it was. And too often, I think
people hear the term statistically significant. And I think they can be forgiven for confusing
it with clinically significant. But as you know, those are not
necessarily the same thing. And I almost think that in this case, Dina, stating that it is
statistically significant, which it is, almost undersells what I think the magnitude of the
differences are. I have a friend who likes to point out that the really powerful stuff in biology is the
stuff that you don't actually need to do the statistics on.
When the delta is so big that you don't need to whip out the student t-test and calculate
the p-value to make sure you're not being fooled, which by the way is very important
and you should be doing it every time, when it's actually just a formality because the answer is so clear looking at the data, that's the really
big aha moment.
And I think I'd be safe in saying that that was the case here, wasn't it?
You're right.
We tend to be really conservative.
We don't want to make an error.
But having said that, you're totally right.
I mean, I can imagine the graphs now, and there were non-overlapping data points.
I didn't need to run the repeated measures between subjects and NOVA to really know that
it was statistically significant.
It was that type of a finding.
So it's a point well made. Sometimes experiments will show you those
differences and then the next time you do it, they don't. Someone who runs the water
maze is wearing a new cologne and the mice get stressed out. They don't always repeat
and are not always replicable. This is a case that it was amazing, but was it going to replicate? Was it going to be true in
different cognitive tasks? What would happen in aging mouse? All of these questions opened up,
and we iterated and we did experiment after experiment, and the data held strong in mice.
That clotho overexpression really enhanced their cognition in young mice, in the aging mice,
in mice that modeled Alzheimer's disease, and also in mice in a publication that's in peer review
now, but also in mice that model Parkinson's disease, that overexpression of clotho enhanced
cognition. It is really a remarkable finding.
I think it's maybe one of the most important findings in my professional career.
I would go one step further.
I feel like it's one of the most important findings in brain health period. I mean,
on some levels,
it's a bit of a mystery to me why this isn't known by everyone.
It's part of why I'm really, really happy to be sitting down with you and why I've wanted
to sit down with you for the better part of six months.
I know that between my schedule and yours, it took a minute to get us on the schedule.
This is such a big deal.
I also want to reiterate something you said a moment ago, and I say this and we'll be
linking to all of these studies so people can see the data for themselves.
But the reproducibility coupled with the magnitude of the effect, so the consistent, enormous
magnitude of difference across studies, across labs, across investigators, that signals something
is going on here.
Again, not to bring up rapamycin again, which people hear me talk about a lot, but people say why are you so confident in Rappamycin's
Giroprotective effects? The magnitude of benefit is enormous and it doesn't seem
to matter who does the study. If it's done in this part of the country or this
part of the world or with this animal or with that animal, it always seems to
work. Now that doesn't guarantee it's going to work in humans.
I'd be the first to admit that, but boy,
I feel a lot better when it works in every model system,
virtually every time. And when it passes the eyeball test,
you don't need the error bars and the P values.
So let's talk a little bit about before we go to primates and
ultimately to the epidemiology
in humans, let's talk about mechanism of action.
Tell me what you think is happening with this soluble clothoprotein.
I assume it's crossing the blood-brain barrier or it wouldn't be having this effect.
Has that been easily demonstrated? It is a winding story, Peter.
And one that's exciting and has led us to very unanticipated findings.
I first wanted to mention, as we think about how different interventions may converge upon
the same biology or relate to the same biology, it is interesting that mice treated with rapamycin
have increased clotho levels. So rapamycin either directly or indirectly will increase clotho levels,
just thinking about the convergence of biology. Now, how does clotho enhance cognition in a young,
aging, and diseased brain? And that immediately became a major question for my lab and many
other labs. And the very first thing that we had turned to was a component of NMDA receptors.
So NMDA receptors are really key in connections between neurons. And when they're stimulated, they let in calcium into a neuron
and they allow essentially a potentiation of a neuron for connections to form, for functional
connections to form, and really for the neural substrate of memory to form. So NMDA receptors
are really key. And there's a component of NMDA receptors called gl Gluen2B. It's a subunit. I'm really
going tiny and molecular here, but it's very important because I had remembered that there
was something called a Doogie Howser mouse, affectionately called a Doogie Howser mouse.
We're kind of dating ourselves here because there's going to be a lot of listeners who
don't know who Doogie Howser is, but suffice it to say this was a prodigious mouse. Yes. It was a mouse that was smarter. There are very few model systems where you can actually
pivot the system to make an engineer a mouse to be smarter. There are very, very few things.
Clotho is one of them. This Gluen2B overexpressing mouse or the Doogie Howser mouse showed a very
similar phenotype to the Clotho overexpressing
mouse.
When I looked at the behavioral data of the Gluen2b overexpressing mouse, in the very
same test, it was performing very well just like the Clotho overexpressing mice.
I just wondered whether Clotho was acting throughuN2B to enhance cognition.
That was the hypothesis.
In this case, the hypothesis,
we gathered data that supported the hypothesis.
There wasn't a big surprise in that over-expressing clotho caused
an increase in gluN2B at the synapse,
at the area where brain cells connect,
and led to a more efficient
connection and memory formation synaptic plasticity at the level of the neural cell.
So this gluen to be was really important when we blocked gluen to be with multiple pharmacologic
inhibitors, we abrogated clotho's ability to enhance cognition. So that was
one clue, but here's the major head scratcher. So Peter, clotho is expressed in the kidney.
It's also expressed in the brain secreted through the choroid plexus, which also makes
the fluid for the brain that the brain sits in. But clotho does not cross the blood brain barrier.
We have looked, others have looked through auto radiography, through IP and Western blot,
we've looked through many immunohistochemistry, we've looked through many different methods
and can see very clearly that it doesn't cross into the brain.
However, when we give a shot of clotho to mice, to monkeys,
in their arm or their belly, doesn't matter,
if we give them a shot of clotho, within four hours,
there is cognitive enhancement.
And that cognitive enhancement lasts for at least a couple
of weeks.
So how is it that giving peripheral clotho
is enhancing brain functions if it's not actually crossing into
the brain.
That's been a major question that we and others have been working on.
And we have some clues that led us in unexpected places.
I'm happy to talk about one of them if this is a good-
I am all ears.
To me, this is, I mean, I didn't want to get to this point yet, but this is
the natural time to talk about this. And then I think we should step back and talk about
some other things. But yes, to me, this is the jugular question, Dina. Why is it that
when we inject a monkey peripherally with clotho, it spends the next 14 to 19 days in a state of cognitive superiority. And that
clotho isn't crossing the blood-brain barrier. This is, to me, the jugular question.
You're right. And we have been working on this. I think I'll step back and say, it's
actually amazing that you give something peripherally.
It has a central action.
It's not crossing into the brain.
And it's something physiologic that our bodies are used to, that we have seen high levels
of upon birth and development.
It does not have known side effects at physiologic levels that have been observed yet.
It's very remarkable.
So what is it doing? We hypothesize. So I'll just say we don't know yet. This is an area
of fervent investigation for us and others, but we have one clue in a study that we published
recently in nature aging. And this is what we did. We started with the premise that maybe Clotho was doing something
in the blood that then would send a messenger into the brain. So maybe there was this messenger of
Clotho that was actually going into the brain when you gave it peripherally. And so we did a very
simple experiment. We injected mice with Clotho. And four hours later, at the time of cognitive
enhancement, we did an agnostic profiling of the proteins in their plasma, in their
blood. And what we found was sort of unimaginable. It was unimaginable in that we found that
all of these platelet factors were increased with clotho
injection in the blood.
And Peter, I have to be honest, when I saw this agnostic data set, I turned to my postdoc
and I said, I'm really not interested in this.
I am not interested in looking at platelets.
They are involved in wound healing, their coagulation factors.
This is weird.
I had a post-doc, Kena Park, who is very persistent
and said, let me try, let me just study this
for a little bit.
I said, go for it, I could see your passion.
One question, Dina, just to ask, in parallel,
did you do the same multi-omic observation of the CSF in the mice in parallel
to see if there was something new that was showing up in the CSF to parallel the platelet
factors in the plasma?
Yes, we did.
We haven't published this yet, but we did.
In the same mice, we took their blood and we took their CSF. And what we found is
that Clotho increased these platelet factors. It was totally bizarre. I didn't believe it.
I tend not to believe data. I need to see things over and over again, need to see functional
studies. I need to see it matters. I tend not to believe data, but there it was. She
repeated the findings in many different ways. but ultimately what she did was a series of studies demonstrating that this biology is real,
that clotho is actually activating platelets very modestly. And then when a platelet is activated,
so let's first just step back. What is a platelet? A platelet is an a nuclear cell in our blood and it contains little compartments of bioactive
chemicals, chemokines.
And when a platelet is stimulated or activated, it actually releases these factors and it
does so in a context dependent way. So when there is a cut or a wound, there is a huge activation of platelets and they release
all sorts of factors that help with the clotting and help with the wound healing.
So that's what they're traditionally known for.
But it turns out Peter, that when we exercise, our platelets are activated and they're releasing factors
that travel into the brain. I mean, who would have thought that platelets could be messengers
of brain health and could take center stage as a messenger of brain health? So when we
exercise, this was found by an Australian group led by Tara Walker, that when we exercise platelets are activated and certain platelet
factors, one of them being platelet factor four, is released and travels to the brain
and actually causes neurogenesis or the production of new neurons in the brain. That was very
new data. And with that data in mind, I felt too, like maybe there's something to this Clotho platelet
connection.
Maybe there's something to it.
So Kana isolated platelets.
She put Clotho on them.
They released PF4, this platelet factor 4,
the same one that the Australian group had shown.
And then she gave this platelet factor four to mice, young,
aging mice. She gave it to them as a shot in the belly, just like we have given clotho. And she
found that that platelet factor four enhanced cognition in a young mouse and in an aging mouse,
and it reversed cognitive deficits,
and it was totally remarkable.
This is where it gets weirder and wilder.
So I go to my colleague and close friend,
Saul Valeta here at UCSF.
He's the young blood and old brain scientist.
So he's really built a really nice body of experiments
and literature showing that if you give young blood
to an old mouse, it rejuvenates their brain.
So Saul is studying brain rejuvenation through blood and I'm having coffee with him.
I said, Saul, we've found something so interesting.
I have to tell you about it.
And I go on to tell him about PF4.
Clotho stimulates platelets.
Platelets release PF4.
PF4 enhances the brain. And he said,
well, he was silent for a second. And he said, Dina, we found the same thing with young blood
and that when we give young blood to old mice, that young blood is enriched for platelet factor
four, which declines in aging. And then we give platelet factor four, we rejuvenate the old brain.
And if you think that's a remarkable convergence of biology, wait till I tell you that Tara Walker
in Australia had found the same thing with exercise. And this is all at the same time,
that exercise was increasing PF4 and that when she gave PF4 to her old mice,
it enhanced their cognition.
All of us who are close friends and colleagues now,
have an incredible convergence of biology where clotho,
young blood, and exercise,
we're activating platelets, releasing PF4.
PF4 is enhancing the brain.
And as an aside, the biology is just amazing.
Each of us had our own unique approaches
and unique way of digging into the biology.
But a practical question, Peter, was like,
what are we gonna do?
Are we competitors now?
Or are we going to hold hands and go through the publication process together? Are you going
to screw me? Am I going to screw you? How is this going to come out? And what we did
is I think a really nice example and model in biology where we held hands and we said,
let's just stick together. We went to editors, we went through a couple of years
of revisions and reviews, but at the end of the day, all three papers came out in the Nature
family of journals on the same day. It made for an impactful splash and it also just highlighted
the convergence of the biology, the reproducibility of the biology. Again, this will never happen
again in my lifetime, but this was amazing and a complete surprise that platelet factors
could play a role here.
So, let's talk a little bit more about this. So, platelet factor four is increased by all
of these three things. Do we believe that it's platelet factor four that is directly acting on gluN2B,
then? If so, what do you think is happening at the level of substrate and receptor?
Dr. Lacey Krofman That's where we went next with the biology and the experiments.
One question was, does platelet factor four actually cross into the brain and we demonstrated that it does. We
gave it peripherally. We immediately looked into the brain and saw that it was ending
up near neural cells or even within neural cells. So it's crossing into the brain. And
then does it act directly in the brain was the next question. And what one of my lab members did
is take hippocampal slices.
So the hippocampus is the area of the brain
that is really executing the learning and memory
and is targeted by aging and diseases of aging.
So he took hippocampus
and then he put platelet factor four on it
and saw immediately within seconds, actually, there was a,
well, I don't know within seconds, but quite immediately,
maybe seconds, maybe minutes, there
was a change in the membrane potential.
And more calcium was being let in.
And we thought maybe this would be gluon2b acting.
And so we put on gluon2B inhibitors and platelet factor four no longer potentiated the synapse
or enhanced the neural function.
So we do know that platelet factor four, like Clotho, is working through gluN2B.
And maybe Clotho is working through platelet factor 4 to change gluen2b.
Sorry, has that same experiment been done, Dina, to look at the clotho that is derived
from the chloride plexus to say when you dump clotho directly onto gluen2b, you see an influx
of calcium.
When you block gluen2b, you abrogate the effect as well. You now have two
things, platelet factor four and Clotho that can independently do the same thing. Is that a correct
statement? We haven't done that experiment yet. We've done transgenic overexpression in the brain
and we've done Clotho peripherally. We haven't't dumped clotho itself onto the hippocampus
to see whether there can be a direct effect.
Other groups have overexpressed clotho
specifically in the brain and seen cognitive enhancement,
but I haven't seen studies seeing whether clotho
is actually directly influencing GLU-N2b. We know that
it indirectly influences gluN2b. We know that we can give it peripherally and it'll increase
synaptic plasticity through gluN2b, but we don't know whether it can actually directly
do that too. It may, based on other people's studies, it may. We don't know. But here's
something else that was actually quite disappointing, but the biology is the biology. We then asked the question, does clotho rely
on PF4 to enhance cognition? Is PF4 required for clotho-mediated cognitive enhancement?
And to do that, we generated a colony of mice with PF4 knockout.
They just don't have PF4.
And then we gave them Clotho versus vehicle, always blinded, always randomized through
a series of cognitive tests, water maze, large Y-maze, one maze after another, because you'd
like to see the same effect reproduce. And what we found was that Clotho continued
to enhance cognition even in the absence of platelet factor 4.
To the same extent, Dina?
To the same extent.
You might think there would have been a diminution,
but to the same extent.
It just tells us that there's more than one factor.
It has to be such an important pathway, and it is so conserved that there is no way biology
is going to let one thing be the messenger.
I'm making all this up, of course, but hypothesizing that if clotho is so important, it can't be
limited to one messenger.
If it gets to the gate and it can't get through, it has to have multiple messengers that it
can deliver the message to PF4 being one of them.
But clearly this experiment would suggest that there's at least one other messenger,
right?
Right.
That's the interpretation and conclusion because PF4 is sufficient to recapitulate
also mediated cognitive enhancement, but not necessary.
When you go back and look at the first experiment you did, the unbiased, was that just a proteomic
assessment or did you look at all omics?
How broadly did you sample the serum way back and was there something else you missed because
the PF4 was such a big signal?
Was there a smaller signal that was there perhaps as
well? There were many signals. There are many other platelet factors. There are many other proteins.
We are systematically marching through those. We're marching through not just proteins, but
metabolites. We are also asking in a cell type specific way through a system called turbo ID that labels the protein as it's
secreted from the liver, the kidney, the heart, from lymphocytes. It's a really unique and elegant
genetic manipulation that does a biotin label once a protein is secreted, let's say from the liver.
And we're asking when we inject clotho, what gets secreted
out of the liver and into the plasma. So we're doing like higher resolution studies to understand
how clotho really changes the systemic circulation in a cell type specific way. And then asking
which one of those factors that comes from the kidney, the liver, the lymphocytes,
the heart, which one of those factors is necessary and required for cloth-immediated cognitive
enhancement? And the answer at the end of the day may be what you alluded to, that there may be many
factors that have overlapping functions of cognitive enhancement. Maybe they work together,
maybe they work independently. But you're right. If this is a very important biologic
function, it really shouldn't rely on just one factor. I think that's a really important
point to underline.
Soterios Johnson Tell me a little bit more about this assay,
the in vitro assay that you were able to basically sprinkle on PF4, sprinkle on
Clotho to the gluen2b subparticle and witness the massive influx of calcium. It seems to me that if
that assay is reasonable, it could also become a great screening tool to identify rather than
having to do the experiments to see if you can enhance cognition, at least do your first
screen there. Now you run the risk that if Clotho is working through secondary mechanisms,
you'll miss it. But at least as a first screen, in as much as Clotho is working through an MDA
via GLUEN2B, let's screen a whole bunch of molecules on it, look for the activation, and then go back
and search for those in a biased way in our serum sample and sort of triangulate back
and forth like that.
I love it.
Let's do it.
And we are doing it and we're doing it with synaptic plasticity as a measure, as a substrate
of cognition. And we're also doing it in vitro
at the cellular level using live cell imaging, where we can isolate neurons from the brain,
from a mouse brain, and then they grow beautifully on a dish. They connect with each other and
they survive for weeks on end, actually. And you can do live cell imaging to see whether if you put clotho on, I have a graduate student
doing this right now, Barbara Shariva, who is putting clotho onto neurons and these other
factors that we're seeing from the omics onto neurons and seeing whether there is an increase
in the neurite outgrowth and the physical connections between
neurons. Again, this isn't cognition itself, but it's a substrate of cognition. It may be a
distant biomarker for it, but it's a really smart way to, when you're asking a question of
this many proteins, which ones are important. It's a way to screen.
So with synaptic plasticity and with neurite connections,
with the outgrowth of the neurons connections.
So all of this makes a lot of sense
for a mouse model of Alzheimer's disease,
where the primary deficit is a cognitive deficit that
seems disproportionately focused in memory.
But you mentioned before I distracted us down this rabbit hole that you are also seeing
positive signals in a mouse model of Parkinson's disease.
Now are you seeing them in the mild cognitive impairment that can accompany Parkinson's
disease or are you seeing it in the movement disorder?
Are you seeing an improvement in the primary issue associated with Parkinson's disease? Or are you seeing it in the movement disorder? Are you seeing an improvement in the primary issue associated
with Parkinson's disease?
Part of this is published.
Part of this is in the peer review process.
But it's really, really exciting to share.
So these are the experiments.
We took mice that overexpress alpha-synuclein,
which is a pathogenic player in Parkinson's disease.
It disrupts the synapse and has a causative role in Parkinson's disease because we know
that people with mutations in causing overexpression of alpha-synuclein will develop Parkinson's
disease.
It's sort of like the equivalent of APP in people with Alzheimer's disease where you have
this... It's not necessarily the dominant driver of the disease, but it's clearly playing a causal
role based on these mutation studies. That's right. And then the mice that I've mentioned
that we've used for our Alzheimer's or Mousheimer's studies were APP mutant overexpressors, just to pedal back to that.
So we took these Parkinson's model mice and they have both motor deficits, meaning that
they walk across a balance beam and they'll fall.
If they're put on a spinning rod, they're dis-coordinated and they'll fall.
They have motor difficulty. They also have cognitive difficulties,
which are not as severe as the Alzheimer's model mice, but they do have cognitive difficulties
and their ability to map a spatial environment and to hold memory in their mind with working
memory. So they have these deficits. We did two things. We injected them with clotho and remarkably saw that clotho treatment improved
their cognition. It didn't normalize, but it improved, if I'm thinking about the data correctly,
by maybe 70% or so, their cognitive abilities. So nearly normalized their cognitive functions. It didn't do anything
to their motor functions, so they continued to have motor dysfunction and clotho did not help
that. That was also true with the transgenic overexpression of clotho all over the body and
brain for a lifetime that warded the cognitive deficits induced by Parkinson's toxicity,
but it did nothing for the motor problems.
And I've spoken to my Parkinson's colleagues about this.
There's a lot of interest in potentially using Clotho as a treatment for Parkinson's disease
because they tell me in our clinic, Stena, we can treat the tremor and the rigidity.
But people complain over and over again consistently
about the cognitive deficits that we now
know are a part of Parkinson's disease,
not just later in the disease, but even
as part of the disease.
And those deficits specifically are problems with executive
function, which is the ability to focus, to shift attention, to make certain judgments,
to think quickly. And it is mediated by the prefrontal cortex, an area that became important
when we looked at human studies.
The bottom line was that in mice, clotho really enhanced, again, cognition, but didn't do
anything for motor functions.
Is there a mouse model that fits between the Alzheimer's model and the Parkinson's model
that is more akin to a Lewy body dementia model where it has some of that alpha synuclein pathology, but also has a much more significant cognitive component.
There are so many mouse models, Peter.
I think that a good mouse model for Lewy body would be alpha synuclein and maybe more in
the brainstem and the hippocampus and cortex.
I bet there is one, but I'm not clear.
But a reasonable hypothesis would be, again,
that it would probably provide some relief
of the cognitive symptoms, though not necessarily
the movement symptoms.
That's right.
So that's what we're seeing consistently.
One of my friends and colleagues had an interesting
analogy of thinking about clotho as a helmet for neurons, that whatever came crashing the
cells way, whether it was alpha-synuclein, tau, amyloid beta, aging stresses, that the
neuron was protected, it remained resilient against multiple toxicities.
The effect of Clotho is really in cognition itself.
In hippocampal and frontal cortical circuits that these neurons and glia and
other cell types are really protected against multiple toxicities.
If we want to jump into clinical trials,
I think this is the time to really move clotho
toward human clinical trials.
Wouldn't it be amazing if we had a cocktail for Alzheimer's disease in addition to leucanumab
or dinanumab that's removing the amyloid beta from the brain?
What about adding something like clotho that can really help shore up the functions of the cells?
Because we know that Alzheimer's disease is a multi-proteinopathy. It's not just one protein
that's causing the disease. We need factors in our treatment. We need cocktails that can
really resist multiple protein toxicities. So I just have this dream that people might be
able someday to benefit from clotho, this factor that naturally circulates in our body that helps
with longevity, that helps with other organ systems and enhances the brain. As we know from
our monkey studies, for weeks at a time, at least a couple of weeks, if not more,
there was funding for a certain amount of time,
but it has a long acting action.
We need to put helmets around the neurons
to really stave off multiple toxicities.
And this is clotho, this is what clotho does.
This is the new job of the Greek fate
that spins the thread of life.
It's to protect our brains. Yeah. You're preaching to the, not just the choir,
but the fully converted and obviously in my other activities, this is something that a lot
of my energy goes into. I want to come back to a couple other questions on the mouth study that
are going to factor into, I think, when we start to talk about the humans more.
Has the following experiment been done where you take a mouse that is genetically susceptible
to Alzheimer's disease, so the equivalent of your APP mouse or something like that,
and you take a control group, maybe just for my own knowledge, if you took an APP mouse,
at what point in its life, how
many months old before it starts to develop a clinical disease?
So maybe two questions.
At what point would it start to be accumulating amyloid in the CSF?
How many months of age?
And then how many age when it starts to show cognitive impairment?
What are those two numbers roughly?
It depends on the mouse model. And there are so many mouse models of Alzheimer's,
including APP models. The one that we've used consistently is called the J20 model. It expresses
human APP, which produces amyloid beta in mutant forms. And it's a more aggressive model, Peter, and it causes synaptic loss, that connection
between cells, it really disrupts that connection between cells really early, before three months.
And then it starts producing cognitive deficits at around three to four months.
And that's the J10?
The J20 model. J20. Okay.
So here's my question, Dina.
Has the experiment been done where you give those mice clotho starting at birth at a high
dose and does it delay the onset of the inevitability?
We know that through over expression throughout the brain and body, that those mice, those APP mice
will have almost normalized cognition, but we don't know whether it's delaying the onset.
There are others that have done human population clinical studies.
I want to make sure I understand what you just said. You're saying if you take the J20, the APP mutant, and you also cause a mutation that overexpresses clotho,
are you saying that you've done that in the same mouse?
Yes.
Tell me about that mouse's lifespan. Is it normal and is it cognitively normal or does
it still get Alzheimer's disease? That mouse that has the APP mutation
and overexpresses clotho by about three to four fold,
that mouse will live much longer, number one,
so it extends its lifespan.
Number two, it will normalize its cognition.
Across its lifespan?
Across its lifespan.
At three months, seven months, eight months, yes,
it normalizes its cognition.
It's very remarkable.
And then when you look in the brain, Peter,
when you look at levels of amyloid beta and of tau,
they're not different from the mice without high levels of clotho.
That is unbelievable.
In other words, their brains are still riddled with the Alzheimer's toxins,
but they've been able to really thwart the effects of those toxins because they show
normal cognition. And if you look at their synapses,
this work was done in collaboration with Leonard Mucki and Eliezer Mazlia, who's at the NIH now.
When Mazlia looked at the synapses in these mice, he saw that the synapses were all preserved.
So again, like this analogy where Clotho is really providing a helmet around
each neuron, it really allowed the synapses to be preserved, but there was a whole bunch
of amyloid and tau still there. And that is resilience. These toxins are present. They
won't necessarily go away with Clotho, at least in mice. The story may be different in humans.
But clotho provided resilience and thwarted the toxicities of Alzheimer's disease.
Okay.
So, I'm going to plant the seed with you, which we'll come back to.
But the reason I'm asking this question, Dina, is I do believe that at some point we will
have clotho as a drug. I think it's
going to come to market. We're absolutely going to be injecting people with clotho.
Where we'll go is do we believe this is a drug we will only want to give to
people once they have MCI for the listener, right? So early, early, early
stages. Or do we believe that we'll take anybody
who's susceptible and just be giving them clotho even before there is a demonstrated
disease risk? So that's where I want to go with this is kind of thinking three or four
moves ahead on the chessboard.
Now let's go back and talk about the primates because we've talked about this incredible
body of literature in mice.
Again, I think it's reasonable to be circumspect around mice literature.
But again, when you start to talk about the volume of literature here and the breadth
of mouse models and the number of investigators that consistently find the same results and
the magnitude of the results, again, it passes the eyeball test.
You don't need to do ANOVA tables all day long
to get the answer and squeak at it.
This looks incredible, but it would be nice
if we had a more convincing model.
So let's talk about the single best thing
we can ever do before we get to humans,
and that's to look at primates.
So let's talk a little bit about primate brains
and the body of literature around clotho in that brain.
This is an area that I wanted so much to get into.
I don't work with monkeys.
I think it's a very, very specialized field
to test cognition in monkeys.
And so I'll just take us back a long time ago, maybe eight
years ago, when I really wanted to know, Peter, whether clotho can enhance a more complex
brain than a mouse brain. And like you said, mice are really important in scientific discovery,
but there are so many examples of what worked in mice
and cured Alzheimer's disease failed in humans.
And that's the valley of death.
Works in mice doesn't translate to humans.
It's not true for everything.
And mice have been very, very important
for fundamental scientific discovery and for medicines.
But I didn't wanna spend my career on something
that was not going to be relevant to humans. and for medicines. But I didn't want to spend my career on something
that was not going to be relevant to humans.
And that might not be that big and important thing
that Stephen Hauser challenged me on when I was a resident.
I was pitching this one attest cloth and primates
all across Silicon Valley to biotech and pharma.
And people were curious and enthusiastic about it, but one individual really got this, Ned
David, who had started a company, Unity Biotechnology, to attack aging.
And we joined forces.
They fundraised, they found really great collaborators at Yale, and really made this happen to test
clotho and cognitive enhancement in a brain like ours.
When people would tell me, if it doesn't work, it's going to kill your research program,
and I would say, let it die then.
I don't want to spend my career on something that's not important.
If it's done well, we'll know. And so this collaboration between biotech, me and Yale
was one in which a lot of thought, a lot of money went into the design and the execution.
So why non-human primates? Rhesus macaques are incredible organisms. They have very complex brains. They are more genetically
diverse than humans. And that genetic diversity is something that often trumps effects, can often
really influence whether an effect is able to survive genetic diversity or not. So having a
lot of genetic diversity is very important to really challenge and test the biology. Mice are inbred. They all have the same genetics,
just to say. So the rhesus macaques are incredibly genetically diverse, more than humans. They
have a functional complexity that's very similar to humans in terms of the even more of the neural circuits, the hippocampus,
the prefrontal cortex, and they have an anatomic complexity that's pretty similar to us. So by
testing clotho and non-human primates, the idea was to jump over this valley of death. If it worked
in primates, let's look toward humans. If not, let's do something else.
I want to pause there for a second, Dina. I mean, I think obviously the listener can
imagine where this story is going, but that's a huge burn your ships moment.
I want to just spend a minute probing that a little bit more and you were a wildly successful investigator before
this. I don't think it's an obvious step that you would take to push that much further because
let's be clear, if Clotho had failed miserably in primates, how many PhDs and post-docs and
students do you have in your lab? We're a group of around, it fluctuates between seven to 11.
Yeah, and obviously you probably have a very well-oiled machine
at generating R1s and all sorts of grants.
It's not easy to start all over again
if all of that dries up, right?
Well, that's why it was so risky.
And some people just go to a human trial.
You can do biomarkers, but this is risky.
And it is risky if it's not done well, but it's super informative even if it's negative.
If it's good negative data, and not because the experiments were done in a sloppy way,
it's good negative data, then it really tells you something.
And we have other projects on longevity. We
study why women live longer than men. What does the second X chromosome have to do with
it? And there are other longevity factors. So it was really important to push forward
and to push forward in a rigorous way. And also with the knowledge that the results could really change the direction
of what we're doing. But we have times in our lives where we reflect on what we're doing,
how we're spending our time. Is it with people that we appreciate? Is it doing things that
are meaningful? And it may be deeply philosophical, but from day to day, I really want to be doing something
that I have a lot of faith in and that potentially has meaning for the human condition to improve
human health.
And if we've done a really great experiment that's negative, and there's really reason
to believe that this isn't going to go forward, then that's fine.
We will mourn it and we will still move
forward with something else. But we just don't have lifetimes to devote to a hundred different
scientific projects. So we have to be very choosy. And I really, really wanted to make sure that this
is something really worth putting time, energy, resource, passion, constant thought, training, fundraising.
I think the reason I'm pointing this out, Dina, is I want to publicly commend you for
something that honestly I don't think most scientists would be willing to do.
I wouldn't even hazard a guess at the fraction, but I think there are a lot of scientists
who have lost sight of what you just said and they think the job is doing science.
But it's not. The job is knowledge creation. The job is knowledge creation for the purpose
of making lives better. And those are very different things. They overlap. The former
is a process to the latter, but it's very easy and it's very easy to lose sight of that in the weeds.
So, I only call it out to say I don't think everybody would have come to the same conclusion
you did.
So, with that said, let's talk about how you and your colleagues at Yale and basically
thought of the right question to ask and then the right design
of an experiment to ask the right question.
First, I just want to say thank you and that is very well said.
Monkeys undergo cognitive decline in a very similar parallel fashion to humans. They have synaptic loss, the connection between neurons with aging,
as do humans do. And the circuits that are affected in aging are really similar to the
human circuits. So they are hippocampal frontal circuits, the hippocampus and the frontal lobes. And aging will preferentially target working
memory and also spatial memory and other types of memory. But this working memory holding something
in your mind, you go to the refrigerator, you open the door and you think, why did I come here? What
was holding that immediate memory in your mind is something that aging really targets. And monkeys undergo this very similar cognitive decline
to humans.
And there are ways to test monkeys
that interrogate those circuits.
In this case, a spatial delayed task was used.
Graham Williams and Stacey Kastner conducted the studies.
And clotho actually preferentially targets those
circuits that aging erodes. So here is a model system. It's genetically, anatomically, functionally
complex. It undergoes cognitive aging parallel to humans. And there are really well-developed
tests by these exceptional scientists who are very experienced and adept at conducting these tests.
So it was a really excellent setup
to ask, can Clotho enhance cognition
in these aging monkeys?
What was done is something called
a spatial delayed response.
And the monkeys presented with a bin of multiple wells.
And sometimes there's a few wells and sometimes there's a lot of wells.
And it's harder when there's a lot of wells to find a treat.
So a treat is placed in a well and they can see you place the treat and then a screen
drops and then all the wells are covered.
You cannot see where the treat is. And then
the blind is opened and they are tested where to choose because they're basing this on spatial
memory and on working memory where that treat was. Again, it's a little easier if there's
only three wells. It's easier to choose one out of three and get it correct. It's harder
when there's nine wells to remember spatial and working memory where that treat was.
It was done very rigorously, I must say.
The study was largely blinded.
And it was done in a way in which
that injection of clotho wouldn't interfere
with causing stress in memory so that everyone got a baseline.
They then got a vehicle. They then got a vehicle and then
they got vehicle or clotho. It's a really well designed study. And just for folks listening,
vehicle is placebo so that they understand what we mean by that. Thank you. So the monkeys that
got a clotho treatment, again, largely blinded, performed better than the ones that got vehicle treatment.
Sorry, Dina, how did you guys decide how to raise the dose? First of all, when you obviously did
all the mice experiments, you're dosing like you probably do based on a certain number of
milligrams per kilogram or milligrams per gram of animal. What did you use to dose escalate that?
Did you just assume the same dose per
unit body weight or did you make other adjustments?
Well, we went with two things in mind. One is we really wanted to stay at least with
one dose in a physiologic range. So with something that the body has been exposed to and has seen over its lifetime. And so in one
dose, we wanted to go somewhere up to like maybe four to five.
Four X. Yep. Yep. You're still just below what they would have been born at.
Right. A very useful level of clothodes, a rejuvenating dose. And then the other doses were higher and were meant to test, could you really push
the system to improve cognition even more than what is observed with that physiologic
dose? By the way, that physiologic dose, that sort of natural dose is what we have always
used in mice. I should say we've also given mice huge, huge doses
that were way beyond what they would ever see or used to. And it still enhanced cognition in mice.
That's going to be different than the monkey story.
06.05.00
06.05.00 By the way, when you gave the supraphysiologic doses to mice, so presumably
doses to mice, so presumably tenfold and beyond. Did you run into any problems?
For example, did the mice ever develop antibodies to the protein?
Did anything else arise that would suggest that more is not always better?
Dr. Kirsten Krohman We didn't see signals in mice.
But having said that, we didn't systematically study whether doses of a hundred micrograms per kilogram was
doing something different. We didn't observe differences in their normal behaviors or like
in their basic blood work, but it worked. It didn't work better. So I should say like those
super low doses were just as well as the super high doses in mice.
And super low doses mean well below 3x normal.
So if we just call 6x the peak physiologic dose, what was the minimum effective dose
in mice relative to that?
More than five times less.
A very, very small dose that still worked. I'm thinking if we gave mice
10 micrograms per kilogram, I'm remembering something like 0.5 or one enhanced cognition,
32 micrograms enhanced cognition. So very, very low, just a touch more, just a boost
enhanced cognition. Our strategy was to stay within a physiologic
dose and make sure that that was represented in the monkey studies. I mean, I really thought
about this, consulted with people that have taken drugs to market. Tom Boone is one of them and was
very instructive and taught me a lot in terms of thinking about relevantly dosing in monkeys.
And then there were higher doses that created much higher doses than the body has seen,
which is maybe 10 times higher and beyond, 10, maybe 20 times higher and beyond.
And what we found is that the low physiologic, that natural dose of clotho enhanced cognition
in the monkeys. And it did so within
four hours. And then that cognition stayed better. Their ability to think, remember stayed
better for at least three weeks, 14 to 21 days. And some were even tested out to a month.
I saw 28 days. Yeah.
And then at some point the study had to be stopped.
It's a million dollars or more and so,
but it was remarkable that one sub-q dose,
like a shot of ozempic would be given,
a sub-q dose that was low physiologic
had an immediate effect on cognitive enhancement
that lasted for a very long time. Now, there were
a few different types of tests done. One was for normal memory load and one was for high memory
load. And the high memory load was a test with a lot of different bins where they had to remember
which one among seven or eight wells was the treat hidden in. It's just more taxing with more
choices to remember. And they did even better in that study of high memory load.
With a low dose.
With the low dose. So the effect was particularly pronounced. They were particularly smarter when the task became harder. And again, why this task, why monkeys,
why clotho? It all came together because clotho works on these circuits that are tested in the
monkeys that decline with aging and with neurodegenerative diseases like Alzheimer's
and Parkinson's. So what about the high dose? Right. So the high dose did not work.
The high doses that gave the monkey's clotho way beyond what they've seen in their lifetime,
they didn't harm.
They did not impair cognition, but they didn't help cognition.
And it's not too much of a stretch to think that too much of something that does multiple
things in the body and maybe multiple pathways that could just create an imbalance that doesn't
support cognitive function.
Again, it wasn't harmful with the measures that we tested, but it didn't help to take
so much more than the body has seen and is used to.
And that again was different from the mice.
So the mice, and this may be a big difference between mice
and a more complex brain with the non-human primate.
In mice, we saw continued cognitive enhancement.
And in monkeys, we really got a window into,
before going into clinical trials,
that if we're giving clotho,
probably should be thinking about a specific
therapeutic window, one that the body knows, one that the body is used to. So Dina, why do you think
the effect lasts for so long? This is also a little counterintuitive when you consider the
little counterintuitive when you consider the hourly variation of clotho naturally,
when you consider the transient effects potentially of things like exercise where you see these large boosts over a short period of time following a bout of exercise, but it seems hard to imagine that the benefits of an hour of exercise persist three weeks later, the way a subcutaneous
injection of clotho did.
So, what do you think accounts for that?
I don't know the answer, but I'll speculate.
First, I'll just say it's remarkable, isn't it?
It's particularly remarkable as we imagine therapeutics in that maybe this is the
type of treatment of rejuvenation that could be administered maybe once a month, once every three
months as a shot in the arm, for example. So I think it has really important therapeutic
implications. Mechanistically, this means that clotho has
not just an acute effect of immediately enhancing NMDA receptor functions, synaptic plasticity,
cognitive function at four hours, but it has an organizational effect. It's doing something to,
in the longer term, really help to, for example, remodel a synapse.
So I would imagine that, for example, with GLUE N2B being trafficked to the synapse and
to promote better functions, that those sort of synaptic organizational effects are happening
for and staying put for a longer period of time.
I think the biology of this organizational effect of clotho has yet to be discovered,
but it's something that's happening at the synapse.
I have a very strong sense of something that's happening organizationally, structurally at
the synapse.
05.
As much as I want to go deeper into that and talk about the conformational changes that
could be occurring, we'll have to save that for another discussion because where we have
to really go next is what do we think about in humans?
And what evidence do we have, because we do have some really interesting evidence, even absent a single experiment,
that everything we've talked about so far might indeed also be relevant to the species of interest.
And no disrespect to the mice and the macaques, but there aren't too many people listening to this
whose mind isn't already wondering, okay, enough about the animals already. Tell me, is this
going to make a difference for my mom or for my dad who are in the early stages of dementia?
Is this going to make a difference for me because of my risk factors, even though it's
20 years from now?
And so tell me a little bit about a particular SNP associated with the clotho gene called
KLVS and what its significance is to this story.
None of this matters, Peter, if it doesn't have potential to help the human condition.
It just doesn't matter.
It's interesting, but the big and important is if it's relevant and may work
in humans. I don't know whether we've done anything big until we test clotho in humans.
So, with that said, back in the early days in, I think it was 2012 or so, or 2011, 2012,
when these first clotho studies, we were doing them in mice and discovering
these cognitive enhancing effects.
I went to, and again, I'm a physician scientist.
I always have humans in mind.
I have my patients in mind, Alzheimer's disease, 50 million people around the world.
This is going to triple by 2050.
Cognitive decline is our biggest biomedical challenge.
Always have humans in mind.
I went to my friend and colleague, Jennifer Yokoyama,
at the Memorand Aging Center here at UCSF.
And I told her that there was this genetic variant of clotho
that had been found.
We can talk more about what that is.
But was there a way, because she's a geneticist,
and she and others at the
Memory and Aging Center, Joel Kramer, Bruce Miller, have built this incredible population
of individuals and patients, Alzheimer's disease with normal aging, frontotemporal dementia,
and they really carefully characterized them, their genetics, their blood biomarkers, et cetera. So I went to Jennifer and
I said, is there any way we can know whether this genetic variant of clotho, KLVS, has any
association with cognition? Because that would mean that this clotho is important to brain health.
It would mean that there was some link with humans and brain health. So what is KLBS that you mentioned and what does it associate with? As we've discussed, we all have clotho
circulating in our blood and around our brain. But some of us, about one to four, one to five of us
will carry a gene for clotho, a genetic code for
Clotho that leads to higher levels of production.
And KLVS refers to two single nucleotide polymorphisms that cause a difference in the coding of the
protein itself.
Again, so about one to four, one to five of us will
have KLVs. Other people are non-carriers. And those people with KLVs will genetically
have higher levels of clothocerculating in their blood. Now, interestingly, it's only
the people with one copy of KLVs, the heterozygotes, and very rarely there are
homozygotes of KLVS, and those people end up having multiple disadvantages in lifespan
and vulnerability to different diseases.
So when we talk about KLVS, we're talking specifically about heterozygosity carrying
one allele of it.
What are some of the health consequences of being homozygous and what is the prevalence
of homozygosity?
It's really rare.
So if the prevalence of heterozygosity is around 25% in populations, homozygosity is
so...
It's probably about 1 to 2% because actually it would kind of mirror APOE4 where
the heterozygosity is about the same.
It's about one in four and the homozygosity is about one in 50 to one in 100.
Yeah, that's about right.
It's pretty rare, but it exists and those people have shorter
lifespans. They have much lower levels of clotho and they are at risk for anything that the
heterozygosity helps with, the homozygosity hurts. But the heterozygosity has been a really
interesting window into clotho and natural experiments.
How much higher is their clotho level?
Well, we did this study.
Jennifer and I took the serum from individuals here at the Memorand Aging Center, a few hundred,
and tested the ones that were non-carriers that just have the typical clotho gene versus
the KLBS heterozygote carriers.
And the heterozygote carrier status increased clotho levels
by about 15%, 15, maybe 20%.
And just to give you context,
if you're wondering am I carrier
and some people are and some people aren't,
I don't want people to forget that exercise is thus far one of the more powerful modulators of clotho expression. And exercise
increases clotho on average by 30%, so much more than what the genetic influence is. But
nonetheless, KLVS increases clotho levels by about 15%. So for example, if an average number, let's say
a non-carrier had 800 picograms per mil of clotho circulating in their serum,
a KLVS carrier might have like 950 or so picograms per mil. That's what I'm remembering from our
graphs. There's reason to believe that it increases clotho because those
two amino acid changes that translate into a different amino acid in the protein itself
influences secretion of clotho from cells. There's a biologic reason that this variant
is probably changing cloth levels.
The other piece to this story,
Dina, that's just so fascinating
is now when you take those data,
the prevalence of heterozygosity of KLVS,
and you cross it with APOE4.
So now we have listeners of this podcast
are absolutely not strangers
to the population-based
risk of APOE4. Of course, we always want to remind people that at the individual level,
very difficult to make that statement. But at the population-based level,
we know that having an APOE3 and an APOE4 is at least a doubling of risk, maybe even slightly more.
risk, maybe even slightly more. And homozygosity for APOE4 could be an 8 to 12 fold increase, so call it a log order
increase in risk.
So tell me, what did you find in people who are homozygous and heterozygous for APOE4,
who also happen to be heterozygous, i.e. have the favorable variant of KLVS single copy.
This is really remarkable data.
I first want to go back to that original experiment
that we published in 2014, I believe, with Jennifer,
and that those people that carried the KLVS allele
did better across cognitive testing.
And this is a normal aging.
This was not an Alzheimer's disease,
but carrying that variant, carrying KLBS associated
with better cognition across the board.
I'm often asked, well, how much smarter
were those individuals?
And I would be careful about smarter and happier,
and we don't know, but the cognitive tests showed us that they
did better. And they did better to the same extent that APOE4 carriers would do worse.
So it's a sort of a similar amount of change in cognition in which KLBS was improving and
APOE4 was decreasing. With that in the background, since then, many groups and many
people have looked at KLBS in their populations. And that association has held largely in most
studies, not in all, but in most studies, there is an association of KLVS and better cognition with heterozygosity.
So then the question comes that you asked about APOE4, Alzheimer's risk and clotho in
KLVS.
We initially did a study in collaboration actually led by Osiyama Konkwo and his group
at Wisconsin that showed that in individuals with APOE4 that were carriers of KLBS, that they just had less
of the effects of ApoE4 in terms of less A beta deposition, less cognitive problems. It was a
smaller study of a few hundred people at risk for Alzheimer's disease, but it was a good study.
But I'll tell you the study that I'm most excited about that we were not a part of that
came from Stanford led by Mikhail Beloy. He's one to watch. He just established a lab at
Wash U and his senior PI, Michael Grecius, and their wizards at doing genetic population studies.
So Peter, they did this remarkable study of 22 different cohorts that were normal cognition,
MCI, mild cognitive impairment that you mentioned earlier, and Alzheimer's disease, over 20,000
individuals, a very large meta-analysis, and they looked at if there was a relationship
between KLVS carriers and ApoE4. And the bottom line of what they found was that if an individual
carried KLVS, the ApoE4 didn't matter. So let me break down what that actually meant in their experiment.
That meant that in those people that carry ApoE4, if they were also heterozygote for
KLBS, they had a decreased risk for developing Alzheimer's disease that was pretty close to the normal population. They had a decreased
conversion from MCI to AD, and they had decreased Alzheimer's biomarkers, both in their CSF and in
their brain. They just had less amyloid beta. It's a really striking study. Again, because it has
the power of statistical analysis, it holds across many,
many cohorts and it's many, many people. KLVS essentially blocks and abrogates the
ApoE4 toxicity by this genetic associations. Really remarkable. Did the double E4s, did the homozygotes reduce to a completely normal 3-3 risk?
My recollection, but it's been a while since I looked, there's a graph that demonstrates
all of this that's a great summary.
My recollection, which could be wrong, that's why I'm asking, was that the 3-4s were completely
abrogated to a 3-3 risk and that the 4-4s ended up coming way down, but they still looked like more
of a 3-4. Am I misremembering that?
They didn't actually publish that. They excluded the homozygotes from the analysis, but it
might be in a supplement somewhere, but I actually reached out to Mikhail and I asked
him about the 4-4. I had the same question. And so this is just by verbal communication, he indicated that the 4-4 also associated
with the protection from the KLBS heterozygosity.
So that the effect of having one ApoE4 wasn't different than having two ApoE4s in terms
of KLBS protection.
It protected in both heterozygosity and homozygosity of the APOE4 allele.
Again, it's just through verbal communication, it might have been in a supplement somewhere.
So, did they find anything negative?
So going way back to the mechanism of this, which at least in part is communicated through a platelet factor. Was there anything that might have been
unwarranted such as an increase in stroke risk? I know that it's actually the opposite,
but I'm just trying to say, is there anything that with an increase in a platelet factor
that could have led to an increase in DVTs or something? When you have a sample size as large
as they did, presumably you would find something
that was negatively associated with the KLVS.
I'm not aware of increase.
As you were saying, it's actually the opposite.
The KLVS, heterozygosity, actually
associates with the protection against stroke risks,
cardiovascular risks.
Even metabolic risk.
And metabolic, yeah, metabolic diseases.
I've searched for something that could be negative. And I did find that in the cancer literature,
KLBS heterozygosity has a poor prognostic indicates a poor prognosis in BRCA1 carriers
indicates a poorer prognosis in BRCA1 carriers with breast cancer. There are examples where it's not helpful and one is in BRCA1 positive breast cancer.
Deana, going back to the human homozygotes for KLVS, do we know how much of an increase
in clotho they produce? If the heterozygotes are producing 15 to
maybe 20% more, how much more clotho is being produced by the homozygotes and does that give
us an insight into toxicity in humans that was not observed in mice and not even observed in
the primates? Because in the primates when you gave too much,
you just didn't get a benefit, but you didn't get a harm. In the mice, you actually got more
and more benefit. As the organisms get more and more complicated, it seems that the therapeutic
window is getting narrower and narrower. Do the homozygotes give us any insight into that?
With Jen here at the Memorand Aging Center, we have access to homozygote serum, and we
actually looked at several homozygotes compared to non-carriers and heterozygotes.
And what we found is that their clotho levels are actually lower than normal.
And we think that has something to do with, I'm going to get a little deeper into this,
because I think it's really important
when we think about therapeutics actually, that many genetic variants won't cause a change
in the protein sequence itself.
If a nucleotide change is in an intron, it's just not going to change the structure or
function or anything of the protein. But in KLVS, there are two nucleotide
changes and they translate into different amino acids in two places of the clothoprotein.
And so what we think is happening, this is speculation, but what we think is happening
based on in vitro studies that Arking did in 2002, that in the clophto heterozygote in which
one allele is making a mutant protein, we think that there's an overcompensation for that mutant
protein by increasing wild type levels. Because what that variant does is it mucks up clothose secretion from the cell. So just to be
clear, carrying one variant is likely changing the structure and function of clotho, but there's
probably an overcompensation from the wild type allele causing higher levels of the wild type
clotho. First of all, that is an unbelievable,
I would not have guessed that,
and I can't believe I didn't think to ask
such an obvious question, which is,
do the KLVS individuals produce the same protein?
I took it for granted that they produced the same protein
and just made more of it, but to be clear,
if you assay those individuals, are you finding two
proteins in them? A protein that mirrors the wild type, that's the one that's elevated 15 to 20%
and an actual mutated protein or a protein that has two different amino acids, which again,
to many people sounds like big deal. You've got a thousand amino acids in the full protein. You've changed two of them.
How much can it matter? Well, unfortunately in biology,
it can always matter. Ask a patient with sickle cell anemia.
So is that what you're seeing?
You're seeing two different proteins and it's the wild type one that is over
expressed.
At this point,
it's the hypothesis that the wild type one is really compensating,
overcompensating by increasing levels
for what may be a mutant protein.
But in reality, and we've done ELISAs
on the enzyme linked to immunoassays
on thousands and thousands of individuals,
but the ELISA itself doesn't distinguish
between a KLBS protein and a normal wild type clothoprotein.
It doesn't distinguish between the two.
And so the answer to that question is not known.
ELISA's just too blunt an instrument to figure that out.
If we take that a step further and say,
what's happening in a clothothopomozygote, they're expressing
a mutant protein from one allele that mucks up secretion, and they're expressing a mutant
protein from another allele that mucks up secretion.
And so they don't have wild type clothe.
All of their clothe is the KLBS form and it's very low.
How much lower is it, Dina?
In our studies, it was, I'm trying to visualize the graph. If there was a 15% increase with
heterozygosity compared to non-carriers, there was probably a 30% decrease with homozygosity
compared to non-carriers. So to give you a sense of numbers, if a non-carrier was at 800, a KLVS homozygous carrier was
below 600, something like that.
This is why this matters.
It matters for many reasons, but there have been some really large scale studies recently,
Peter. One is called the N. Haines study published in 2022, I believe.
And what this study showed us is that clotho levels as measured by this immunoassay by
Eliza, that the clotho levels really correlated with mortality.
I want to make sure I get this right. So it was over 10,000 people in the United States.
Clotho serum was measured on everyone.
The mean age was about 56 years.
And if the mean clotho level was around 800,
then something less than what they defined as 666
by their study, picograms per mil, associated with
a 30% mortality over five years.
And that was replicated in another study called Inchianti, that lower levels of clotho.
And again, this isn't like half the levels.
Maybe it's like 30% of a decrease of levels associated with a 30% mortality over five years. And
that mortality was primarily in cancer and cardiovascular disease. I think levels are
going to matter as we think about our human aging, as we think about our organismal health,
heart health, brain health, cancer health, the levels, at least by association right now,
matter and lower levels are really associated with more diseases of aging.
It's just like, can we even scratch the surface of this? I mean, we've just sat here and talked
for two plus hours about the limits of our understanding of clotho and brain health and
yet this NHANES study is actually looking at
something totally different which is all-cause mortality and saying basically if you're in your
50s and your clotho levels are 30% below what would be expected for someone to your age,
it's associated with a 30% increase in all-cause mortality over the next five years. If that turns out to be causal,
and you're saying that the manner in which that death was distributed was cardiovascular and
cancer, so it doesn't even have to do with more dementia, which of course wouldn't be kicking in,
in your 50s, it suggests that clotho is doing even more than just protecting brain health, which if it did nothing else would still be arguably the most important thing
that we should be trying to get into humans for clinical trials.
So I know that your area of expertise is not oncology or cardiovascular medicine,
but I'm just curious as to what your thoughts are as to how that could be
happening.
I couldn't agree with you more.
And my expertise is as a neurologist and neuroscientist is really on the brain.
But having said that the side effects of clotho increasing it may be much more than in helping
brain health.
It clearly has a very strong association with protection against cancer, cardiovascular
disease and kidney disease.
There is, Peter, a very, very, very large literature
on clotho and organ health, a very strong one.
In the cancer field, there are many preclinical models,
again, in rats and mice, that show that giving clotho
the soluble hormonal form actually stopped
and reversed cancers in mice, like pancreatic cancer, for example.
And then there are association studies in humans too, but there is so much more to the
biology of clotho than only what it's doing in the brain.
In kidney disease, clothothe is gonna come to market
and maybe the kidney people beat the brain people.
If clothe comes to market,
I think people are gonna benefit no matter what.
But in chronic kidney disease,
there is a decrease in clothe secretion
and decrease in clothe in the blood.
And the kidney folks, the kidney specialists
are really developing
clotho as a diagnostic biomarker to understand when the kidney starts failing. The idea there
is that right now the measures are glomerular filtration rate, urea, and these are not as
sensitive as they would want biomarkers to be in detecting kidney dysfunction.
As soon as the kidney starts having trouble, clotho declines. As soon as it starts having
trouble, so there's a fervent interest in developing clotho as a biomarker for kidney function.
And I would say, again, if we think in a broader sense about clotho, maybe someday and maybe
someday soon we would have our clotho levels checked just as we do our blood pressure and
our cholesterol and we get breast exams and we have colonoscopies.
Why not have a clotho level checked?
Everyone's will be different.
Now, how do we get around the issue that it has this diurnal effect?
That's the only thing that jumps to my mind when you're talking about a marker of EGFR
because I know in our practice, we're fanatical about monitoring EGFR.
We look at cystatin C, we look at creatinine, we're triangulating them.
At least to our ability, these don't depend terribly on time of day.
Do you think that the answer for clotho is you have to have it done 60 minutes after
you wake up in the morning, it's non-negotiable, don't ever let somebody check your clotho
level at two in the afternoon, otherwise we can't interpret it?
Is that how you think it's going to factor in?
I think that is going to give us the best and more accurate level of clotho.
And in fact, I mentioned that we've done thousands and thousands of clotho
measurements and human serum and human CSF, cerebral spinal fluid.
And I always make sure that whoever we're collaborating with and who has collected
the specimen did it in a morning fasting manner.
So the individual or patient hasn't eaten and it's first thing in the morning.
Why do you know that eating impacts cloth levels?
I don't know that and I don't know that it's really been tested per se,
but in the spirit of what one does, for example, just being incredibly consistent
and rigorous and with less confounding factors in the spirit of what one does, for example- Just being incredibly consistent and rigorous in a way.
Yeah, with less confounding factors in the background.
But having said that, the decline over the course of a day isn't dramatic.
The decline by age is more relevant than the decline by time of day?
I would say that the decline by age is a consistent decline, but the time of day
does matter because it can decrease by almost 40% by midnight. And this is done with, I have to say,
I've looked through the literature, the diurnal rhythm of cloth has been shown in a small number
of people, but the data look good. That it's not really statistically different until like afternoon, but it's starting
to decline. I had to design the time and day and I would say that we need a clotho test
that people take in the morning, fasting like they do with their cholesterol, just get it
along with the cholesterol, draw. And most importantly, we need a standardized assay.
Yeah. I was about to say, is there a CLIA based assay for this?
No, no. I think it's coming given how many people and how many samples levels haven't
been coordinated between institutions and labs. So everything we do in our lab is
consistent to our methods and standards and we standardize everything to, and I include my own
samples in there, but it's all standardized. But then my lab may have slightly different
values than what is going on in another lab. Then of course, so if I woke up in the morning
and I checked my level and I was at 800, is it picograms per milliliter? Is that the unit?
Yeah. Yeah. If I did my test at 6 o'clock in the morning and I'm it picograms per milliliter? Is that the unit? Yeah.
If I did my test at six o'clock in the morning and I'm 800
picograms per milliliter, and then from seven o'clock to nine
o'clock in the morning, I'm in the gym, I could easily be 1200
picograms per milliliter after.
So how does that impact our understanding of what's going on?
I think that's a really good important point.
I do want to emphasize that the human studies
show chronic exercise increases clotho
by after a few months, after three months.
And there's been a mouse study that shows an acute bout
of exercise can really nearly double clotho.
But I think we would be interested again,
we're imagining a not so distant world.
It would be important to get a baseline level.
Where are you living?
So just to be clear, you're saying you do not have human data
that demonstrate that acute bouts of exercise
dramatically change you.
I have not seen that.
I have not seen that.
I know a person who might be willing
to do that experiment for you.
We could discuss that offline.
Sure.
Okay. So, final question on this, Dina, because I know it's come up for us a lot in our practice.
So, I'm hoping you have a better solution for us. We do like to test our patients, especially those
who are higher risk for Alzheimer's disease. We do like to test to see if they have the KLVS polymorphisms.
It is very difficult to get that information.
Are you guys doing that work yourselves?
Are you outsourcing that?
How are you guys getting that information on your own?
I realize that much of the research you're doing is on a database where that's been done, but are you aware of any commercially easy ways for individuals to determine their
KLVS status, should they choose to know?
I don't know the exact answer to this.
I know that the information is represented somewhere on 23andMe.
We can't seem to get it out of 23 and me, even when we use Prometheus.
I may be wrong about that.
Yeah, we can't seem to do it.
Hopefully somebody listening can say, no, you're wrong, Peter.
You just got to do this, but we haven't figured it out.
Yeah, I have to say that in our case, when we collaborate with the clinical researchers,
they have their populations completely genotyped for Clopo ApoE4 and most other genes out there with the polymorphisms.
There's been a really deep sequencing and genetic screen of all of those individuals,
but I'll have to circle back to you about that.
Okay. Well, Dina, this has been an, I mean, I think at the risk of being hyperbolic,
just an insanely fascinating discussion. And I'm so happy to be able to have
discussed your work with you and then by extension to be able to put this in front of so many people
because I do think that for a disease like Alzheimer's disease that if we're being brutally
honest of all the what I call the four horsemen of death, if there's one that we have very, very little to offer patients,
once they're in the throes of the disease, this is the sad poster child for it.
It is unfortunately today, I don't need to tell you what you see every day,
but I think people watching this understand that even with the advent of anti-emolloid
therapies, they have barely, barely been able to put a dent in this enormous problem.
It's for that reason that I'm very excited about this and just applaud you on your career.
I think this is only the beginning.
We have to get this into human clinical trials.
That's what's next.
Thank you so much for this invitation.
It's been really, really fun talking to you. Alzheimer's disease is one of our biggest biomedical challenges.
Our entire world is aging.
It used to be the US, Japan, Sweden, but it's the entire world.
China, India, Africa, all continents are aging.
We're aging rapidly.
Again, this is one of our biggest biomedical problems.
We really do
not have effective therapies. And I'm hopeful that with multiple shots on goal right now,
something will come to market that provides that resilience. In addition to anti-amyloid therapies,
that is a cocktail that provides resilience for the suffering that really erodes our memory.
It destroys families, economies.
It's just a devastating problem.
We're glad to be working on it and we hope that we can do something big and
important, but that remains to be seen.
I also just wanted to say I'm so lucky to be doing what I love to do.
Really, really love to do.
to be doing what I love to do, really, really love to do. And I am also lucky enough to have a very diverse portfolio
of being funded by the NIH, by foundations,
and in the past by biotech and also by philanthropy.
I can't emphasize enough how much friends and supporters
of our lab have enabled really risky science, have enabled us to
just take a leap, ask big questions, take a big risk, and see what happens and win or lose to
really see what happens when we go down that route. And I think that's so important to progress in
science is to do something risky, to take a risk. So I'm very grateful to be doing what I'm doing.
I'm glad you mentioned that.
I do think that philanthropy can have an enormous impact
in science when philanthropists come in
with the right attitude, which is,
I want to fund something that is asymmetric
in the following way.
It is too risky for, the NIH to fund or too
risky for industry to come in and fund, but yet it has enough biologic plausibility and enough
potential upside that if it works, it's a game changer. And look, the reality of it is, I think
Clotho is a poster child for that type of work.
At least as of this moment, it looks like it's actually paying off.
I and countless others are grateful for what's been done.
Dina, thank you so much for your time today.
Really, really appreciate it.
Thank you.
This was wonderful.
Thanks.
Thank you for listening to this week's episode of The Drive.
It's extremely important to me to provide all of this content without relying on paid
ads.
To do this, our work is made entirely possible by our members, and in return, we offer exclusive
member-only content and benefits above and beyond what is available for free.
So if you want to take your knowledge of this space to the next level, it's our goal to
ensure members get back much more than the price of the subscription. Premium
membership includes several benefits. First, comprehensive podcast show
notes that detail every topic, paper, person and thing that we discuss in
each episode. And the word on the street is nobody's show notes rival
ours. Second, monthly Ask Me Anything or AMA episodes.
These episodes are comprised of detailed responses
to subscriber questions, typically focused
on a single topic and are designed
to offer a great deal of clarity and detail
on topics of special interest to our members.
You'll also get access to the show notes
for these episodes, of course.
Third, delivery of our premium newsletter,
which is put together by our dedicated
team of research analysts. This newsletter covers a wide range of topics related to longevity
and provides much more detail than our free weekly newsletter. Fourth, access to our private
podcast feed that provides you with access to every episode including AMA's Sans le
Spiel you're listening to now, and in your
regular podcast feed. Fifth, the Qualis, an additional member-only podcast we put
together that serves as a highlight reel featuring the best excerpts from
previous episodes of The Drive. This is a great way to catch up on previous
episodes without having to go back and listen to each one of them. And finally,
other benefits that are added along the way. If you want to go back and listen to each one of them. And finally, other benefits that are added along the way.
If you want to learn more
and access these member-only benefits,
you can head over to peteratiamd.com forward slash subscribe.
You can also find me on YouTube, Instagram, and Twitter,
all with the handle peteratiamd.
You can also leave us a review on Apple Podcasts
or whatever podcast player you use.
This podcast is for general informational purposes only and does not constitute the practice of medicine,
nursing or other professional health care services, including the giving of medical advice.
No doctor patient relationship is formed.
The use of this information and the materials linked to this podcast is at the user's own risk.
The content on this podcast is not intended user's own risk. The content on this podcast
is not intended to be a substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis, or treatment. Users
should not disregard or delay in obtaining medical advice from any medical condition they have,
and they should seek the assistance of their healthcare professionals for any such conditions.
Finally, I take all conflicts of interest very seriously. For all of my disclosures and the companies I invest in or advise, please visit peteratiamd.com
forward slash about where I keep an up-to-date and active list of all disclosures. Thanks for watching!