The Peter Attia Drive - #314 ‒ Rethinking nutrition science: the evolving landscape of obesity treatment, GLP-1 agonists, protein, and the need for higher research standards | David Allison, Ph.D.

Episode Date: August 19, 2024

View the Show Notes Page for This Episode Become a Member to Receive Exclusive Content Sign Up to Receive Peter’s Weekly Newsletter David Allison, a leading expert in obesity and nutrition, quant...itative genetics, clinical trials, and research methodology, returns to The Drive to explore the evolving landscape of nutrition science and obesity treatment. In this episode, David begins by discussing the intricate relationship between nutrition, obesity, and body composition, emphasizing the multifaceted impacts of food beyond mere calorie intake. David provides a critical analysis of the complexities in nutrition research and their practical implications for tackling obesity. He critiques historical public health policies, addresses the trust issues plaguing nutrition science, and underscores the need for a paradigm shift to more effectively combat obesity. The conversation also delves into the rise of GLP-1 receptor agonists like Ozempic and Mounjaro, exploring their ethical and practical considerations in obesity treatment. The episode concludes with an in-depth look at protein intake recommendations and highlights the significant research gaps that remain in the field. We discuss: The complex relationship between nutrition, body weight, and body composition [2:30]; The slow progress in addressing obesity and public health despite substantial effort and investment [7:30]; The very limited success of public health initiatives in curbing obesity [17:15]; The evolving landscape of obesity research: public health initiatives and the impact of pharmacological success [26:30]; Rethinking obesity solutions: the need for a paradigm shift [32:45]; Understanding environmental triggers and embracing a balanced approach to addressing obesity that includes both pharmacological treatments and realistic lifestyle changes [41:45]; The need for higher standards in obesity research [51:45]; The rapid success of GLP-1 receptor agonists for weight loss: a discussion on the societal impact and controversy of their growing usage [1:02:15]; The ethical and practical considerations of obesity drugs: risks, benefits, and motivations for usage [1:11:30]; The use of GLP-1 agonists by athletes as performance enhancers [1:23:45]; Unanswered questions about protein intake and health [1:30:45]; Future research needed to understand basic questions around protein intake [1:45:00]; David’s weekly newsletter: “Obesity and Energetics Offerings” [1:50:45]; and More. Connect With Peter on Twitter, Instagram, Facebook and YouTube

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Hey everyone, welcome to the Drive Podcast. I'm your host, Peter Attia. This podcast, my website, and my weekly newsletter all focus on the goal of translating the science of longevity into something accessible for everyone. Our goal is to provide the best content in health and wellness, and we've established a great team of analysts to make this happen. It is extremely important to me to provide all of this content without relying on paid ads. To do this, our work is made entirely possible by our members, and in return, we offer exclusive member-only content and benefits above and beyond what is available for free.
Starting point is 00:00:46 If you want to take your knowledge of this space to the next level, it's our goal to ensure members get back much more than the price of a subscription. If you want to learn more about the benefits of our premium membership, head over to PeterAtiyaMD.com forward slash subscribe. My guest this week is David Allison who returns to the drive for a second sit down. David is currently the Dean and Provost Professor at the Indiana University Bloomington School of Public Health. He's authored over 500 scientific publications and received many awards and his research
Starting point is 00:01:19 interests include obesity and nutrition, quantitative genetics, clinical trials, statistical and research methodology, and research rigor and integrity. In our conversation today, we discussed the relationship between nutrition, obesity, and body composition, and how food affects body composition beyond caloric intake. This leads us to a discussion
Starting point is 00:01:39 around the complexity of nutrition research studies and how confusion continues to remain with translating knowledge into practical outcomes such as reducing obesity. We talk about the public health efforts and policy and why they have failed historically in regard to obesity and why there's such a trust problem with nutrition science. Next we dive into the emergence of GLP-1 agonists in treating obesity and what is happening both socially and psychologically with drugs like ozempic and manjaro. GLP-1 agonists in treating obesity, and what is happening both socially and psychologically
Starting point is 00:02:05 with drugs like Ozempic and Manjaro. We end the discussion talking about protein intake and the adequacy of current protein intake recommendations and the research gaps that lie between what we are told and maybe what is actually known. Overall, this was a fascinating and philosophical at times discussion on the evolving landscape of nutrition science, obesity treatment and the impact of research. Without further delay, please enjoy my conversation with David Allison. David, good to see you once again.
Starting point is 00:02:39 Good to see you, my friend. Lots to talk about today. The world of nutrition and health are always in the spotlight, in particular around a class of drugs that no listener to this podcast will be a stranger to called GLP-1 agonists. I want to spend some time talking about those, but I think before doing so, I want to just maybe go back and talk a little bit about what we know and maybe don't know about the relationship between nutrition and obesity, which sounds like it should be obvious. Tell us what you think is actually known about the relationship between food and body composition.
Starting point is 00:03:18 I like the way you phrased the question and using the word body, the phrase body composition as opposed to just obesity or weight. There are obviously three different things. Obesity implies a threshold, you're too much. There's a judgment about the effects of the excess. Then there's body composition, the tissue, how much is fat, how much is lean, where is the fat lean, what is the fat composed of, what is the lean composed of. And then there's just weight, which is just your mass on this planet. Those three things are highly related but not identical.
Starting point is 00:03:51 What we know indisputably and even people who sort of rail against something they call the energy balance model, which you and I have discussed, whether it's really a model is unclear. It's really more of a constraint. It's really a restatement of the first law of thermodynamics, which is the law of conservation. Matter and energy can neither be created nor destroyed, but only converted. It is a constraint by which all other descriptions of what happens with weight and mass and food intake and energy intake and energy expenditure must operate. It's not a description or an explanation of what happens. It just says if you describe any proposed explanation of what happens,
Starting point is 00:04:33 it's got to follow that first law of thermodynamics in order to make sense. And that first law of thermodynamics in the field of nutritional obesity often gets stated as something like changes in energy storage equal changes in energy intake minus changes in energy output or delta energy stores equals delta energy in minus delta energy out. Food intake can affect those things. Alternatively, you could say that energy intake is one of those things. So it gets back to that descriptive thing. Now one of the questions becomes, how does all the other aspects of food besides the
Starting point is 00:05:11 mere energy content of it affect the amount of weight one gains or loses, the body composition, the tissues, where the mass is distributed, what types of tissues it's in, composition of those tissues. And then of course, whether or not one exceeds some threshold, there's every reason to believe that many, many aspects of food from the marketing and pricing of it, which then can influence the intake of it and other things as well to the taste, the smell, the timing, what you eat it with, what it's combined with, phytochemicals in it, micronutrients, macronutrients,
Starting point is 00:05:54 all can affect energy expenditure, subsequent energy intake or nutrient partitioning, which is a fancy phrase for where you stick the energy that you store in the body. Do you stick it into fat or muscle or bone or visceral fat or subcutaneous fat, et cetera? So all those things can come into play. Now, what do we really know? The truth is I think what we know is modest and partly that's because it seems to me to be very specific. That is, we can do a study and even when it's honestly done and well done and honestly reported and we find that in this species with this delivery of this composition in this way, this thing happens. And then when you look in a different species or a slightly different food,
Starting point is 00:06:48 you get different results. So there's many, many studies saying, well, we got this with pea protein and casein, but not whey, or we got it with whey, but not casein, or we got it when we fed it two hours before the test meal, but not one hour before, or we got it in men, but not in women. This makes me think we're talking often about subtle effects that may not be that clinically reliable and meaningful. And so the really big effect seems to be how many calories do you eat,
Starting point is 00:07:24 but all these other aspects of food may then influence how many calories do you eat. But all these other aspects of food may then influence how many calories you eat either of that food or in subsequent occasions. Those can seem to have big effects but we're still sort of I think trying to suss those out. Do you ever spend time interacting with physicists or chemists, biochemists who sit on the sidelines and look at the field of human energetics and wonder to themselves, why is there so much noise and why is there so little understanding? I don't think anybody is standing around blaming the scientists and saying, well, in physics, we have great scientists. In chemistry, we have great scientists. In human energetics, they must be subpar and that's why they don't know anything. I can't imagine there's anybody
Starting point is 00:08:13 that thinks that. What do you think it is at the meta level that explains the obvious but but important observation that our knowledge in this space is woefully deficient relative to the effort that has been put in to elucidating truth. Just to restate that more poignantly, for how hard the field of science has worked to try to get at the questions we're going to discuss today. Why do we know very little relative to the same amounts of effort that have gone into physical sciences, for example? I think there are many reasons. Some are perceptual and some are actual. Some of the perceptuals do we really know that much less and we can argue about it. I think there's still questions in physics where you say, gee, we really don't know that exactly how is it that relativity and quantum physics are compatible or is dark matter real or what have you. I think there are questions there. Though I would sort of just interject for a second, David, and say another way to think of
Starting point is 00:09:20 it would be if you look at the amazing progress that has been made, that has been enabled by the knowledge of physics and chemistry. If you just consider what's happened in the last 100 years in terms of what we've been able to do, just look at computing power, look at semiconductors, look at airplanes. Look at technology that has been enabled by engineering, physics, chemistry, we're multiple logs of advancement. The same cannot be said of what we're talking about now. Our understanding of obesity 100 years ago versus our understanding of obesity today, while maybe greater, hasn't actually translated into a multiple log improvement
Starting point is 00:10:07 in the outcome of interest, which in this case might be a reduction of obesity, just as it might be in the interest on the other side, which would be computing power. I think that's only half true. I think we don't give ourselves certain credit for certain things. In physics, there's not a lot of discussion. In modern times of the power of Newton's universal law of gravitation, those are pretty big deal and a pretty big accomplishment, but we don't talk about it a lot. It's been figured out a long time ago
Starting point is 00:10:33 and we take it for granted that we know that now. But do we even have the equivalent in energetics? Yeah, I think we do. Some simple examples, both at the practical level. In this country and in most industrialized countries, there's very little food shortage. That's a big deal. It is a big deal that we know that alcohol contains calories.
Starting point is 00:10:54 We take that for granted, but Wilber Atwater, who's the person who stated that, was vilified for it at that time by the temperance movement. And he himself was a teetotaler, by the way, that alcohol had no nutritional value and he said no it doesn't, it's seven kilocalories per gram. So that's an example, folate supplementation which has radically reduced spina bifida, iodized salt, micronutrient deficiencies being maybe not eradicated in this country but radically reduced to among other things
Starting point is 00:11:26 to supplementation, greater food safety. So, we've made a lot of practical progress. We've feeding a number of people through nutrition and agriculture that back all the way to Malthus but even more recently in the 1970s when we were told there was going to be a population explosion that would threaten our ability as a species. But isn't that really more about agriculture than nutrition science? It's agriculture, it's food science, but some of the nutrition science is more the micronutrients all the way back to eliminating scurvy through the work of James Lind and figuring out eventually that it was vitamin C. They first thought it was just citrus in general. They didn't understand it was vitamin C to the folate
Starting point is 00:12:09 and so forth. I think our notions, our understanding about LDL cholesterol, which again, you know more than I about is very important, the role of saturated fats and that. We're still learning more, but we do know some things about that. So I don't think we want to take for granted that we have learned a great deal in obesity itself until about, oh, I don't know, maybe five years ago or a little more. When I would give talks about this, I would say we actually have learned a lot, but it's just not all that clinically relevant. And what's clinically relevant is mostly truly in the clinic, not in the community and the population.
Starting point is 00:12:48 So I said, we've learned a lot about genetics. And that's true. We have log orders, I would argue, magnitude increase in our knowledge about the genetic underpinnings of obesity that we didn't have prior to 1980. But until recently, we've had moderate improvements in the clinic and virtually no improvements in the sort of public health community domain. If you allow me to be humored with an analogy though, just because I'm going to keep pushing back on this a little bit. When the Wright brothers first put an airplane into the sky, I don't think anybody would have said aviation is amazing. That was a proof of concept. It was a wonderful example. But I think
Starting point is 00:13:34 it's safe to say that almost monotonically aviation has become safer and safer and safer over the past hundred years. I think that allows us to say our understanding of Newtonian physics, Bernoulli's principle, material science, all of the things that enable aviation to be what it is today relative to a hundred years ago are probably getting better and we're also getting better at applying them to a real world problem. Conversely, if the rate of airplanes falling out of the sky were increasing steadily over the past 50 years, such that in 1970, whatever, 10% of airplanes fell out of the sky, but today 50% of airplanes fell out of the sky, I don't think anybody would be walking around
Starting point is 00:14:22 saying, we're doing really well. We understand much more about the physics of the airplane. Yes, it's true. More of them are falling out of the sky. And yet, I would argue that in the presence of all of this knowledge that we have, we're getting fatter and we're getting sicker. So, how do we reconcile the fact that our knowledge is somehow increasing and we're so much more knowledgeable and yet the actual problem that matters seems to be getting worse, not better. Right. Well, what we don't have is, again, with a couple of exceptions we're going to get to later, I think, we don't have the sort of sea change, the real orbit jumps in knowledge of a utilitarian, useful knowledge, knowledge
Starting point is 00:15:07 that helps us change the way we do things now that lead to better outcomes that we don't yet have. So we have useless knowledge. What's the contrapositive of that? We have knowledge that is useful for understanding and we hope we can build on to get to practical knowledge. Steve O'Reilly gave a nice talk about this about two years ago at the Royal Society meeting that he and I and others spoke at and hosted. He said, as a physician, geneticist, biochemist, who works in the field, he looks at this and he also thinks about his early
Starting point is 00:15:45 days in blood pressure. And when he started his career a few decades ago, he said, we didn't really have a lot of good drugs and blood pressure. And people kept hammering at the molecular biology and the biochemistry and the physiology of blood pressure. And bit by bit, things started to break. And he says, now we can treat blood pressure enormously better. He said, I think that's where we're going to go with obesity. He said,
Starting point is 00:16:10 but we're just sort of getting to the breaking point. I think that's what we're seeing now with the GLP-1 agonists as well as some other drugs. In other words, we might get to a point in 30 years where we're sitting here and obesity rates are back to the level they were 50 to 100 years ago. Virtually everybody will be on a drug which we may or may not understand the mechanism of action for. I think we will understand more of the mechanism of action 30 and 50 years from now but it is true that today we don't fully understand the mechanisms of action. I think that's a reasonable analogy comparing it to blood pressure or comparing it to lipid management for that matter. Even 40 years ago, we didn't really have tools to manage lipids. Where is the investment going? There,
Starting point is 00:16:54 the investment was going not only but heavily toward biochemistry, molecular genetics, physiology and pharmaceuticals. We are now seeing an uptick in that. We've seen an uptick and we're seeing more of an uptick in that because some success is being achieved. And the pharma companies, many of which who over the last few decades would be tepidly in and out, they dipped their toe in the water of obesity, wouldn't go so well, they'd pull out. Now they're saying there's real success coming. So there were a couple of recent Cochrane collaborations that came out discussing the success or lack thereof of public health initiatives around obesity. Do you want to
Starting point is 00:17:38 say a little bit about those and maybe also talk a little bit about the history of why, if I'm going to be blunt, if I'm going to extrapolate from what we've just said, one would say that public health efforts to curb obesity have been a failure and the future of obesity management will be pharmacologic, not public health related. Is that a fair prediction? I think it's a very reasonable prediction. I'm not sure it's one I will share completely. I would share the first part that public health efforts to affect obesity in a meaningful way have thus far been singularly unimpressive. We'll come back maybe a little bit to why and where that's going and where we should go with it. I do think in the present,
Starting point is 00:18:27 and even more so in the not too distant future, clinical management, including surgery and pharmaceuticals ever more, will be ever more powerful, safe, effective, and utilized. I don't think they will ever become the complete solution. And I don't think that there's no solution in public health, but I think we've got to approach it differently. So let's go back in time a little bit. When I started my career as a real professional, basically 1991, I come to New York,
Starting point is 00:18:59 the New York Obesity Research Center at Columbia University and St. Luke's-Rosalte Hospital. It's the only federally or NIH funded obesity research center at the time. It's the first. It's run by Xavier Pisseny, the legacy of Ted Van Italy, across the park. You've got Jules Hirsch, Rudy Liebel and that group at Rockefeller. And it wasn't at the level of public interest that it is now the topic of obesity. What you had is these very interdisciplinary groups, physiologists, geneticists, physicians,
Starting point is 00:19:34 psychologists, statisticians, nutrition scientists, et cetera, all working together on these problems. Many had been working together for decades, very academic but also clinical. You had the powerhouses that were in that region. You had Mickey Stunkard over at UPenn. You had Marcy Greenwood and others at Vassar and so forth. If a young person like me made some foolish statement in a seminar about some aspect of physiology or medicine that showed that given my training, I had no understanding of what the heck I was talking about. One more senior person would put me in my place, but in a very constructive way and explain that I didn't know what I was talking about. And the statisticians would argue with the physiologist and so on. So,
Starting point is 00:20:22 you had a depth of knowledge, real depth of expertise and an understanding. Then NHANES 3 data came out and there was the sense of crisis and panic, public health. That was what, 94? It started to come out in the early 90s, the midpoint of it, 90, 91 was just starting. Tell folks what NHANES is and what the data showed. This is the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. It was at the time only done every few years. First, there was something earlier in the 60s called NEFIS, I think, National Health and something else. Then they developed the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. They did two of them. And then the third one was done,
Starting point is 00:21:05 I think between 88 and 93 maybe. So I think the midpoint data they released around 91, if my memory is right. And people started using the word epidemic and they saw what looked like a jump, whether there's a real jump or not around the late eighties or in the eighties is actually not so clear. If you look at skin folds, you see less of a jump and you see the increase starting earlier. If you look just at BMI and you look at increase, it's been going up for hundreds of years.
Starting point is 00:21:36 The data from the Nobel laureate Robert Fogel, who won it in economics, he's since deceased, but he's a terrific, generous guy. He collected all these old data on British naval cadets from the 18th century and French cadets and civil war soldiers and recaptured slaves during the civil war and looked at these different groups. And you see that obesity levels in BMI have been increasing for centuries, but they clearly did seem to be an acceleration and that caused a panic. And then you had probably the most powerful voice at the time in this domain was Kelly Brownell. Kelly had been a real devotee of Mickey Stunkard. He
Starting point is 00:22:17 was one of Mickey Stunkard's proteges and mentors. And he was a behavioral psychologist, still is a behavioral psychologist, doing behavioral treatment. As a grad student, I'd go to his lectures and learn the mechanics of how to do behavioral treatment, cognitive behavioral therapy for obesity. Pete Slauson Meaning CBT to help people eat less? Dr. John Gerstle Eat less, exercise more, and so on. And then he had a change in the, I guess this would have been the very late 80s, early 90s. He shortly thereafter switched to Yale. He got a MacArthur Prize, the so-called genius award. And he started to look at maybe concerns about the negative effects of obesity. And he
Starting point is 00:22:58 was one of the most powerful, not the first, but one of the most powerful voices to start raising questions about the effects of yo-yo dieting or weight cycling going up and down. Are we doing more harm than good? Are we just building false hopes up for people because obesity treatment is useless? And that started to change and then he morphed into it's the environment. And he introduced, at least to me, the phrase toxic environment. We live in a toxic environment. You can't drive down the street, he would say, without encountering a fast food restaurant. And so this is the problem. We need to stop the individual treatment. He sort of abandoned his roots. We need to go to the public health treatment. Many others were grasping that
Starting point is 00:23:41 idea, inspired often by him, but others on their own, and the public health community rushed in. And this was a community that was up until that point working on smoking or what types of- Smoking, food safety- Let it gasoline. All kinds of things like the sanitation, vaccination, so on. They rushed in and I think there was a lot of sense of this is simple. People eat too much, they don't exercise enough.
Starting point is 00:24:09 Eating less is good. Eating more, quote unquote, healthy food is good. Some foods are considered healthy, some are not. If you eat the healthy food, something magical will happen. More exercise, of course, without any real understanding of this. I've had public health people who said to me, one person wrote and said, well, I think if we got people to not walk with their iPhones, then they would walk a little faster and then they would expend more energy while they're walking across campus and that will help with weight loss. And what do you think? And I thought,
Starting point is 00:24:40 well, we're still going to cover the same distance. There's a nonlinear relationship between walking speed and energy expenditure and the amount of energy is trivial and etc., etc., etc. And I just thought nobody who understands movement science and energetics and kinetics would make such a statement. But if you're a public health person and you just think, I just need clever ways of getting people to behave the way I already know they should behave, then you come up with ideas like that. That's where if you're embedded in a group of people, that doesn't happen. That wouldn't have happened at the New York OBC Research Center. If I had said that in 1991, I would have been immediately educated by senior people who had been thinking about this.
Starting point is 00:25:23 So in other words, the public health field wasn't really able to self-police ideas that were not grounded in science. That's right. One of the things that we're very proud of in our school, School of Public Health in Indiana University, Bloomington, is that we have a kinesiology department as in exercise science. We're only one of four schools in the United States, schools of public health that have a named kinesiology department. Now every school of public health studies physical activity, but studying physical activity and being an expert in exercise science are two completely different things.
Starting point is 00:25:53 We have actual experts in exercise science who understand this, who treat it as a science and take it seriously. We're very proud of that and they do great work. So we got a lot of nonsense rushing in the field. We got a lot of things that never would have in the beginning, you wouldn't have predicted to work, but people tried them because they sounded good. They felt good, gave people a positive feeling, vending machines, farmers markets, walking trails without really saying, all right, let's really work this out. How many people are gonna do it? If they do it, how much effect will it have?
Starting point is 00:26:28 Will they compensate by eating more or less or moving more or less later? Those things weren't done, so we got a whole couple of decades of lousy, uninformative research. But while the public health movement was taking hold, what happened to the guard, the old guard, so to speak, that you referred to having learned the physiology of obesity in the late 80s, early 90s? What was their response to this? Were they a part of the movement? Were they
Starting point is 00:26:58 distinct from the movement? I think it's a mix. As with anything with many people in politics and money and careers and dominance and egos, all kinds of interesting things happen. So some of the real strong behavioral people rushed in, did good science in the sense of things that were rigorous, but maybe not always well conceived that they were likely to be impactful, but there was grant money to be had and people went after it and still do and so be it. So was this also just driven by funding? Was there a change in funding priority? There's a lot more funding for obesity, still not as much as many people including me would like,
Starting point is 00:27:33 not as much as perhaps could have or should have been, but definitely big increases. There was the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation, which didn't put a huge amount of money in, but put money in whether it was intentionally or not, in a very strategic way. Meaning they put in, relative to what NIH or pharma put in, they put in a small amount of money, but it sounded like a big amount of money. It had millions. They made a lot of noise about it very successfully. They got a lot of careers started. They drew a lot of people into the field around public health, around community intervention, around diversity issues, and that's all to the good. It's great. What I think often happened is
Starting point is 00:28:15 the amount of money they were dangling in the, again, relative to NIH was small. And so many people would rush in and get started that way, but then they'd go to NIH and get bigger studies. So it did catalyze a lot of activity and that's good. What we've learned, you can make different arguments about it. The very famous story with Edison where his backers come to him and whether it's true story or not, I don't know, but say, all this time and money and you still have nothing to show for your efforts toward making a light bulb. And he said, no, I now know a thousand ways not to make a light bulb. These two new papers you referred to earlier were from Summer Bell. She was the senior author, not the first author.
Starting point is 00:28:53 They're just out in the last week. I just put up a LinkedIn post on them about three days or so ago. What they do is one is in children, five to 12, I think. The other is an adolescence and They do systematic reviews, very thorough, very objective meta-analyses according to the Cochrane method. What they find is that for both groups, there is no compelling evidence of what you would call a consistent, reliable, long-term clinically or public health meaningful effect on preventing obesity in either children or adolescents. I included a lot of adjectives in there and those are important. Most important
Starting point is 00:29:31 one is probably preventing. I didn't say treating, I said preventing. That doesn't mean that treatment of obesity in children doesn't have any efficacy. The second thing is these are community diet exercise interventions. By the way, how easily do we distinguish between prevention and treatment? Obviously, conceptually it's trivial. Prevention is reducing the number of new cases. Treatment would be reversal of. Can you give a sense of what reversal of obesity rates look like? With public health stuff, I think it's, I don't want to say it's zero because you just always have some spontaneous reversal.
Starting point is 00:30:09 You said a moment ago that this article focused on the prevention side, not the treatment side. Correct. Was the implication of that, that public health treatment has been successful, but prevention has not? No, no, no. I'm sorry. I see.
Starting point is 00:30:24 Okay. but prevention has not? I'm sorry. I see. Clinical treatment, I think there's some evidence for success, more so in adults, but some evidence for success. Does clinical treatment include drugs and surgery? It can include, but it doesn't have to. So, you know, the idea of somebody coming to the clinic, they go to see, let's say a Len Epstein at Buffalo, and they go to his clinic and he puts them in a study
Starting point is 00:30:44 or what have you for weight loss. I would call that a clinical intervention as opposed to Len Epstein saying, I've got an idea, I'm going to go out to the public schools and set these programs up and we'll try to get everybody to be less obese and see if we prevent obesity. That's how I would distinguish those things. And Len, by the way, is one of the sharpest cats around and he's a very good skeptic and a very good commenter on what we really know and don't know in that domain. But what Summerbell shows, basically, is not only is there no compelling evidence for effects,
Starting point is 00:31:16 but there's reasonably compelling evidence that given the methods we've used today, the effects are either zero or trivial. And so I think that's really important because I hear tremendous defensiveness now among people who are not practitioners of pharmaceuticals or favorable toward pharmaceuticals so much because of the great success of some of the drugs, especially the GLP-1 agonist-related drugs. In the same way as in the mid-90s, I heard tremendous defensiveness from the behavioral psychology community and others about genetics because they weren't going to do genetics.
Starting point is 00:31:59 And the Rudy Liebels of the world and the Claude Bouchard of the world were talking it up and it was going to be big and they were starting to feel threatened and often did not know much genetics but would try to somehow minimize the role. I think there is a lot of fear now among people who want community intervention, public health intervention who say, I don't want our solution to obesity be let everybody get obese or let two-thirds of the population get obese and then we'll give them surgery or drugs for the rest of their life. It's too expensive. It's not my ethos, et cetera, et cetera. I think they're worried that people like me who say, look at the efficacy data, we need to think about this more, are implying that we should shut down all
Starting point is 00:32:45 the other stuff. At least for me personally, that answer is absolutely not true. But I think we need sub-paradigm shifts within paradigms. What I mean by that is I don't think the paradigm shift of saying don't ever think about nutrition anymore, only think about drugs and surgery is warranted. I don't think the paradigm shift of saying don't ever think about behavior, community intervention, family intervention is warranted. Don't ever think about public health or policy. That's not worth it. I don't agree with those at all. What I do think we need to do is to say within the paradigms of behavior,
Starting point is 00:33:21 community, family, policy, let's be honest. Let's look at Carol and Summerbell's data and others and say, there is no compelling evidence that any of this has had a meaningful impact. You can cherry pick here and there. You can say this policy led to differences in how much of that food was purchased in this context. Even if that's true, and sometimes those are a little shaky, those conclusions, say, did it lower obesity rates? Those have never been shown. How do we do that? I agree with you by the way. In my own personal, because I think everybody has to have a personal sort of bias if they're
Starting point is 00:34:00 being honest. My personal bias is that so many of these public health ideas on the surface just make a ton of sense. I can simultaneously hold true the following truths, which is on the one hand, I can completely see why it was logical in the early to mid 90s to say we have to change the food environment. Richard Thaler's work, another Nobel laureate, would suggest that that's the answer. You fix the environment. You Thaler's work, another Nobel Laureate, would suggest that that's the answer. You fix the environment. You make the default environment better and people will opt into good choices. By the way, the default environment used to allow people to eat in a way that was clearly ad libitum and obesity rates were not what they were. So something about the environment
Starting point is 00:34:42 200 years ago or 100 years ago or even 50 years ago was significantly different from the environment today. It's not that our genes changed. Nobody would argue there's been such a genetic drift that the reason that obesity rates are two thirds as opposed to 10% is due to a change in our species. An environmental trigger or set of triggers seems more likely and therefore, public health solutions towards those seem very logical. We can hold that truth here and then we have to be brutally honest with your assessment
Starting point is 00:35:14 as well, the same as Caroline's assessment, which is this has been an abject failure. If at the end of the day, you're only measuring the outcome of interest, it hasn't changed. We can say whatever we want, but the outcome of interest, it hasn't changed. We can say whatever we want, but the outcome of interest hasn't changed. Either people smoke less or they smoke more or they smoke the same. That's the only metric that matters if smoking cessation is what you're after. It's not, do we collect more tax revenue? Are the commercials more or less favorable? Do people smoke less in restaurants versus not in restaurants? No, we care if people as a society smoke less or smoke more. So given that, how do we still say, and I'm
Starting point is 00:35:53 not saying I disagree with this because again, my bias is there should be solutions in public health, but how do we know after 30 years and billions of dollars with no effect that we should stay within the paradigm of public health solutions and just abandon all of the ones we have when we don't really have a sense of why they failed. So, we definitely don't want to only rely on public health solutions. I would strongly oppose that. I agree with you that there is a superficial sensibility to the public health arguments that were made for the various things tried. And it was reasonable to try them. But I say superficial sensitivity or sensibility because everything that's true
Starting point is 00:36:37 makes sense as once we understand it, if we're wrong about something, then it didn't make sense. We just didn't understand that. It didn't make sense at the time. Some of that is assumptions and it goes back to that public health thing. I had a wonderful lunch with the most generous, interesting person, Daniel Kahneman, who won the Nobel Prize. He recently passed away. Right. Before he died. He and his wife were gracious enough to allow myself and Michelle Cardell, who now works at WW, was a former student with the
Starting point is 00:37:05 group I led to take them to lunch. We talk about obesity a little bit and he's this great behavioral economist. He says to me without artifice, he says, well, I think this nudge stuff is really good. So you could put things on the menu and that would make people eat less. I say, well, that's a good idea and some things like that are being tried and have been tried. And I said, but the big thing is compensation. Yes, you can get a person to eat a little less in this context, but then if they go home for dinner and they just eat more at dinner, it goes away. And he looks at me without artifice and he says, hold it a second. So you're telling me that there might be mechanisms in people that
Starting point is 00:37:47 lead them to adjust for reduced calories. And I said, yeah. And this was a revelation. He said, you've opened my eyes. And I was an economist, he didn't think about this. He's great with math and he's great with creative study designs, but this was again rushing in. I think that was a big part. A lot of things didn't make sense because they didn't take into account compensation and many other factors. They didn't take into account magnitude of effect and so forth. The second thing is the data themselves. People published a nice thing about a meta-analysis of nudge type stuff in PNAS, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences a couple of years ago. Someone else just went in
Starting point is 00:38:31 and redid it and said, if you adjust for publication bias, it doesn't look like there's much holding up there. So often we're presented with evidence and we may want to come back to this when we talk about some other things like especially protein intake. We're presented with statements as though we confidently know these and yet when you really start to open the hood and peel things back, you say, hey, there's not a lot of air there on the data. So the data that nudge works is actually shaky. So that's the second problem. The third is we seem to be unwilling to learn from the outcomes of our studies. That is unwilling to say we tried the school-based thing and it didn't get a big effect. We tried it again. Fair enough. Let's try the second. Let's
Starting point is 00:39:16 try the third time. At a certain point we say enough. So if someone were to come to me, and I've been saying this for 20 years now, but I'll say it even more strongly today. If someone were to come to me, and I've been saying this for 20 years now, but I'll say it even more strongly today. If someone would come to me and say, we've got this opportunity to invest in these big school-based, community-based, public health-oriented trials to reduce obesity levels in children or adults, and we have the money available, we want to do good, should we do it? And I would say, show me how this proposed idea is radically different than what's been done for the last 30 years. Then let's talk.
Starting point is 00:39:50 If it's not radically different, why are we wasting our time and money on that? I think we really need radically different public health paradigm. We need to stay in the public health paradigm, but within the paradigm, we need a sub-paradigm shift to say, nutrition education, modest physical activity, build a little bit of a facility to allow people a little more activity. These have been tried, they don't work. They don't have big meaningful effects. Let's try something completely different. It's worth a try. That's what I think we need within the paradigms of public health, policy and so on, radically different proposals.
Starting point is 00:40:26 Now, if you were czar of the universe and the ultimate resource allocator, what percentage of resources would you put into a new and different form of public health, i.e. radically different approaches? And what percent would you put into medical treatments for such as surgery and drugs? So first, I find it very entertaining to think about being the czar of anything since my grandparents spent a lot of time successfully escaping the czars. It's interesting that what I would say is probably a little more in the near term on the clinical treatment because I think we can make more rapid gains in that while we need some slower, longer term assessment of the others. But also I would
Starting point is 00:41:14 amp up the non-pharmaceutical, non-clinical, non-surgery a little bit, the funding from the government because I think a lot of that funding for those other things will come from industry. because I think a lot of that funding for those other things will come from industry. So if you look at a budget of a Pfizer or a Lilly or a Novo Nordisk and what they put towards certain areas, then you look at what NIH can put to those areas. We're not talking about NIH being this overwhelming big dog. In fact, when you combine the pharmaceuticals on certain areas, they presumably exceed. Yeah. There's still something here that just philosophically doesn't sit well with me, not morally. I want to be clear. I don't have a moral issue with the remarkable success of the drug class that is now probably
Starting point is 00:42:00 going to be the first thing that bends the arc of this. I don't know when the next check-in will be, the next N. Haines check-in. Now it's annual. Yeah. It seems likely that very soon, if not already, we're going to see for the first time in five decades, obesity rates going down. I hope we do. The reason that I'm still a little troubled is from a public health perspective, we don't have the answer to the question, what was the or what were the environmental triggers? I mean, we think we know the answer, but every time we try an intervention against those things, it doesn't work, which makes us call into question what the answer is. Clearly, we did not get obese because of a GLP-1 shortage that is now being
Starting point is 00:42:39 ameliorated with GLP-1 drugs. Clearly, we have something that was causing the problem, again, multifaceted, likely, and then you have a totally different hack to work around the problem, which is why you're saying, I think what you're saying, which is we need to do both of these things. We still have to get back to this. If you had to speculate, what is it about the world in the early part of the 21st century that makes obesity and by extension type 2 diabetes a problem that it wasn't, again, the year I was born. It's literally a log fold difference in type 2 diabetes, a log fold. That's hard to imagine in 50 years.
Starting point is 00:43:19 I think there's multiple closely related factors. One is the food supply and its availability itself. I think the second is kind of lagged intergenerational effects. Just for fun, I'm going to try to rebut you on the genetics point, but only pedantically. I think that we have seen genetic changes. S1 0530 Epigenetic changes or genetic changes? S1 0530 Both, but certainly I'm gonna put more
Starting point is 00:43:47 my direct knowledge and confidence on the genetic as opposed to the epigenetic changes. And this is assortative mating, differential mating. Do these fully account for the obesity epidemic? No, of course not. Am I trying to say that they are the biggest influences? No, of course not. But I do think it's important
Starting point is 00:44:05 to push back and say these are factors and they come in through migration, through differential fertility, and through assortative mating. We've written papers about all these and as have others. If you look in things like Framingham, you see that people in certain BMI ranges have more children than people in other BMI ranges. And some will say, but obese people have fertility problems. We're not asking about how good you are in theory at producing offspring. We're asking how many offspring you produce. If richer, thinner people use more birth control and have fewer offspring, and there's some genes for thinness, you're going to reduce their prevalence and vice versa.
Starting point is 00:44:44 So through migration, differential fertility, and then the other is assorted mating, which doesn't change allele frequencies, but changes gene frequencies, which you get like meets with like. But if you had to, again, all of those things make sense. I just wanted to- They strike me as somewhat marginal though.
Starting point is 00:45:01 I had to be a professor for a minute and get the pedantic points out. Fair, okay. All right, so pedantic points out. Fair. Okay. All right. So now that's out. I think that it is largely but not exclusively the increased availability of a greater variety
Starting point is 00:45:14 of foods, of highly palatable foods, of foods that are relatively modest in cost, foods that are easy to acquire, the control of ambient temperature, which makes it easier to overeat foods. You don't want to overeat a lot if there's no air conditioning and you live in Austin, Texas and it's 110 degrees out. But if there's air conditioning, the buffet's okay. And then I think there's some intergenerational lagged effects that we, or at least I, don't fully understand.
Starting point is 00:45:43 If you look at the Danish data, Torkel Sorensen and others have written about this. They, for over a hundred years, conscripted, if that's the right word, every 18-year-old healthy male into the Danish army. And they have not only heights and weights of each one naked, kind of weirdly, they have photographs of each of them naked. And what you see in these BMI levels is you'll see a period where it's flat for a little bit, approximately, then you'll see a steep acceleration or steep increase. And then it'll flatten out again a little bit. And then you'll see a steep slope.
Starting point is 00:46:20 This has happened in three or more cycles, I think. I don't think anybody exactly understands why. Diana Thomas' mathematical model, she's a professor at West Point, studies obesity. Her mathematical models predict some of that. I don't fully understand how that works, but we might ask her. It does suggest to me even culturally or behaviorally, there could be some lags whereby the weight of your parents or grandparents is affecting you. Socially or genetically?
Starting point is 00:46:50 Both. So the oocyte that formed you was formed in your grandmother. So potentially through epigenetic things you've mentioned or others, that could be affecting you. Then there's the cultural part. I think about it, when I was a kid and we went out to dinner with my dad, and we weren't poor but we weren't rich, we were decidedly lower middle class, creeping up. If we went out at the local Italian restaurant or something, order shrimp.
Starting point is 00:47:17 You had to ask that about that. The chicken parm you could order without asking. Shrimp you had to ask, because shrimp was expensive. You can get shrimp by the bucket now at the local buffet for next to nothing. So I'm prepared to eat a lot more shrimp than my dad ever would have thought of ordering or sitting down because of our changing economic times and so on. Now my kids think nothing about ordering dinner in from DoorDash every night, or I still think even though I could afford to do it as well as my kids could, because I spend my money some of the time, which is great, I'm glad they're doing it, but I think, oh, that just seems excessive to me.
Starting point is 00:47:57 It seems too indulgent. So I think there may be sort of levels at which one ratchets culturally as well as physiologically or anatomically. So I think all of these things can be in play. I also think we need to change some of the attitudes. This is speculative on my part. I have no proof that this is true. But I think one of the bad things that the nutrition field has done, including very much the public health community, which talks about, I used to realize that the healthy foods that have magical effects, but I also think the low carb advocates and zealots who came up through the late 90s and still exist at present and have very powerful voices,
Starting point is 00:48:36 and yet others still. I think there's the sense that there's a right way to eat. Nobody agrees on what the right way is, but there is an underlying supposition that there is a right way to eat. Nobody agrees on what the right way is, but there is an underlying supposition that there is a right way to eat. And if you just ate that right way, then you would maintain the weight you want to maintain and the fat level you want to maintain without ever feeling lack of satiety or dissatisfaction or what have you. You and I were talking about our personal diets. What's interesting about that is that's actually philosophically not that different from a drug approach. In other words, if you constructed a lot of parallel universes, it's certainly possible that if you put everybody on a perfectly adherent version of diet X, Y, and Z on each of those parallel planets,
Starting point is 00:49:27 you would eradicate obesity. By the way, one of those planets you might say, well, we're also going to put everybody on tersepidide. You now have multiple different dietary treatments when perfectly adhered to that will dramatically improve obesity. One of those will be just a drug, maybe two of them will be a drug, another one will be a gastric bypass, etc. It still doesn't answer the question what triggered the problem. It still doesn't answer the question. I don't know that we want to spend too much more time on that because these are unanswerable questions. What is the right diet to fix it doesn't mean that the absence of that diet is what caused it
Starting point is 00:50:06 I agree The point I was trying to make is that by saying to people there is a right way to eat We may foster a delusion that is the real debate perhaps is not between the low-carb guy and the Non-low-carb guy as to what this thing is or the eat locally or whatever. The real debate may be, is there a right way to eat compositionally or behaviorally or time of day or something that will satisfy you, not make you feel deprived in the real world we live in, not in parallel universe we could construct. And the answer may be no.
Starting point is 00:50:42 And yet by continuing to sell that idea, we may continue to have people searching in the wrong spot instead of searching for how do I control or overcome my incomplete satisfaction with eating only this amount. And instead they're looking for what's the way to eat that I don't have that dissatisfaction. And I think what we may have to accept at some point is that for most of us, there are exceptions, but for most of us to maintain a truly thin or lean body composition, if that's what we want, and I'm not saying everyone should want it, but for those who do want
Starting point is 00:51:20 it, that we may have to accept that either we're going to have to alter our desires in part through pharmaceuticals, or we're going to have to accept that we don't get to meet all our desires at times, as opposed to continue what may be the charade, that there is a way that you can just eat a certain kind of food or certain type of diet or eat in a certain way that will lead you not to ever feel dissatisfied. So I think that's an important stoic approach, right? A little more stoicism. Let's go back to something you were asking though about evidence earlier.
Starting point is 00:51:59 And I do want to make a point about this, that we also need to increase the quality of the evidence and the standards we hold. We spend too much of our research budget on lousy evidence. So in the childhood obesity field, my group, for example, often will write letters to the editor and another paper was retracted last week because we found statistical errors in it. And if you think about it, this was a randomized controlled trial of a treatment for obesity-related or nutrition-
Starting point is 00:52:29 Behavioral? I think so. We find it in diet behavior, et cetera, not usually drugs. What we see is if you think about a randomized controlled trial, at the low end, a randomized controlled trial is usually over $100,000 to conduct. At the high end, it's tens of millions. The ones we're looking at that often we find these mistakes in, and many cases are attracted, especially in childhood obesity, are probably in the multiple hundreds of thousands of dollars, occasionally millions. And then you think that's all wasted if they misanalyzed and misreported the data and got
Starting point is 00:53:03 the wrong answer. So we kind of feel like we're rescuingreported the data and got the wrong answer. So we kind of feel like we're rescuing those dollars in some sense by getting the wrong answers out and the right answers in. So we think it's an important service. But I think we need to hold our field's feet to the fire much more strongly on doing research that answers new questions, that answers questions well, that honestly reports the data. Do you think that that problem, which I'm quite aware of, of course, is disproportionately present in this field or do you feel that it's both acknowledged and demonstrated at the same frequency in all fields of medicine? Do you feel that we have a brighter spotlight on it here
Starting point is 00:53:43 thanks to certain individuals? How do you think this stacks up? All of the above. What we know is that there are many anecdotal statements by leading thinkers like Stuart Ritchie as just one example, Gary Taubes, our mutual friend who say nutrition is singularly bad. There are some of these all wonderfully colorful statements. Johnny Anides said, we need to accept that nutrition epidemiology is a dead science and bury the corpse. That's a quotation. Those are opinions. Those are not bits of data. If we go further and we look at the Pew Charitable Trusts – But I mean one could agree with that on some pretty objective facts. But the question I'm
Starting point is 00:54:24 asking is more on the challenges of experimental research that you're talking about where real dollars are being thrown at experiments that are being done incorrectly or being analyzed incorrectly or where the questions that are being asked are incremental, useless, uninteresting and unlikely to add meaningfully to the fund of knowledge. Let's just forget about nutritional epidemiology, but I want to talk about this other, what seems to be more distressing problem based on both the dollars that go into it, but also I think the confusion that it sows and the noise that it creates. Right. It's clearly created that confusion noise and that's what the
Starting point is 00:55:01 Pew Charitable Trusts have shown that in surveys, now we're talking data, in surveys of representative samples of American population, people trust nutrition experts, clinicians, purveyors of knowledge more than they trust nutrition scientists and they trust nutrition science less than they trust other forms of science. So that's a fact. We do have a trust problem in nutrition science. Now let's go to the last stage, which is our research really better or worse. Harder to pin that one down. There's not enough concrete, strong comparisons to other areas. We're trying to start some in our group, but there was a recent paper that came out in economics. It's not a one-to-one comparison, but there was a recent paper that came out in economics
Starting point is 00:55:45 It's not a one-to-one comparison, but in an economic journal looking at reproducibility Reproducibility and replicability are not quite the same reproducibility is can I get your original data? Run exactly the same analysis you said you ran and get exactly the same result if If I can, I've reproduced your research. Doesn't mean your result was right. Maybe you ran the wrong analysis, but at least I could do what you said you did. We do that in nutrition and obesity and we find we don't have exact numbers. It's not a random sample of papers, but we find what seems to be a not infrequent errors, irreproducibility, or what we call verification problem. Meaning we could reproduce your result, but it was wrong.
Starting point is 00:56:28 It was wrong because you ran the wrong analysis. We write the right analysis, get a different conclusion. And you might do how many of these a year? More than a dozen. Of the dozen you run a year, how many turn out to be not reproducible or not verifiable? I would say probably, again, these are all approximations, maybe half, but keep in mind, we're not randomly sampled. Understood. What's the criteria upon which you select besides size of study?
Starting point is 00:56:54 It's usually one of two things. It's interestingness or it's something doesn't look quite right. So if it's something doesn't look quite right. Your pre-test probability is higher. Then we take a closer look at it more often. Or if it's something doesn't look quite right, then we take a closer look at it more often. Or if it's just very interesting, we say, that's really interesting and it was published in Nature and that could be paradigm change. I'd be curious when you have enough data to know if you take out the, that looks fishy sample and just said, hey, when we looked at the, this is interesting, if half of those are coming up
Starting point is 00:57:23 unverifiable, that's a crisis. Yeah. I would say we should do it. I hope there's a funder out there listening who will want to fund it. NIH, as you might imagine, may not be always so keen on having us answer this question and it's hard to get that through. But yeah, we'd like to do that and hope we can do some more. We're doing little spot checks in the area. My sense is even within obesity, if you look at pharmaceutical done randomized controlled trials, and I'm not trying to say that people at pharmaceutical companies or pharmaceutical companies somehow morally superior or not, they're people, they're just responding to their environment as well. But their environment is a very strong regulatory authority called FDA that holds their
Starting point is 00:58:05 feet to the fire and so on. Do you think that's the reason that drug studies tend to be very rarely found to require retraction? In modern times, yes. In modern times, I mean, somebody said to me, do you trust randomized controlled trials coming out of the pharmaceutical industry more or less than academia, infinitely more from the pharmaceutical industry? Yeah. Think about that for a moment. Let's just reflect. That's a big statement.
Starting point is 00:58:33 I agree with you, but I think it's not intuitive to the average person listening to us. Many people listening to us would say, what? The data coming out of Pfizer are more trustworthy than the data coming out of Harvard. But the point here, the key point is that Pfizer has to answer to somebody, the FDA, who will bring down a much greater and swifter punishment if issues are discovered in methodology, statistical analysis, reporting, etc. Whereas the academic community doesn't have that degree of policing basically. And the funding. Often people say, oh, the industry is so much more efficient than academia because they have the profit motive.
Starting point is 00:59:16 I think it depends what you put in your denominator of efficiency. If you say output per unit time, no question, industry in general and pharma in particular blow academia away. But if you say output per unit dollar, academia probably blows industry away because we know how to stretch every penny. Right. You guys are working on a shoestring budget. Exactly. But that means often not much rigor. Whereas the big pharma company who's going to put their registration trial in is checking and double checking and having professionals check and so on. Now, there may be more – I hate to use the word bias because it's not clear. By the way, it might be worth also explaining to
Starting point is 00:59:56 folks that when people talk about quote unquote a Pfizer study or a J&J study, they're hiring CROs to actually do the study. I think sometimes people are under the impression that when Lilly is doing a study on a drug, it's like the whole Lilly team doing the experiment as opposed to Lilly providing the agent, helping think about the experiment, but basically having a clinical research organization actually do it and having independent folks do the analysis. That's right. I think that's important. Now, that doesn't mean that there's no – and again, I was about to use the word bias. I don't have a better word right now to use,
Starting point is 01:00:33 but I use that one hesitantly. But there may be more bias in some ways in the industry funded work and that's often in the question asked. I was just about to say it's how the question is asked, which determines how the study is designed to look for a particular answer for sure. Exactly. So an industry group might say, I'll compare my new drug to the worst old drug in class. If a university guy did it, he or she might say, no, I'm going to compare it to the best drug. Exactly. But once they've decided on the question, then the design, execution, and reporting of the study seems to be enormously more rigorous in pharma. Now, that's not true if you said, what about dietary supplement industry? Different game.
Starting point is 01:01:19 We've got a more complex answer there. So back to this, I think we need better data. answer there. So back to this, I think we need better data. I think we need to assess this. I think within the non-industry funded stuff, typically like the public health, the school-based stuff, the child obesity trials, it's going to vary a lot. So the cluster randomized community school-based childhood obesity trials tend to be quite poor. And I think the non-verifiability rate is very high. Whereas if you went to certain other kinds of trials, the NIH funded clinical management of obesity trials will tend to be better. So it's going to vary a lot. And just hopefully I'm not biased, but I'm sure some of them will think I am and that's okay, they're entitled to their opinions on this. I'll disclose that I have funding from all these groups. So I've got most of my funding
Starting point is 01:02:07 is government and NIH, but I have funding and the school I lead is funding from industry, including many of the pharmaceutical companies to think about clinical trials design and biostatistics. We're funding from food industry at times, commodity groups. So I just wanted to disclose all that. Let's pivot now and kind of talk about the current state of obesity, which is really seeing a success it's never seen. And it's been a relatively short period of time. I think three years ago, very few people knew what semaglutide was or even Ozempic, which
Starting point is 01:02:43 is the trade name given to the diabetes version of that drug. Whereas today, I can't imagine too many people haven't heard the words Ozempic or some of its derivatives. I think Ozempic might be one of the most recognized of these drugs. It's pretty remarkable. It's also worth noting that these are not new drugs. Semiglutide and trisepatide are newer drugs, but they've been around for a while, at least semiglutide has. Liraglutide. Liraglutide and others have been around for at least a decade. They've successfully treated people with type 2 diabetes and like all things, or it's often the case, you notice
Starting point is 01:03:26 something in treating one subset of patients that gives you an insight into treating another. So basically, as people with type 2 diabetes were treated with this class of drug, you notice that it wasn't just improving their diabetes, they were also losing weight. That led to what became a set of dedicated experiments to test the efficacy of these drugs in non-diabetic obese patients and the rest is history. Talk a little bit about what you think is socially and psychologically happening at the moment. Why? Why are people so interested in this drug? Dr. Scott Gottlieb It's fascinating. I think people are interested for the obvious reason.
Starting point is 01:04:05 The obvious reason is lots of people want to lose weight and lots of people want to help other people lose weight. For the first time in history, as you've noted, we have drugs that are now powerfully effective and appear to be reasonably safe. We've had drugs that were powerfully effective before but would kill you. We've had drugs that were powerfully effective before, but would kill you. And we've had drugs that were reasonably safe before, but at best, modestly efficacious. We now have ones that are powerfully effective and appear safe, reasonably safe. Safety is a social judgment, not a factual determination. Risk is a factual determination.
Starting point is 01:04:42 Safety is a social judgment. And so it invites all kinds of interesting speculations about cause. What is the role of GLP-1 in causing obesity? Is there a role? Just because things involving GLP-1 treat it doesn't mean it's involving the cause. What's the effect on stigma? If we can treat it, does that reduce stigma? In the same way that Viagra changed many things around erectile dysfunction. Interestingly, I didn't predict the full cultural impact of that, which shows you it's hard to predict these things. People didn't predict what Viagra was for. It was being used for something else. They noticed erections as a side effect,
Starting point is 01:05:23 and then they started working on it. In the early 90s when I went and visited one of my buddies who's a biostatistician at Pfizer and that individual told me they were working on this new thing and explained what it was to me. I laughed at it and I said, why are you wasting your time on something so ridiculous and unimportant? Why don't you do some important research? Shows you what I know. So I think here we're learning that again, that we get surprised in science. We're seeing a moral panic. This is subjective on my part, but this is something I'm noticing. A lot of old arguments that had kind of gone semi-dormant, at least in the academic community over the
Starting point is 01:06:02 years of, well, if you give people a drug for obesity, it doesn't teach them anything. And therefore, when you stop the drug, the weight just comes back. And this was said as a criticism, as opposed to saying, well, who said it had to teach them anything? Who said that was the goal? And for many drugs, anti-seizure medications, if you have seizures, antihypertensives, anti-diabetes drugs, etc. You're going to take those for the rest of your life if you're in the right class for
Starting point is 01:06:30 that. We don't say, but the person with schizophrenia shouldn't get the drug because if we stop giving it to them, the schizophrenia symptoms come back. Say, no, schizophrenia is a serious disease. We need to give it to them. With obesity, this has come up again. It seemed to be put down a few, that idea a few years back. And now I'm seeing, I'm hearing it again, this kind of moralistic judgment about that. We're also hearing the moralistic judgments
Starting point is 01:06:57 come about motivation. It's okay if you're motivated for health. It's not okay to get the drug if you're motivated for something other than health not okay to get the drug if you're motivated for something other than health, which implies that assuming we have the same health issues, assuming the person would equally benefit from their health, we make a moral judgment about your motivation. But there is no evidence that I know of that people who are motivated for health to lose weight do better than people who are motivated for cosmetic or any ego business, any other reasons. So I think we need to get over some of that moral panic. Once we get past the safety, the cost, and the availability issues, and I don't want to trivialize those, the safety,
Starting point is 01:07:36 the cost, and the availability issues are big issues. The safety issue is really, and in that sense I'm defining safety in the sense that sometimes the FDA defines it, which is safety involves risk and risk involves uncertainty as opposed to being risk involving known factor. I don't just mean the probability that you get this. I mean the fact that we don't know what happens if you take it for 40 years. So there is some safety issue, some open questions. No one's taken it for 40 years,
Starting point is 01:08:05 so we don't know what happens if you take it for 40 years. Right now, it's very expensive. Our country is divided on how healthcare should be paid for. There's a lot of different opinions. And also there's an availability problem. But let's just fast forward to a time when we say, we've learned the safety. By the way, say a little bit more about the availability problem. I mean, I only realize it because you see compounding pharmacies now making semaglutide and terzepotide, which when I first saw that, I couldn't understand how they were doing that legally because that's pretty clearly not within the statute of what a compounding pharmacy can do. A compounding pharmacy can't make an existing FDA approved drug. They have to make
Starting point is 01:08:45 a variation of that drug. For example, they have to change the delivery mechanism if they make something topical that would only be available orally or something of that nature. Unless, and one of the exceptions to the rule is if the FDA approved drug can't be produced in sufficient quantities, then a compounding pharmacy can create the exact same drug that is available through the FDA label. Presumably that is happening. Do we have a sense of why it's happening? What is the manufacturing bottleneck? Obviously, demand is outstripping supply, but the question is why is supply not able to meet demand? Then secondly, do you have any insight into whether the quality control at the compounding pharmacy level matches that of Lil I or Novonortis?
Starting point is 01:09:34 With respect to the first part, why is there an availability problem? I don't know the technical mechanics of it, but my understanding is that the technical process by which these drugs are produced is different than some other drugs and the technical process is a slow one. And so until they ramp up more and more production sites, they just can't do it fast enough. But they are ramping up more and more production sites. That's good. Novo just bought Catalon, which happens to have a plant in my backyard in Bloomington, Indiana. So, we'll probably see more of that ramping up. The second thing is about the compounding pharmacies. When I first heard about it,
Starting point is 01:10:16 I'm far from an expert in compounding pharmacies or the legal aspects, but I too was skeptical, is this okay? Was the quality control? Was this's the quality control and it's just a kind of shady thing. And I started to hear a lot of reports about this described as though it was a very shady endeavor. And again, that moralizing came in again. Then I've talked to some other people who are experts in it and who are using these. And I've said, now admittedly, again, this is their business. So they have a motivation, And I've said, admittedly, again, this is their business, so they have a motivation. But they have said, well, when we do it,
Starting point is 01:10:47 and they've described, and I said, tell me your process. Who do you use? How do you do it? What quality control? And then they've gone through, say for this compounding pharmacy that I use, we use it in this way, this degree of quality control. And I say, wow, that sounds to me,
Starting point is 01:11:03 I have not physically inspected the plants, I'm not an expert, it sounds to me like some very rigorous quality control. So I don't think we should be dismissive of the concerns around compounding pharmacies, but I also don't think we want to paint everybody with the same brush. The question becomes as with anything is show me your data, show me your evidence on your quality control, your procedures, and if they're good, they're good, then let's use them. Let's get over the moral panic. I don't know that I think of that as a moral panic. I think the bigger moral panic is less about the source of the drug, but the use of the drug. And so you brought up an interesting distinction, which is let's take an individual who is medically obese, and by the way, metabolically
Starting point is 01:11:45 unhealthy. That's the key point I want to get out here. This is a person whose health is compromised by their weight, both from an orthopedic perspective and metabolically. Then let's take another individual who's overweight, but if you're looking at them objectively, you don't see the metabolic signs of overweight. They're not suffering physical and orthopedic issues associated with it. Both of these people, let's just assume, have a desire to lose weight.
Starting point is 01:12:11 One of them to primarily ameliorate the medical conditions and also the aesthetic conditions. Then the latter person just for the aesthetic conversions. Okay. We probably look at those people differently. When I say we, I mean society might make a different moral judgment on those two. That's right. You're arguing that's a false dichotomy. It's a legitimate dichotomy to see the situations as distinct situations, but not necessarily implying distinct recommendations coming from those.
Starting point is 01:12:45 Let's refine it to a two by two. We've got people, let's just say four individuals come to you and we're going to say that you're the objective, all-knowing agent. Meaning I determine who goes on the drug. No, you determine their state of being. Half of the people are objectively at medical physical risk because of obesity and would be objectively medically helped by losing weight on this drug. Half of the people are not at objectively medical increased risk and would not be predicted
Starting point is 01:13:19 to have a medical, a major medical benefit. Within each of those groups, half of them think they have a medical problem, regardless of whether you objectively determine they do, and think they would benefit. And half of them aren't interested in that. They're want to do it for cosmetics, income, other opportunities, et cetera, stigma reduction, quality of life. The question is how should those four groups be treated? Now it seems to me from an obvious point of view, if we're concerned about expense and the expense is borne by society, not the individual coming, or if there's shortages and we're going to take it away from someone who's genuinely medically needed, then going to the non-medically
Starting point is 01:14:03 needy people is questionable. But if we get over those problems, if the person says, I can afford to pay it for myself, and the availability is there, and we think there's no big safety problem, or even if there's some safety problem, but we've told them, fully consented, take the libertarian view, it's their choice,
Starting point is 01:14:24 it seems to me. It's hard to imagine any reasonable person could argue with that position. Well, one of the big statements that got in some news was a very reputable entity, major player in mainstream medicine who has an interest in actually promoting this. A three sort of step statement was made. Step one is the drugs were intended and designed and studied for this use, meaning treatment of medically needy people. Second, the drugs were proved for that use. Third, therefore they should only be used for that. And the third part is a moral judgment,
Starting point is 01:15:00 not a factual judgment. The first and the second are true and what they really tell you is therefore the cost benefit analysis has to be viewed through the lens of that patient population. In other words, when you ask the question about risk and benefit, you have to at least acknowledge that the long-term risk, long-term benefit are studied in that population. And as such, this is what the data are. These are the risks, these are the benefits, make your judgment. Conversely, if you ask the question, hey, for a person who is subjectively 10 pounds overweight,
Starting point is 01:15:43 like me, you could argue I'm 10 pounds overweight. Nobody knows but me basically, but hey, should I be taking this drug? So let's take an analogy. Patient comes to you. They're very wealthy. They're in good physical health. They have a house, they have a car, they have all the material things they need. They have a family.
Starting point is 01:16:04 Family loves them. They don't engage in violence. And they'd say, I feel miserable. I'm anxious all the time or I'm depressed all the time. You might try a few things, explore it, but assuming you've explored it, it's real. Maybe you tried some cognitive behavioral therapy, didn't seem to work. You might say, yeah, an anti-anxiety drug or an antidepressant might be for you. FDA approves those things. We take the person's quality of life and their feelings
Starting point is 01:16:32 into account. Why is it that the person who says, I feel too fat and I want to be 10 pounds thinner and look good in my bathing suit or I want to get this job as the leading actor in that film or I want a promotion in my environment and I think I'm more likely to get it if I'm thinner or I'm hungry all the time and I don't plan to lose weight, I just want to stop being hungry all the time. Why are that person's feelings or non-medical desires any less valid than the person with depression or for that matter, the person with an unusual but not health damaging physical feature, an unusual nose or something who says, I just feel like I'd be judged better.
Starting point is 01:17:20 I don't think it is. I guess the only thing I would suggest as the backstop to that is, when the person who doesn't like their nose goes to the ENT surgeon or the plastic surgeon to have the completely non-essential but emotionally beneficial procedure, if they're seeing a good surgeon, the surgeon can tell them with unambiguous clarity, what the probability of negative outcomes is. And I think the same is true in the case you described at the outset about the individual with depression or anxiety. A very good physician can explain to them what the risks are.
Starting point is 01:18:04 And by the way, as you know well, very few physicians would give you a medication for anxiety or depression without also prescribing in parallel to it, psychotherapy. The data are pretty clear that medication by itself is nowhere near as effective as medication coupled with psychotherapy. You have two things going for you that make this analogy not apples to apples, which is in the case of depression, we can say much more about the long-term side effects, and we're combining it with a behavioral therapy that aims to improve the efficacy. Again, I'm not suggesting that the person who wants to lose 10 pounds
Starting point is 01:18:46 doesn't have a legitimate concern. I think my concern is we don't know enough about the long-term risk to tell them for their relatively minor health compromise, is it potentially worth it? Is the trade-off worth it? I think we could probably say that with a higher degree of certainty for the individual with significant obesity because even if we would have kind of a small bracket of understanding the downside potentially of the drug, we really know the downside of having a BMI of 40. Being insulin resistant, having type 2 diabetes, having a BMI of 40 has such a clear downside that the other side of that bed is a pretty easy one to take. I think that to me, for me it's not a moral question at all when I'm confronted with this question, which I am all the
Starting point is 01:19:38 time. Every week I probably, or every two weeks at least, interact with a patient who fits the exact description you're talking about, which is I'd love for this to be easier. Again, I don't think there's anything wrong with wanting something to be easier. My hope is we get to a point where we could give them the same degree of clarity around risk that the plastic surgeon can give the patient who wants to undergo a rhinoplasty. Right. I agree with with you on that. And I think the moral questions come in around how do you conceive of the role of FDA, society, physicians in regulating choices. And by the way, to be clear, that's why I'm not taking one of these drugs. I'd love to be 10 pounds lighter. I would love to be 10 pounds lighter. I would love to never be hungry. All of the things that these drugs do, by the way, they improve glycemic control.
Starting point is 01:20:29 All of those things are appealing to me. But the truth of it is for somebody who is quite a risk taker, and I am quite a risk taker. You are. I am. When it comes to my health, I would argue I'm quite a risk taker. But I've watched countless patients take these drugs and as I've shared with you and others, without exception, the resting heart rate overnight goes up about 10 beats per minute. And I don't know what it is about that fact and the fact that heart rate variability goes down slightly. That just has me asking the question for me personally, is it worth the trade off? Is there some underlying
Starting point is 01:21:05 sympathetic parasympathetic imbalance that results from this drug that is doing a whole bunch of other good things vis-a-vis my appetite potentially? But you know what? Over the arc of my life, is it worth it? Maybe if it were 40 pounds and it was medically a problem, I'd say, oh, I'll take the heart rate bump any day of the week. So informationally, I'm with you 100%. And in terms of the morality of the honest communication, I'm with you 100%. Well, by that I mean, informationally, we have a fair bit of data that allowed FDA
Starting point is 01:21:36 to make its decisions on the use of these drugs for particular indications in patients who are judged to be quote unquote, medically needy of those drugs. And we don't have a lot of data on the person who's thin but who says I just want it to be easier or the person who's thin but says I'd like to be 10 pounds thinner. And I think any treatment or provision of something to people without a full disclosure of what you know and an honest disclosure is not right. So I think if I were in your shoes,
Starting point is 01:22:05 I'm not a physician, I don't prescribe drugs, but if I were in your shoes and that person came to me, my bare minimum is that I've got to say to them, I want you to be aware that I have no data on this over many decades. We only have a few years. I want you to be aware that it was only tested thoroughly in these populations, which is not your population. And you need to know that there are, as Rumsfeld famous said, the unknown unknowns. Then I think there's an issue of choice. There are lots of things that I think it's acceptable
Starting point is 01:22:37 that our society permits, but I don't personally wanna do them. Think freedom of speech. I think it's perfectly acceptable and necessary that we allow certain people to come out publicly and make certain statements. But I'm not sure I want to make all those statements. And I can imagine you saying, I think it may be acceptable that somebody provides this drug to this person under these circumstances, but that's not what I want my career or life to be. And I think you should have that choice. So I think these are things we ought to do and it comes down very much I think to this sense of after we have the inputs, we can agree on
Starting point is 01:23:10 the facts or we should be able to agree on the facts, then what we do with those facts, we can disagree because we have different values and I think that's where it's how much of a paternalist is one, the FDA is very paternalistic. They're going to decide which drugs are good for whom or how much are you a libertarian where you say, we'll tell you about the effects to the extent we can of this drug or this treatment, but how good it is, whether you should do it, whether you want to do it implies values and you make that decision as long as it's a fully informed decision and those are different views of how we should proceed. All right. Let's consider one more zinger on
Starting point is 01:23:51 this topic. You are now in charge of both WADA and USADA. So World Anti-Doping and US Anti-Doping agencies. Thank you for defining those for me. You have an obvious and clear hard line against drugs that improve performance. An athlete cannot take testosterone or growth hormone or EPO or anything that boosts performance. Now if you think about it, a lot of sports have their performance improved when the athlete is lighter. Weight management is a big part of many sports. Cyclists, runners, gymnasts, if you think about it,
Starting point is 01:24:31 rowers, any sport that is cardiac output versus body weight, those athletes, and I used to be one of them, you are just as focused on weight management as you are cardiac output. Should these drugs be banned by WADA and USADA? Are they indeed performance enhancing drugs? Dr. Craig Lovell Great question. I hadn't thought about that until you asked it. Great question. Because it introduces a whole different set of interests. Prior, we were talking mainly about the individual persons taking the drugs interest and a little
Starting point is 01:25:07 bit about the provider's interest, you, a little bit about society, costs, FDA, so on. Here you've introduced a fourth party and that party is the sport. All the spectators, the people who own it, the other participants, the sport has rules. Sport is very different than some other things where there's an arbitrariness to it. Why does the baseball bat have to be this long and not that long? Why does the tennis racket have to be within these dimensions? Well, that part's arbitrary, but what's not arbitrary is we want it to be equal. We want everybody to have the same chance. So in other words, we don't
Starting point is 01:25:45 spend too much time worrying about the length or weight of the baseball bat. We worry far more that you didn't screw into yours and put cork in there and change the weight of it. That's the thing we care about is fairness. Because that's the rule. But the rule, we even change the rules about the intrinsic things. So we change the rules about, in some places we don't condition on age, in others we have age brackets. Some boxing we have weight brackets, wrestling we have weight brackets.
Starting point is 01:26:13 We don't have height brackets in basketball. Some colleagues and I try to write a whole paper on, mathematically what is bias? What do we mean by that? And we use basketball as an analogy and I use myself as the example and say, if I try out and I don't do well for the basketball team because I'm short, I don't call that bias because intrinsic to the idea of basketball is these are the rules. We don't have springboards
Starting point is 01:26:35 for shorter guys. We could, but we don't. We don't have height classes. And so that's not biased. In contrast, if you ask me to try out to be a biostatistics professor and the book is on the top shelf that you want me to lecture from, and there's no step stool, I would argue that's biased because you could have put a step stool there,
Starting point is 01:26:55 and it's not intrinsic to biostatistics professor performance to be able to reach tall things. And so we need to look at the sport and say, what do you want it to be? And if somebody says, I want it to be things where part of the sport is being able to maintain your weight. And so I don't want anybody to have a performance enhancing drug. And to me, so be it.
Starting point is 01:27:17 I could also alternatively turn around and say, we just want you to be able to get the basket in the hoop or we just want you to be able to row the boat. And if you do it by having more money and hiring a better coach and you do it by taking Ozempic and you do it by having good genes, all is fair. I don't think there's a right answer there from the sport point of view. I think- But given that the sport has already made several decisions, they've already said, you can't take a drug that increases the number of red blood cells that you have. That's EPO. You can't take a drug that increases the rate at which your
Starting point is 01:27:55 muscles repair themselves after hard training. That would be testosterone. Go on and on and on. You can't take a drug like a diuretic that takes body weight away from you. This is not a philosophical question about drugs. It's a practical question about this class of drugs whose efficacy is, as you said, profound and its safety, at least in the short term, unquestionable. Are we going to basically see at the Olympics this year in France, if they were drug testing for it, what fraction of athletes would be taking GLP-1 agonists of the sports where body weight regulation is a key? I don't expect many shot putters to be taking it. Fascinating. No.
Starting point is 01:28:39 But I do wonder how many boxers and rowers and runners and cyclists will be taking it. Really interesting. We should do that study. Let's work on it. So I don't know the answer. Haven't heard about that before. I think your speculation is apt. I think that as a formalist, I would go and say, well, what is these groups that have said you can't take testosterone and this and this and that? They probably put out some underlying principles. It probably said, you cannot take a drug that enhances performance unless you have a medical need. I don't know if they've said that, but if they have, then it could get really tricky because now you say, well, who defines the
Starting point is 01:29:17 medical need? That's right. What about now is it fair if we take the person who's just below the threshold for needing it, who says, I don't get to take the drug, but the person who's just below the threshold for needing it, who says, I don't get to take the drug, but the person who's just above the threshold who does, you have then also this idea of a fairness of disabilities issue. If I have obesity, particularly I've got a strong genetic predisposition to it, I can't manage to be not obese without the drug. Do I effectively have a disability? Is this now prejudicial or violation of the Americans with Disabilities Act or something like that or different countries have different variants? Is there a fairness issue? Again, I don't know that there's a right answer.
Starting point is 01:29:57 I think these would be tough political and moral questions, but it's really particularly tough because you bring in the interest of the sport. And then you're going to get also, it's going to reflect back when you get into the health interest of the individual, just as with many sports, we might say it's in the interest of the team or the coach or the sport itself to have this person at greater risk. But of course it's not in the interest of their situation. And yet we somehow accept that we allow people to play football even though there's concussion risk and we allow people to box and many other things. But are there some limits where we might
Starting point is 01:30:37 say, we're not comfortable with you're putting yourself at risk for this. We need to protect you as much as the sport. All right. Let's pivot to something a little bit easier to talk about. You've already alluded to protein. It's a huge interest of yours clinically, personally. It's a topic I've addressed a number of times. What do we know about protein and what do we, at least in your view, what do you think we think we know that we don't know? So you're as much or more of an expert on the physiology and biochemistry of it, but I will venture a few things.
Starting point is 01:31:10 So with respect to what we know, we know some very basic things. We know you can't live without protein, without consuming some protein. We know that the body is made not totally, but heavily of proteins. They're essential for functioning. We know that proteins are made up of amino acids. They're different amino acids that have different effects. Some amino acids can be synthesized in the body. Some can't be.
Starting point is 01:31:35 I think you did a podcast with Luke Van Loon recently, which I found enormously educational. And so I'd refer people to that one. And he knows a lot more about protein than I do. So I think we know that we need protein, we need a certain amount, we need certain amino acids and we can get them from various foods or combinations. Animal-based foods, we can pretty much get all the proteins we need from them. If we only eat plant-based foods, it's not impossible with the exception of maybe taurine but whether we have to consume taurine or not, if you're a cat,
Starting point is 01:32:08 you have to consume taurine, but if you're a human, maybe not. You could drink Red Bull and still eat plant-based protein and you're fine. So then we get into, are there known things about the amino acids in terms of long-term human health? I think modest. So we see certain things about leucine being important for skeletal muscle growth, anabolic effects. We see some things about isoleucine in mice maybe not producing longevity. We see taurine supplementation
Starting point is 01:32:41 in mice and some other species appearing to prolong life in Vijaya Dev's work. We see methionine restriction in Rich Miller's work prolonging life, thionine is related to torrents. A lot of confusion. What will really prolong life in humans is unclear. Whether the same things that will prolong or shorten life in terms of macronutrient composition in mice will do the same thing humans is unclear. And there are different outcomes. This is again part of why I railed against the idea of healthy food so-called or unhealthy food so-called. Healthy for what? You might want to be 10 pounds thinner. I might want to be able to lift 10 more pounds on the bench press.
Starting point is 01:33:23 That person wants to live 10 years longer. The three diets for those things may be different. So I think that after that, after the idea that we need some protein, we need some minimal amount, we need amino acids, I think it gets shaky then. In other words, minimums and maximums were not necessarily a part of what you just described with much certainty. Right. I think there's reasonable confidence and you've been a great progenitor of this idea, that the old school recommendations for this much is enough, were probably too low. 0.8 grams per kilogram body weight is the RDA. Right. I think many people think that's too low that you can survive on.
Starting point is 01:34:06 It's not that you can't survive, but can you thrive? And that's sort of, I think, a big point of your book and other people's lives, your book Outlive, which is, yes, we can think about treating diseases. We can think about preventing diseases, but neither of those are equivalent to optimizing our lives and our health. And different people have different ideas of optimal. Is optimal optimal comfort? Is it optimal length? Is it optimal ability? Whatever it is, it doesn't seem that that's the level the LRDA 0.8 grams per kilogram is the optimal level for health or longevity or anything else. It's probably somewhat higher.
Starting point is 01:34:46 Next question is, are there minimal thresholds in any sitting? So Don Lehman and others have argued there are. Sometimes you hear 20 said, 20 grams, sometimes you have 30 said. So 20 to 30 grams in a particular sitting is the minimum to get anabolic. Is that true? When I as a statistician hear this, I'm like, really? A threshold in biology? You're telling me there's a step function and you know it? Now, I don't really believe it's a step function, but maybe it's sigmoidal. Maybe it's sort of a little flat and then it goes up steeply and then it flattens out a little bit. And then I say, how much sample size and how many different doses would you need to really get a fix on that and test whether it's there?
Starting point is 01:35:31 And then you look at the studies done and you go, you've got to be kidding me. We talked earlier about pharmaceutical company studies. Think about the numbers of people on which we tested COVID-19 vaccines. Think about the numbers of people we've tested statins and now GLP-1 agonists. Now think about the numbers of people used in randomized controlled trials from the nutrition community to look at protein needs. Not all of us need to take a statin. A lot of us do. Not all of us need to take GLP-1 agonists, we've talked about that. All of us need to eat protein. And yet the quality of evidence and the quantity
Starting point is 01:36:11 of evidence we have is tiny, it's dust compared to what we have on these pharmaceuticals. And so we really need to ramp this up. I would say, I don't think that we really know that you don't get anabolic until you hit 20 or 30. But again, just to be clear, I mean the mechanisms that are described on those are based on small studies and they're really small by necessity just based on funding and complexity of doing these studies. I mean these are amino acid labeled tracer studies where they give people various doses of protein and they look at muscle protein synthesis. I'm not here to say that we shouldn't be doing bigger, better studies, but some of the
Starting point is 01:36:52 studies that have been aimed at elucidating this are quite rigorous in terms of their mechanistic insights. And so I guess the question is, isn't it at least biologically plausible that there is a threshold? I agree, it's very unlikely a step function. It's more likely a sigmoidal shaped curve. It seems at least biologically plausible, which doesn't make it right, that at low doses, at 10 grams of amino acids, the liver itself might just prioritize gluconeogenesis. There's a saturation point at which it says, oh, okay, we have excess nitrogen now, let's go off and do this other thing.
Starting point is 01:37:31 I think it's entirely plausible, but we talked many times. Yeah, lots of plausible things turned out to be wrong. Exactly. So we need to do the studies. I'm not putting anybody down for these studies. No, no, I got it. Yeah. Some of them, when I hear about what Don Lehman's done and what Luke Van Loon has done and others is really impressive and rigorous. But as a statistician who's saying, do we know the answer? I say, not really. We're also interested in long-term effects. And so there's that old saying, there's many a slip, twix, cup and lip. What's the saying? There is many a slip, twix between cut and lip. Drink and you spill. You think it's a done deal.
Starting point is 01:38:05 If I've got the cup and I'm moving it through my mouth, I get the drink, but maybe not. The tracer studies are important, but- They're indirect. Right, what we really wanna know is, if you do this for a year, are you stronger, are you bigger, are you, you know, et cetera. So I think we don't really know that unequivocally.
Starting point is 01:38:22 You did a great discussion recently of a study in which Luke Van Lee was one of the authors that used up to 100 grams and looked at the other end of the threshold. Is there an asymptote? Is there a level in which you don't get any more benefit? Right. Where conventional wisdom was 40 grams-ish was the ceiling. And I think the study from Luke suggested, man, that might be true for a very rapidly hydrolyzed protein such as whey, but with casein at least his data suggested maybe not. Time release protein might be a value and
Starting point is 01:38:53 meals like a steak might be closer to casein kinetics than whey kinetics. Right. I think we don't know that there's an upper limit. We don't know the full duration. Some people said you only stay on a ball for two hours after eating the protein. I think that Luke study shows more. So I think there are those things. What about the maximum amount of protein? Again, traditional thinking here is three grams per kilogram is the maximum. And if you consume more than three grams per kilogram as a healthy
Starting point is 01:39:24 individual, you risk kidney damage. Right. This is something that's been intriguing to me. When I look at this, and I haven't done a complete thorough check, but I'm sort of in the process of working through it, I hear it's going to reduce bone mass or could reduce bone mass, excessive protein intake that is, could lead to kidney function problems, some other unspecified problems. There's even this old thing called rabbit starvation, which you can find papers going back on this at least a hundred years. And they talk about hunters and survivalists and so
Starting point is 01:39:56 on out in the woods who can shoot a lot of rabbits and eat their fill of rabbits and yet starve to death because they don't have enough fat and carbohydrate to properly digest. But if you say, now let's go back and find the trials that showed this. So you find a paper and it says, here are the limits. Steve Himesfield and Sue Schaps has just had a wonderful nutrition 101 commentary in New England Journal of Medicine. And they talk about some of the upper limits and they cite some papers and they say, some bad things can happen if you eat too much protein. You go back to those papers, they're review papers, they're not trials.
Starting point is 01:40:32 Those papers say the limits are like this and they cite a few things. You keep going back and we're unable to find trials where people do it. All you seem to get to is somebody said, well, but there was this group of hunters in this population who ate this many grams and they were okay. So don't go above that. But nobody said, if you go above that, something bad happens. There's this study and I say loosely of one or two guys who ate nothing but meat for six months to a year and they were fine. Interestingly, there's a corresponding study from 1928, two Polish scientists put two Polish people on a diet for six months of nothing but potatoes, fruit, and a little bit of fat to cook the potatoes in.
Starting point is 01:41:17 And the idea was, can you get enough protein and nitrogen out of the potatoes? And the answer was, yes, at least for six months, they were fine. So people have been fine eating nothing but meat and no plants for six, 12 months. These are semi-anecdotes, they're intervention studies, but they're not big randomized control trials, or nothing but potatoes for six months as a protein source, and they've all been fine. Do you remember in any of those studies how much weight was lost in each group? In the potato study, there was neither weight lost nor gained. Really? Yeah.
Starting point is 01:41:48 How did those people not lose weight? They were probably thin to begin with. If you go back to the classic studies of Ted Van Italy and Sammy Hashim from 60s, I think, where they would bring in, as they described them at the time, lean Columbia University students versus obese adults. They gave them Metrical, which was the boost or insure of 1960, and threw a tube where they could get unlimited supply, but they couldn't really see how much they were eating. What they found is that the obese people generally lost weight.
Starting point is 01:42:24 They didn't fully compensate or the monotony made them reduce intake, whereas the lean students all maintained weight. So, it's probably that the effects of diet on weight change vary a great deal depending on where you're starting. Anyway, so the potato eaters neither gained nor lost weight and they have beautiful nitrogen balance. From what I've understood, I haven't studied those papers as carefully, but the all meat eaters at least for six, 12 months were fine. So I think either way it can be done. What I have not seen is somebody who said, to test this rabbit starvation thing, we brought a bunch of healthy
Starting point is 01:43:02 adults in and we fed them nothing but cooked rabbit for six months and something good or bad didn't happen. I've not seen somebody say, we fed enormous levels of protein to normal adults and we saw leaching of bone mass. And by the way, do you think that study would need to be rabbit because it's so lean or could it be rib eye which is equally void in carbohydrate but at least is high in fat so from a macro perspective, you're dividing things up. I think it depends on what you think the mechanism of action is and it's not crystal clear to me that people have specified a crystal clear mechanism of action. There is some speculation and again there's some nice recent
Starting point is 01:43:45 papers on this that especially for people who want anabolic effects, bodybuilders, weightlifters that when you eat protein, you should have some carbohydrate with it that will enhance the anabolism through the insulin. Exactly. Or you inject insulin. But my understanding and again I'm just really entering in this, but I've read so far, there is no compelling evidence that that is true. That is that you get more anabolic effect if you eat carbohydrate with your protein than if you eat protein alone. So another presumption or myth. So rabbit starvation, bone loss, kidney problems, and you must have carbohydrate with it. All of these things are, I think these things that are presumed known and readily talked about,
Starting point is 01:44:31 but I don't think demonstrated. So I've not seen any trial data yet in normal adolescents or adults that suggests a negative health problem, not conjectured, but observed as a result of too much protein. I'm not saying there isn't such a study, but I have not yet found those studies. If anybody else knows them, please send them to me. Yeah, that would be interesting. Let's just assume that those studies don't exist in fact, or if they do, they're very, very small and therefore probably not worth extrapolating to the ends of the universe on. What is the probability that such basic questions like this will be answered in the coming decade
Starting point is 01:45:12 of nutrition science? What is the appetite, no pun intended, for this type of clinical investigation, especially in light of everything else we've spoken about, which is, hey, the name of the game in nutrition science now is pharmacology. It's not these mundane questions about macronutrients. For obesity treatment and closely related things, diabetes treatment, prevention, I think you're right, the name of the game. For other areas, I think there'll be more interest, so longevity promotion, and you'll get something like the evolution foundation weighing in, which could conceivably do big studies. But even there, nothing's unlimited. They may say, well, we're going
Starting point is 01:45:55 to focus a big trial that'll be definitive and really give the answer, but it's going to give the answer only in this age group or something like that. I think NIH will fund some, I think industry will fund some, but NIH industry are likely to fund in most cases things that are small enough that we're not going to know the answer about every dose at every period of time in every race, age, sex, and health status group. What we'll know is pocket answers. I think what we could get, for example, is a study funded in which we very, very thoroughly looked at ordinary healthy adults over 60 years old who want to increase strength and muscle mass and we'll look at protein intake and we'll look at other upper limits. I think that could conceivably be done and we could probably nail that answer.
Starting point is 01:46:47 But then you might come back and say, well, you've shown it's safe or unsafe for a 60 year old. That doesn't mean it is for a 20 year old. And we'll say, that's true. And you say, well, you showed it with casein whey, but not with pea protein and whatever. That's true too. So how compelling do you find the data that high protein diets reduce longevity? There are many proponents of this view out there, often I suppose within the plant-based community, although again, I don't think those are necessarily an overlap, although that just
Starting point is 01:47:20 seems to be where I notice most of the lower protein is better rhetoric. But how do you assess the strength of that claim? Very low. I think it's going to depend on species and that's important because it leads to the extrapolation issue. If you're a butterfly, I think it probably does reduce lifespan and I think a higher carbohydrate diet may increase lifespan more if you're a butterfly. We do have some butterfly listeners of this podcast, but I'll be honest with you, our efforts to increase butterfly listenership have largely been, I would just say they've been less successful than I would have enjoyed.
Starting point is 01:47:59 We'll keep working on that. But I think in humans, there's no compelling, in my view, no compelling evidence. I would even say there's some evidence and reasons to believe the contrary. Some of that's going to be tied to wealth. Wealthier people eat more protein than less wealthy people, including within our country. So it's hard to tease all this part in epidemiology. But if you look at the association studies, even there, I don't find it compelling. Then you can say, well, do you accept the association studies? Not all that much. If you look at the mouse studies, I'm not sure there you see the full translation, but also I'm not sure they're all that compelling, that low protein.
Starting point is 01:48:37 So I think nothing that I know of would say to me there's very strong reason to believe, even if not definitive RCT, that higher protein will lead to less longevity. And if anything, I think there's more compelling reason. The contrary. Now my friend and IU alumnus, Barry Sears, talks about the zone diet and the idea of the zone is that there's not too much on this end. You shouldn't be too high on this end. Don't be too low over here. There's a zone of things in the body and in diet that are right. You've got to find the right spot. And he very strongly believes that you want to upregulate AMPK to live longer
Starting point is 01:49:15 and you want to not upregulate mTOR too much to live longer. And I know you've talked about rapamycin, which sort of has effects that would go along with what he's saying. He might argue from that point of view that too much protein would reduce longevity. Again, I can't say he's wrong. I think it may also depend on at which period of time. What's good for you to do or eat early in life to prolong life may not be what's good for you later in life.
Starting point is 01:49:47 Sometimes we call that antagonistic pleiotropy. Here we might say, John Holizy found this in rats. I don't know if it holds up as one study, but found that exercise in rats reduced mortality rate in the first half of life, but it increased mortality rate in the second half of life. And so, if we accept that as causal and valid and replicable, then maybe the same things might be true for protein, might be good to eat more when you're younger and less when you're older, or less when you're younger and more when you're older. I don't know. But I think that we also need to think that longevity is only one factor. We talked about this a little bit the other day and there's no right answer to this. But if somebody were to say to me, live this way,
Starting point is 01:50:31 and our best guess is you'll die two years earlier. But until you die, you'll be stronger, you'll feel more energized, you'll look better. I'd say, I'll make that trade. Now someone else might say, I won't make that trade and who's to say who's right. David, one last thing before we go, you serve as sort of the editor of a newsletter that comes out every Friday, Obesity and Energetics. I've been a subscriber for I guess a decade, maybe a bit more, right? When did it start? It started kind of organically all the way back when I was just in grad school basically or getting out and then there was no formal letter at the end of the internet, I would hand my professors
Starting point is 01:51:11 papers I had read and said, hey, maybe we could talk about this or something. Then I moved away, I would mail them and then I mailed to a couple of people and it kept going and soon people started asking to be adding to my list. Then it became electronic. Then it became a formal web thing. So now it goes out to over 100,000 people worldwide. We don't charge anything for it. It's free.
Starting point is 01:51:32 It's called Obesity and Energetics Offerings. We don't accept any commercial support for it. It contains usually about 100 or a few more links to mostly scientific papers, sometimes popular media articles commenting on things in virtually every category related to obesity, energy metabolism, nutrition. Well, I'm a big proponent of it. My team all subscribes to it and it's one of the not too many newsletters that I rely on. Again, people ask me, how do I stay up to date on things? And the truth of it is I have to rely on other people doing a lot of the aggregation. And then I'll kind of go where my curiosity goes and sometimes go a bit deeper.
Starting point is 01:52:13 But anyway, I just wanted to make a plug for people to subscribe to obesity and energetics offerings. It's great. And one of the fun things that I think is great for people learning is one of the sections is always called headline versus study. And I just think that if folks listening to us now are not going to read any of the subheadings there, just read that one. Because it gives you a great sense of how misleading the traditional media can be. Not necessarily because they're nefarious. I don't think that's
Starting point is 01:52:46 the case. I think it's scientific ignorance and a misalignment of incentives. One, they're simply not qualified. They don't have the scientific literacy to understand what a study shows. Secondly, they're really incentivized to get you to read a study and click through something. They have to come up with a headline that makes that appealing. As you point out every week, there's a great example of one where the headline is patently false at worst and at best so misleading as to be useless. What I think is valuable for folks is to get into the habit of checking that once a week and seeing, hey, that was a headline.
Starting point is 01:53:24 I could see that headline. I could see how I'd get fallen. I'd get duped for that, but oh, there's the study. The headline could be something as outrageous as women are so much more likely to outlive their partners if they have sex three times a week. And then you look at the study and it's about this rare species of fruit fly
Starting point is 01:53:43 that sometimes mate with male fruit flies that die a little premature. Exactly. Like, you know what I mean? It can be so ridiculous. Well, I really appreciate your pointing that out. Anybody can subscribe for free. Just type into Google obesity and energetics offerings. You'll find it. If not, email me. I hope Andrew Brown, who's a professor and a former mentor of mine at University of Arkansas now is listening. He took over handling that category a few years ago. He does a beautiful job with it.
Starting point is 01:54:10 And so he sets these things up and often finds these interesting things and even got to the point where others picked up on his use of the phrase in mice as kind of like a standard thing, which is just as often a way of saying, well, we found this in mice, but in mice is often left off in the headlines. How many people are involved in curating that list each week? In any one week, it's about five people, including me. So there's me, there's an editor, then there's Andrew who cleans everything up. And then there's Colby Vorland who cleans everything up and someone else who posts it. That's a lot of work for five people. That's a lot of work. But it rotates. So the editor rotates every week, but the rest of us, including me, are on.
Starting point is 01:54:51 Thank you for never asking me to be an editor on that. I hadn't thought about it until now. Maybe I will. No, no. It is a lot of work, but it's fun. It's a labor of love. Well, David, thank you so much. This was a super fun discussion, and I know that folks are going to get a kick out of it. Thank you, Peter. Great to be here with you. Thank you for listening to this week's episode of The Drive. It's extremely important to
Starting point is 01:55:12 me to provide all of this content without relying on paid ads. To do this, our work is made entirely possible by our members and in return, we offer exclusive member only content and benefits above and beyond what is available for free. So if you want to take your knowledge of this space to the next level, it's our goal to ensure members get back much more than the price of the subscription. Premium membership includes several benefits. First, comprehensive podcast show notes that detail every topic, paper, person and thing that we discuss in each episode. And the word on the street is, nobody's show notes rival ours.
Starting point is 01:55:48 Second, monthly Ask Me Anything or AMA episodes. These episodes are comprised of detailed responses to subscriber questions, typically focused on a single topic, and are designed to offer a great deal of clarity and detail on topics of special interest to our members. You'll also get access to the show notes for these episodes, of course. Third, delivery of our premium newsletter, which is put together by our dedicated team
Starting point is 01:56:13 of research analysts. This newsletter covers a wide range of topics related to longevity and provides much more detail than our free weekly newsletter. Fourth, access to our private podcast feed that provides you with access to every episode, including AMAs, Sans the Spiel you're listening to now, and in your regular podcast feed.
Starting point is 01:56:34 Fifth, The Qualies, an additional member-only podcast we put together that serves as a highlight reel featuring the best excerpts from previous episodes of The Drive. This is a great way best excerpts from previous episodes of The Drive. This is a great way to catch up on previous episodes without having to go back and listen to each one of them. And finally, other benefits that are added along the way. If you want to learn more and access these member-only benefits, you can head over to PeterAteaMD.com forward slash subscribe.
Starting point is 01:57:01 You can also find me on YouTube, Instagram, and Twitter, all with the handle peterateaMD. You can also leave us a review on Apple Podcasts or whatever podcast player you use. This podcast is for general informational purposes only and does not constitute the practice of medicine, nursing, or other professional healthcare services, including the giving of medical advice. No doctor-patient relationship is formed. The use of this information and the materials linked to this podcast is at the user's own risk. The content on this podcast is not intended to be a substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis, or treatment. Users should
Starting point is 01:57:38 not disregard or delay in obtaining medical advice from any medical condition they have, and they should seek the assistance of their healthcare professionals for any such conditions. Finally, I take all conflicts of interest very seriously. For all of my disclosures and the companies I invest in or advise, please visit peteratiamd.com forward slash about where I keep an up-to-date and active list of all disclosures. you

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.